Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-s9k8s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T04:19:26.591Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is core knowledge a natural subdivision of infant cognition?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2024

Caroline M. Kaicher*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ckaicher@andrew.cmu.edu; jjconti@andrew.cmu.edu; adedhe@andrew.cmu.edu; laulet@andrew.cmu.edu; jcantlon@andrew.cmu.edu https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
Julia J. Conti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ckaicher@andrew.cmu.edu; jjconti@andrew.cmu.edu; adedhe@andrew.cmu.edu; laulet@andrew.cmu.edu; jcantlon@andrew.cmu.edu https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
Abhishek M. Dedhe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ckaicher@andrew.cmu.edu; jjconti@andrew.cmu.edu; adedhe@andrew.cmu.edu; laulet@andrew.cmu.edu; jcantlon@andrew.cmu.edu https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
Lauren S. Aulet
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ckaicher@andrew.cmu.edu; jjconti@andrew.cmu.edu; adedhe@andrew.cmu.edu; laulet@andrew.cmu.edu; jcantlon@andrew.cmu.edu https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
Jessica F. Cantlon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ckaicher@andrew.cmu.edu; jjconti@andrew.cmu.edu; adedhe@andrew.cmu.edu; laulet@andrew.cmu.edu; jcantlon@andrew.cmu.edu https://www.cmu.edu/dietrich/psychology/kidneurolab/
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

We examine Spelke's core knowledge taxonomy and test its boundaries. We ask whether Spelke's core knowledge is a distinct type of cognition in the sense that the cognitive processes it includes and excludes are biologically and mechanically coherent.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Spelke's central thesis classifies infant knowledge into the following distinct core knowledge systems: Objects, places, number, forms, agents, and social beings. These systems apply to specific domains of entities in the world and capture specific properties of those entities. They share many features – perhaps the most critical being that they are ancient, automatic, center on abstract concepts, and emerge early in life. Spelke characterizes these systems as innate, invariant over development, impervious to explicit beliefs, and dependent on attention, with a primary function of supporting learning and operating (in part) through mental stimulation. Thus, core knowledge systems are defined not only by their functional role in human cognition but also by characteristics of the mechanisms supporting them. Spelke's core knowledge taxonomy provides a framework for understanding the evolutionary and developmental origins of human knowledge, including the foundations of complex cognition.

However, we question the ability of Spelke's core knowledge taxonomy to “carve nature at its joints.” Testing the boundaries of Spelke's core knowledge framework is important because it helps refine the theory, enabling more precise predictions about the emergence and progression of infant cognition. Here, we ask whether Spelke's collection of core knowledge domains represent a meaningfully distinct type of cognition. Infants have several other cognitive functions that share characteristics with the core domains in that they are ancient, automatic, early-emerging, and abstract cognitive processes that are integral to infants' information processing, and equally essential for explaining what they know. We describe three examples – categorical perception, referential understanding, and algebraic rule learning – to demonstrate this point, raising the question: Is Spelke's “core knowledge” a natural subdivision of infant cognition?

Spelke uses the common characteristics shared by object, place, number, form, agent, and social systems to argue that core knowledge is a distinct cognitive type. Specifically, she claims that all the shared characteristics of the core domains “go together,” and that, “any ancient, abstract conceptual system that has some of these properties is likely to have all of them” (Spelke, Reference Spelke2022, p. 198). Critically, Spelke states that core knowledge systems focus on “the problem of understanding what the sensed world consists of: what entities inhabit it, how those entities behave, and why they do what they do” (Spelke, Reference Spelke2022, p. 36). But, given the criteria, which mechanisms of infant cognition are not core knowledge – and why not?

Here we argue that there is no sharp boundary between Spelke's core knowledge and the rest of infant cognition by showing how three fundamental cognitive processes – categorical perception, referential understanding, and algebraic rule learning – are not only automatic, unconscious, ancient, and abstract, but also support knowledge in infants and are essential to their understanding of the perceptual world.

Categorical perception: Infants display categorical perception, or the propensity to assign discrete boundaries among stimuli varying along a continuum. This process is demonstrated when perceptual discriminations are easier for items belonging to different categories, and more difficult for items belonging to the same category, even when their physical differences are objectively equal (Goldstone & Hendrickson, Reference Goldstone and Hendrickson2010). The most prominent example of this is speech perception, during which we perceive phonemes that are abstracted from the pure acoustic properties of the signal. This process is automatic (Kasai et al., Reference Kasai, Yamada, Kamio, Nakagome, Iwanami, Fukuda and Kato2003), and is demonstrated in infants as young as 1 month old (Eimas, Siqueland, Jusczyk, & Vigorito, Reference Eimas, Siqueland, Jusczyk and Vigorito1971). This ability is not uniquely human – macaques exhibit the same phoneme boundary effect as 1-month-old infants (Kuhl & Padden, Reference Kuhl and Padden1982), European starlings can learn vowel sound categories (Kluender, Lotto, Holt, & Bloedel, Reference Kluender, Lotto, Holt and Bloedel1998), and chinchillas detect changes along phoneme boundaries in particular (Kuhl, Reference Kuhl1981). Additionally, the neural basis of this ability is shared between infants and adults (Dehaene-Lambertz & Gliga, Reference Dehaene-Lambertz and Gliga2004), and also among humans and non-human primates (Ley et al., Reference Ley, Vroomen, Hausfeld, Valente, De Weerd and Formisano2012). Beyond speech, 2-month-old infants categorically perceive some non-speech sounds (Jusczyk, Rosner, Cutting, Foard, & Smith, Reference Jusczyk, Rosner, Cutting, Foard and Smith1977), 7-month-old infants display categorical perception of facial expressions of emotion (Kotsoni, de Haan, & Johnson, Reference Kotsoni, de Haan and Johnson2001), and 4-month-olds categorically perceive color (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Drivonikou, Bevis, Davies, Kay and Regier2008). Categorical perception of color has also been shown in goldfish (Goldman, Lanson, & Brown, Reference Goldman, Lanson and Brown1990) and zebra finches (Zipple et al., Reference Zipple, Caves, Green, Peters, Johnsen and Nowicki2019).

Categorical perception makes items that are meaningfully different more distinct, and makes those that are meaningfully similar more similar. This helps infants eliminate unnecessary information and allows them to more efficiently represent the stimuli around them (Oakes & Madole, Reference Oakes, Madole, Rakison and Oakes2003). This capacity provides the “building blocks” for higher-order categories (Harnad, Reference Harnad and Harnad1987), which is not only critical for learning language (Werker & Lalonde, Reference Werker and Lalonde1988), but it may also be a basis for social categorization in infants (Liberman, Woodward, & Kinzler, Reference Liberman, Woodward and Kinzler2017). Thus, the propensity to create discrete category representations is a core aspect of infant cognition that is abstract, ancient, early-emerging, automatic, and supports learning.

Referential understanding: Referential understanding refers to the ability to understand that communicative signals such as words and pointing are linked to something concrete in the world, and to use such signals to imply intended referents (Wynne & Udell, Reference Wynne and Udell2013). Infants as young as 3 months old demonstrate this by using words to help them categorize objects (Ferry, Hespos, & Waxman, Reference Ferry, Hespos and Waxman2010). At 1 year old, infants understand the referential nature of deictic gestures (Gliga & Csibra, Reference Gliga and Csibra2009) and begin to utilize pointing (Tomasello, Carpenter, & Liszkowski, Reference Tomasello, Carpenter and Liszkowski2007). Referential understanding is also automatic, as demonstrated every time we use language. The ability to match symbols or gestures to referents is also present in dogs (Kaminski, Call, & Fischer, Reference Kaminski, Call and Fischer2004), dolphins (Herman, Richards, & Wolz, Reference Herman, Richards and Wolz1984), and apes (Savage-Rumbaugh, Shanker, & Taylor, Reference Savage-Rumbaugh, Shanker and Taylor1998). Spelke argues that our propensity to use symbols is rooted in human-specific language abilities, but since this capacity is shared between species, it may be more primitive. In fact, linking labels to referents can be considered an associative process during which children use space/object and space/word associations to link words to objects (Samuelson, Smith, Perry, & Spencer, Reference Samuelson, Smith, Perry and Spencer2011). Associative processes such as this are abstract (Delamater, Desouza, Rivkin, & Derman, Reference Delamater, Desouza, Rivkin and Derman2014) and are present in a variety of non-human animals (Rescorla & Holland, Reference Rescorla and Holland1982).

Referential understanding is key for word learning in infants (Gentner & Boroditsky, Reference Gentner and Boroditsky2001), and in their second year they begin to utilize the non-arbitrary referential actions of others (i.e., looking and pointing) to establish arbitrary referential relationships, such as mapping words onto objects (Baldwin, Reference Baldwin1993). Additionally, 12-month-old infants rely on referential cues to connect others' emotional messages with novel objects (Moses, Baldwin, Rosicky, & Tidball, Reference Moses, Baldwin, Rosicky and Tidball2001). Thus, referential understanding is not only abstract, early-emerging, ancient, and automatic, but it also plays an important role in infants' learning about the world around them.

Algebraic rule learning: Algebraic rule learning requires one to detect relations between entities, and is characterized by an ability to generalize patterns to novel items (Dehaene, Meyniel, Wacongne, Wang, & Pallier, Reference Dehaene, Meyniel, Wacongne, Wang and Pallier2015). Infants demonstrate this through their remarkable ability to extract rules from visual and auditory input. For example, 3-month-olds can generalize same/different relations among arrays of toys (Anderson, Chang, Hespos, & Gentner, Reference Anderson, Chang, Hespos and Gentner2018) and 4-month-olds can do so with geometric shapes (Addyman & Mareschal, Reference Addyman and Mareschal2010). Additionally, newborns can discriminate spoken syllable patterns (Gervain, Macagno, Cogoi, Peña, & Mehler, Reference Gervain, Macagno, Cogoi, Peña and Mehler2008). Our detection of algebraic rules is also an automatic and unconscious process (Dehaene et al., Reference Dehaene, Meyniel, Wacongne, Wang and Pallier2015; Miller, Reference Miller1967). Kanzi the chimpanzee demonstrated the ability to understand word order grammatical rules (Schoenemann, Reference Schoenemann2022), dolphins display key elements of syntax (Kako, Reference Kako1999), and crows and monkeys can even generate recursive sequences (Ferrigno, Cheyette, Piantadosi, & Cantlon, Reference Ferrigno, Cheyette, Piantadosi and Cantlon2020; Liao, Brecht, Johnston, & Nieder, Reference Liao, Brecht, Johnston and Nieder2022). In addition, macaques can learn context-free grammars based on embedded spatial sequences (Ferrigno, Reference Ferrigno, Shwartz and Beran2022; Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Long, Cao, Li, Dehaene and Wang2018), demonstrating an evolutionarily conserved propensity for algebraic rule learning.

Infants' rule learning abilities are essential to the development of complex capacities such as language. For instance, 4-month-olds can detect non-adjacent grammatical dependencies in a novel language after only one learning session (Friederici, Mueller, & Oberecker, Reference Friederici, Mueller and Oberecker2011), and 17-month-olds can segment words in fluent speech based on non-adjacent dependencies using statistical learning (Frost et al., Reference Frost, Jessop, Durrant, Peter, Bidgood, Pine and Monaghan2020). Infants' rule-learning abilities also help them learn the “grammar” of music (McMullen & Saffran, Reference McMullen and Saffran2004). Thus, abundant evidence demonstrates that algebraic rule learning is an abstract, early-emerging, automatic, unconscious, and ancient aspect of infant knowledge, supporting learning in multiple domains (Rabagliati, Ferguson, & Lew-Williams, Reference Rabagliati, Ferguson and Lew-Williams2019).

Perhaps what makes the core domains in Spelke's theory distinct is that they “operate on a limited domain of entities” and “capture only a limited subset of properties that our perceptual systems deliver” (Spelke, Reference Spelke2022, p. 190). However, we question whether Spelke's core domains are more selective, rigid, or filtered than other systems. For instance, knowledge of number can be used with any discrete set of things or events, and adapts to new, evolutionarily recent information such as digits and verbal counting. Numerical information automatically interacts with perceptual and semantic information from disparate domains during development (e.g., Gebuis, Cohen Kadosh, De Haan, & Henik, Reference Gebuis, Cohen Kadosh, De Haan and Henik2009). Ferrigno, Jara-Ettinger, Piantadosi, and Cantlon (Reference Ferrigno, Jara-Ettinger, Piantadosi and Cantlon2017) showed that when both numerical and surface area information is available for approximate magnitude discrimination, numerical biases are uniquely enhanced in humans compared to non-human primates. Additionally, they found that within the Tsimane’, a non-industrialized group in Bolivia, adults who have learned to count display a greater number bias than those who have not. Spelke herself even discusses how Mundurucu children and adults who have been exposed to formal education have more precise numerical representations than those who have not (Piazza, Pica, Izard, Spelke, & Dehaene, Reference Piazza, Pica, Izard, Spelke and Dehaene2013). Spelke uses this evidence to show that the core number system supports learning of the symbolic number system, but it also shows that the core number system can be penetrated by novel domains and inputs. Thus, the number system may not be as independent, rigid, or limited as it is made out to be. Similarly, the limitations of the “core” systems, such as the numerical system, are not greater than the biases and constraints on other informational systems such as categorical perception, referential understanding, and rule learning. All mechanisms have their own unique cognitive signatures and constraints for abstracting information across diverse entities while adapting to novel inputs and problems.

Mechanisms that are (perhaps erroneously) considered more “general purpose” than the core domains also exhibit biases and constraints on processing. This is even the case for reinforcement learning, in which avoidance responses to different reinforcers (induced nausea or shock) are more readily associated with certain cues (gustatory and audiovisual, respectively) than others in rats (Garcia & Koelling, Reference Garcia and Koelling1966). This bias is present in humans, as shown through the privileged role of nausea in the acquisition of food dislikes (Pelchat & Rozin, Reference Pelchat and Rozin1982). Thus, deep information processing biases are present in this “general” mechanism and influence learning in humans. Our three purportedly general-purpose mechanisms also display innate biases and are subject to information constraints and filters. For instance, rule learning, like the number system, has capacity limits – just as larger numerical differences are easier to discriminate than smaller ones, shorter range dependencies are easier to learn than longer ones (Futrell, Mahowald, & Gibson, Reference Futrell, Mahowald and Gibson2015). For referential understanding, children display specific biases, such as the whole-object, taxonomic, and mutual exclusivity assumptions, that constrain how they map words onto referents (Markman, Reference Markman, Gelman and Byrnes1991). Additionally, information processing through categorical perception is constrained so that objective similarities between stimuli are filtered based on useful category boundaries (Goldstone & Hendrickson, Reference Goldstone and Hendrickson2010). Category formation can also be constrained by the number of exemplars, their variability, and their similarity (Needham, Dueker, & Lockhead, Reference Needham, Dueker and Lockhead2005).

Thus, categorical perception, referential understanding, and algebraic rule learning are three examples of key components of infant cognition – things that babies “know” and that are integral to their understanding of the world. These processes exhibit innate biases and are subject to information constraints, abstraction, and filters similar to Spelke's core knowledge domains. The range of infant abilities that are early-emerging, abstract, automatic, ancient, and not considered core knowledge indicates that infant knowledge emerges independently of the purported specificity of its domain. In this sense, the boundaries of core knowledge set by Spelke are not biologically and mechanically coherent, and are displaced from the evolutionary and developmental origins of infant cognition and the knowledge it generates. The disconnection between well-known evolved cognitive functions and Spelke's lens limits the explanatory and predictive power of “core knowledge” as a taxonomy – if the boundaries of core knowledge arbitrarily exclude key forms of infant cognition, then the framework cannot anticipate what babies naturally know.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (JFC, grant number R01HD107715); and National Science Foundation (JFC, grant number 2148343).

Competing interest

None.

References

Addyman, C., & Mareschal, D. (2010). The perceptual origins of the abstract same/different concept in human infants. Animal Cognition, 13, 817833. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0330-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, E. M., Chang, Y., Hespos, S., & Gentner, D. (2018). Comparison within pairs promotes analogical abstraction in three-month-olds. Cognition, 176, 7486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29(5), 832. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.5.832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Meyniel, F., Wacongne, C., Wang, L., & Pallier, C. (2015). The neural representation of sequences: From transition probabilities to algebraic patterns and linguistic trees. Neuron, 88(1), 219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Gliga, T. (2004). Common neural basis for phoneme processing in infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 13751387. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042304714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delamater, A. R., Desouza, A., Rivkin, Y., & Derman, R. (2014). Associative and temporal processes: A dual process approach. Behavioural Processes, 101, 3848. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.09.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171(3968), 303306. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3968.303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrigno, S. (2022). Sequencing, artificial grammar, and recursion in primates. In Shwartz, B. L., & Beran, M. J. (Eds.), Primate cognitive studies (pp. 260290). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrigno, S., Cheyette, S. J., Piantadosi, S. T., & Cantlon, J. F. (2020). Recursive sequence generation in monkeys, children, US adults, and native Amazonians. Science Advances, 6(26), eaaz1002. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aaz1002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrigno, S., Jara-Ettinger, J., Piantadosi, S. T., & Cantlon, J. F. (2017). Universal and uniquely human factors in spontaneous number perception. Nature Communications, 8(1), 13968. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13968CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: An advantage of words over tones. Child Development, 81(2), 472479. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01408.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Bevis, L., Davies, I. R., Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2008). Categorical perception of color is lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(9), 32213225. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0712286105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Mueller, J. L., & Oberecker, R. (2011). Precursors to natural grammar learning: Preliminary evidence from 4-month-old infants. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e17920. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frost, R. L., Jessop, A., Durrant, S., Peter, M. S., Bidgood, A., Pine, J. M., … Monaghan, P. (2020). Non-adjacent dependency learning in infancy, and its link to language development. Cognitive Psychology, 120, 101291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2020.101291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Futrell, R., Mahowald, K., & Gibson, E. (2015). Large-scale evidence of dependency length minimization in 37 languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(33), 1033610341. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1502134112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123124. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03342209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gebuis, T., Cohen Kadosh, R., De Haan, E., & Henik, A. (2009). Automatic quantity processing in 5-year olds and adults. Cognitive Processing, 10, 133142. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0219-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, 3, 215256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(37), 1422214227. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0806530105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gliga, T., & Csibra, G. (2009). One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychological Science, 20(3), 347353. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02295.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman, M., Lanson, R., & Brown, G. G. (1990). Wavelength categorization by goldfish (Carassius auratus). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 4(3). https://doi.org/10.46867/C4JS4XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., & Hendrickson, A. T. (2010). Categorical perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 6978. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.26Google ScholarPubMed
Harnad, S. (1987). Psychophysical and cognitive aspects of categorical perception: A critical overview. In Harnad, S. (Ed.), Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition (pp. 125). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Herman, L. M., Richards, D. G., & Wolz, J. P. (1984). Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition, 16(2), 129219. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(84)90003-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, X., Long, T., Cao, W., Li, J., Dehaene, S., & Wang, L. (2018). Production of supra-regular spatial sequences by macaque monkeys. Current Biology, 28(12), 18511859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., Rosner, B. S., Cutting, J. E., Foard, C. F., & Smith, L. B. (1977). Categorical perception of nonspeech sounds by 2-month-old infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 5054. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199467CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kako, E. (1999). Elements of syntax in the systems of three language-trained animals. Animal Learning & Behavior, 27, 114. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for “fast mapping”. Science, 304(5677), 16821683. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1097859CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasai, K., Yamada, H., Kamio, S., Nakagome, K., Iwanami, A., Fukuda, M., … Kato, N. (2003). Neuromagnetic correlates of impaired automatic categorical perception of speech sounds in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 59(2–3), 159172. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00382-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kluender, K. R., Lotto, A. J., Holt, L. L., & Bloedel, S. L. (1998). Role of experience for language-specific functional mappings of vowel sounds. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 104(6), 35683582. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.423939CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotsoni, E., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2001). Categorical perception of facial expressions by 7-month-old infants. Perception, 30(9), 11151125. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K. (1981). Discrimination of speech by nonhuman animals: Basic auditory sensitivities conducive to the perception of speech-sound categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 70(2), 340349. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.386782CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., & Padden, D. M. (1982). Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the voicing feature in macaques. Perception & Psychophysics, 32(6), 542550. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ley, A., Vroomen, J., Hausfeld, L., Valente, G., De Weerd, P., & Formisano, E. (2012). Learning of new sound categories shapes neural response patterns in human auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(38), 1327313280. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0584-12.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, D. A., Brecht, K. F., Johnston, M., & Nieder, A. (2022). Recursive sequence generation in crows. Science Advances, 8(44), eabq3356. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abq3356CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). The origins of social categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(7), 556568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, E. M. (1991). The whole-object, taxonomic, and mutual exclusivity assumptions as initial constraints on word meanings. In Gelman, S. A., & Byrnes, J. P. (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 72106). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511983689.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMullen, E., & Saffran, J. R. (2004). Music and language: A developmental comparison. Music Perception, 21(3), 289311. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1967). The psychology of communication. Human Resource Management, 6(3), 43.Google Scholar
Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72(3), 718735. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Needham, A., Dueker, G., & Lockhead, G. (2005). Infants’ formation and use of categories to segregate objects. Cognition, 94(3), 215240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakes, L. M., & Madole, K. L. (2003). Principles of developmental change in infants’ category formation. In Rakison, D. H., & Oakes, L. M. (Eds.), Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion (pp. 132158). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelchat, M. L., & Rozin, P. (1982). The special role of nausea in the acquisition of food dislikes by humans. Appetite, 3(4), 341351. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80052-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piazza, M., Pica, P., Izard, V., Spelke, E. S., & Dehaene, S. (2013). Education enhances the acuity of the nonverbal approximate number system. Psychological Science, 24(6), 10371043. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612464057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabagliati, H., Ferguson, B., & Lew-Williams, C. (2019). The profile of abstract rule learning in infancy: Meta-analytic and experimental evidence. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12704. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12704CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A., & Holland, P. C. (1982). Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology, 33(1), 265308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samuelson, L. K., Smith, L. B., Perry, L. K., & Spencer, J. P. (2011). Grounding word learning in space. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e28095. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028095CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Shanker, S., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoenemann, P. T. (2022). Evidence of grammatical knowledge in apes: An analysis of Kanzi's performance on reversible sentences. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 885605. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.885605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S. (2022). What babies know: Core knowledge and composition volume 1 (Vol. 1). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Lalonde, C. E. (1988). Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology, 24(5), 672. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.5.672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynne, C. D., & Udell, M. A. (2013). Animal cognition: Evolution, behavior and cognition. Bloomsbury Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zipple, M. N., Caves, E. M., Green, P. A., Peters, S., Johnsen, S., & Nowicki, S. (2019). Categorical colour perception occurs in both signalling and non-signalling colour ranges in a songbird. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1903), 20190524. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0524Google Scholar