Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-s9k8s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T02:15:57.594Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural habituation during acute stress signals a blunted endocrine response and poor resilience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 June 2023

Yadong Liu
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Xiaolin Zhao
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Weiyu Hu
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Yipeng Ren
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Zhenni Wei
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Xi Ren
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Zihan Tang
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Nan Wang
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Haopeng Chen
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Yizhuo Li
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
Zhenhao Shi
Affiliation:
Center for Studies of Addiction, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
Shaozheng Qin
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
Juan Yang*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
*
Corresponding author: Juan Yang; Email: valleyqq@swu.edu.cn
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

A blunted hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis response to acute stress is associated with psychiatric symptoms. Although the prefrontal cortex and limbic areas are important regulators of the HPA axis, whether the neural habituation of these regions during stress signals both blunted HPA axis responses and psychiatric symptoms remains unclear. In this study, neural habituation during acute stress and its associations with the stress cortisol response, resilience, and depression were evaluated.

Methods

Seventy-seven participants (17–22 years old, 37 women) were recruited for a ScanSTRESS brain imaging study, and the activation changes between the first and last stress blocks were used as the neural habituation index. Meanwhile, participants' salivary cortisol during test was collected. Individual-level resilience and depression were measured using questionnaires. Correlation and moderation analyses were conducted to investigate the association between neural habituation and endocrine data and mental symptoms. Validated analyses were conducted using a Montreal Image Stress Test dataset in another independent sample (48 participants; 17–22 years old, 24 women).

Results

Neural habituation of the prefrontal cortex and limbic area was negatively correlated with cortisol responses in both datasets. In the ScanSTRESS paradigm, neural habituation was both positively correlated with depression and negatively correlated with resilience. Moreover, resilience moderated the relationship between neural habituation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and cortisol response.

Conclusions

This study suggested that neural habituation of the prefrontal cortex and limbic area could reflect motivation dysregulation during repeated failures and negative feedback, which might further lead to maladaptive mental states.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

A blunted stress response is a lower level of cardiovascular or endocrine response during acute stress (Howden et al., Reference Howden, Perhonen, Peshock, Zhang, Arbab-Zadeh, Adams-Huet and Levine2015; Jansen et al., Reference Jansen, Gispen-de Wied, Gademan, De Jonge, Van Der Linden and Kahn1998). Researchers found that this blunted response, especially to psychological stressors, is associated with various mental and behavioral disorders, such as a lower level of resilience and a higher level of depression (Adinoff, Junghanns, Kiefer, & Krishnan-Sarin, 2005; Carroll, Phillips, Hunt, & Der, Reference Carroll, Phillips, Hunt and Der2007). Since the brain is the core regulator of the stress response (Dedovic, Duchesne, Andrews, Engert, & Pruessner, Reference Dedovic, Duchesne, Andrews, Engert and Pruessner2009; Kogler et al., Reference Kogler, Müller, Chang, Eickhoff, Fox, Gur and Derntl2015), understanding the blunted hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity at the neural level could not only provide insight into the mechanisms of the maladaptive stress response and its relationship with mental health but also offer potential treatment options (McCarty, Reference McCarty2016).

The HPA axis activation begins with the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus releasing the corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) and ends with the adrenal cortex producing glucocorticoid hormones (mainly cortisol in humans) into blood (Koning, Buurstede, Van Weert, & Meijer, Reference Koning, Buurstede, Van Weert and Meijer2019; López, Akil, & Watson, Reference López, Akil and Watson1999). Glucocorticoid hormones act on the body via mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) and glucocorticoid receptors (GRs) (Kim, Han, & Iremonger, Reference Kim, Han and Iremonger2019), which regulate various stress processes, such as threat detection, emotion regulation, and cognitive control (Carroll, Ginty, Whittaker, Lovallo, & de Rooij, Reference Carroll, Ginty, Whittaker, Lovallo and de Rooij2017; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Dedovic, Khalili-Mahani, Engert, Pruessner, Buss and Lupien2008). It has been found that a repeated psychological stressor would lead to a decreased cortisol response (Cyr & Romero, Reference Cyr and Romero2009), and further influence the risk of various mental and behavioral disorders (Peters & McEwen, Reference Peters and McEwen2015).

Both MRs and GRs are widely distributed in the brain, particularly in the prefrontal cortex and limbic area (e.g. hippocampus, amygdala) (Kim, Pellman, & Kim, Reference Kim, Pellman and Kim2015). Animal studies have demonstrated that cell loss in the hippocampus can affect HPA axis function (Bratt et al., Reference Bratt, Kelley, Knowles, Barrett, Davis, Davis and Mittleman2001), while human studies have found that higher hippocampus and amygdala volume levels can predict a more robust HPA axis activity (Pagliaccio et al., Reference Pagliaccio, Luby, Bogdan, Agrawal, Gaffrey, Belden and Barch2014; Pruessner, Pruessner, Hellhammer, Bruce Pike, & Lupien, Reference Pruessner, Pruessner, Hellhammer, Bruce Pike and Lupien2007). Functional neuroimaging studies have also shown that insufficient activity in the hippocampus, amygdala, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is negatively correlated with the endocrine stress response ( Ginty, Gianaros, Derbyshire, Phillips, & Carroll, Reference Ginty, Gianaros, Derbyshire, Phillips and Carroll2013; Lederbogen et al., Reference Lederbogen, Kirsch, Haddad, Streit, Tost, Schuch and Meyer-Lindenberg2011; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Dedovic, Khalili-Mahani, Engert, Pruessner, Buss and Lupien2008). Importantly, researchers found that the neural response to repeated stimuli will exhibit habituation patterns too. This neural habituation phenomenon was first discovered with sensory stimuli (Wilson, Babb, Halgren, Wang, & Crandall, Reference Wilson, Babb, Halgren, Wang and Crandall1984), then with emotional stimuli (Stevens et al., Reference Stevens, Kim, Galatzer-Levy, Reddy, Ely, Nemeroff and Ressler2017). Recent evidence further documented neural habituation during acute stress. First, CRH neuronal activity robustly habituates to repeated presentations of the same stressors (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Han and Iremonger2019). Furthermore, a number of neuroimaging studies found that habituation in the prefrontal cortex and limbic system was more pronounced than that in other areas (Plichta et al., Reference Plichta, Grimm, Morgen, Mier, Sauer, Haddad and Meyer-Lindenberg2014; Sladky et al., Reference Sladky, Höflich, Atanelov, Kraus, Baldinger, Moser and Windischberger2012). Lastly, a study that used aversive images to induce stress found neural habituation in the hippocampus and insula (Sinha, Lacadie, Todd Constable, & Seo, Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Todd Constable and Seo2016).

Notably, although previous studies have documented the critical role of the prefrontal cortex and limbic activity in ensuring a sufficient endocrine response, whether neural habituation during the task could affect the HPA axis response is still unclear. Given that habituation represents a reduced response to certain stimuli (Rankin et al., Reference Rankin, Abrams, Barry, Bhatnagar, Clayton, Colombo and Thompson2009), we hypothesized that neural habituation in these areas might interfere with the endocrine system and result in a blunted endocrine response. Further, depression and chronic stress are common mental disorders associated with a blunted endocrine response (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Phillips, Hunt and Der2007; Ren et al., Reference Ren, Zhao, Li, Liu, Ren, Pruessner and Yang2022). Therefore, we also hypothesized that neural habituation would positively correlate with depression and chronic stress.

Conversely, resilience refers to the ability to overcome adversity and stress to promote adaptive behaviors and positive health outcomes (Holz, Tost, & Meyer-Lindenberg, Reference Holz, Tost and Meyer-Lindenberg2020; Rutter, Reference Rutter2006). The prefrontal cortex is the critical component in generating resilience coping (Maier & Watkins, Reference Maier and Watkins2010). Previous studies have documented that increased vmPFC activity during stress positively correlates with resilience (Sinha et al., Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Todd Constable and Seo2016). Therefore, we hypothesized that neural decline in the prefrontal cortex caused by habituation would be negatively associated with resilience. Moreover, evidence also showed that the low emotional resilience group would exhibit a blunted endocrine stress response. In contrast, the high resilience group will not be related to the endocrine response (Krkovic, Clamor, & Lincoln, Reference Krkovic, Clamor and Lincoln2018), indicating that resilience may moderate the relationship between neural activity and endocrine stress response. This hypothesis would be also tested in this study.

An adapted version of the ScanSTRESS paradigm (Lederbogen et al., Reference Lederbogen, Kirsch, Haddad, Streit, Tost, Schuch and Meyer-Lindenberg2011), which adopts uncontrollability and social evaluative threat to induce acute stress (Lederbogen et al., Reference Lederbogen, Kirsch, Haddad, Streit, Tost, Schuch and Meyer-Lindenberg2011), was used to induce stress. During this paradigm, stress contexts were induced four times, and both whole-brain-wise and region-of-interest (ROI)-wise analyses were conducted to explore neural habituation during stress induction. Furthermore, we used the amplitude difference between the first and last stimulation blocks to estimate the individual level of neural habituation (Plichta et al., Reference Plichta, Grimm, Morgen, Mier, Sauer, Haddad and Meyer-Lindenberg2014). Subjective and endocrine stress reactivities were also measured repeatedly throughout the experiment, and the levels of depression and resilience were also collected. The relationships between neural habituation, cortisol response, depression, and resilience were also estimated. Finally, we used the Montreal Image Stress Test (MIST) to validate the neural habituation hypothesis (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner2005) and its relationship with a blunted endocrine response. In this dataset, chronic stress was used to validate the relationship between neural habituation and mental health.

Materials and methods

Participants

Seventy-seven participants were recruited online for the ScanSTRESS study (mean age, 19.10 years; range 17–22; 37 women). All participants were healthy college students. Exclusion criteria were head injury, a history of alcoholism, or drug abuse. Female participants were tested during their luteal phase (around 10 days before menstruation) and did not use oral contraceptives leading up to the experiment (Roche, King, Cohoon, & Lovallo, Reference Roche, King, Cohoon and Lovallo2013; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Smith, Boukina, Dordari, Mistry, Taylor and Ismail2020).

ScanSTRESS paradigm and experimental procedure

There were two conditions in this paradigm: a stressful ‘performance phase’ and a control ‘relaxation phase’ (Nowak et al., Reference Nowak, Dimitrov, Oei, Walter, Adli and Veer2020). Under stressful conditions, social evaluative threat and uncontrollability were used to induce acute stress (Fig. 1a). These two components were removed in the control condition (Fig. 1b). There were two runs in this paradigm; each run contained four blocks, two of which were stressful (each block contained a serial subtraction task and a mental rotation task) and the other was relaxed (each block contained a figure matching and a number matching task, Fig. 1c). To mediate the effect of the cortisol rhythm on the experimental results, participants were required to arrive at the laboratory in the mid-afternoon, between 12:00 and 15:00 hours. Subjective stress reports and salivary cortisol data were collected repeatedly from the scanner five times throughout the experiment (Fig. 1d).

Figure. 1. ScanSTRESS paradigm and an overview of the experimental procedure. (a) In the stressful ‘performance phase’, participants were asked to solve challenging cognitive tasks (serial subtraction and mental rotation) under time pressure; in addition, two juries (a man and a woman) is presented on the screen to increase the social evaluative threat. Time limits were adapted to the individual's performance resulting in frequent failure; if the participants could not answer the question in time or correctly, there would be negative feedback shown on the screen as ‘work faster’ or ‘error’ insistently. In the meantime, one of the juries would press a red button in his/her hand and another jury would record the participant's failure; this could also increase participants' feelings of social threat. (b) In the control ‘relaxation phase’, participants went through similar although much easier tasks (find the matched figure or number) with abundant time, and the negative feedback from the screen and the juries were removed. Furthermore, the juries were instructed to look away to remove the social evaluative threat. (c) The order for conditions is counter-balanced, in the first run, the stimuli were given in the ‘control–stressful–control–stressful’ order; in the second run, the stimuli were given in the ‘stressful–control–stressful–control’ order. (d) After arriving at the laboratory, the participants were asked to rest for 30 min before entering the MRI scanner. During the scanning, a T1 image was acquired first, followed by a resting-state image. Immediately, the ScanSTRESS paradigm was used to induce a stress response for 22 min. Then, there was another resting-state scan and structural scan; lastly, participants were debriefed for 10 min before they left the laboratory. During this period, participants provided five saliva samples. The experimenter paused the scan to collect the second, third, and fourth samples. Participants were temporarily removed from the scanner for saliva sampling, after which they returned and completed head localization again. The first and fifth samples were collected outside the MRI scanner.

Data acquisition

Salivary cortisol data acquisition and analyses

Saliva samples were collected using a sampling device (Salivette, Sarstedt, Germany), and all saliva samples were stored at −20 °C until analysis. Cortisol concentrations were analyzed using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay kit (IBL-Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany). The sensitivity of the cortisol assay was 0.005 μg/dL. Following the previously reported methods of calculation (Calvi et al., Reference Calvi, Chen, Benson, Brindle, Bristow, De and Granger2017), the interassay coefficient of variation for the cortisol assay was 11.92%, which is acceptable (below 15%).

Subjective reports

Participants self-reported their feelings of stress on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (total). Self-reported measurements were collected five times along with salivary cortisol collection.

Connor–Davidson Resilience scale

The Connor–Davidson Resilience scale (CD-RISC) was developed as a measure of ‘bounce-back’ and adaptability by the original authors (Vaishnavi, Connor, & Davidson, Reference Vaishnavi, Connor and Davidson2007), with a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (almost always).

Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale

The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) was designed to measure depression mood in general populations (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). There are 20 items on the CES-D, including 4 negative and 16 positive items. Participants were required to estimate the frequency of symptoms in the last week on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most or all the time).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data acquisition

Functional and anatomical whole-brain images were acquired using a 3T Siemens Trio MRI scanner (Munich, Germany). A total of 331 volume functional images were acquired from each participant using a T2-weighted gradient echo-planar imaging sequence during the task [repetition time (TR), 2000 ms; echo time (TE), 30 ms; matrix, 64 × 64; spatial resolution, 3 × 3 × 3 mm3; field of view (FOV), 192 × 192 mm2]. High-resolution T1-weighted three-dimensional (3D) fast-field echo sequences were obtained for anatomical reference (slices, 176; TR, 1900 ms; TE, 2.52 ms; slice thickness, 1 mm; FOV, 256 mm × 256 mm; voxel size, 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm).

Functional MRI data analysis

Preprocessing

Functional MRI data were processed with MATLAB (Natick, MA) using the DPABI toolbox (Yan, Wang, Zuo, & Zang, Reference Yan, Wang, Zuo and Zang2016). First, 3D images were transformed into four-dimensional images, which were then sliced time-corrected in milliseconds for each slice individually. Subsequently, all images were realigned to correct for the head motion for acquisition, co-registered with individual participants' T1-weighted images, spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template using the Dartel segments, and smoothed using a 4-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel.

Neural habituation analysis

Each run included two regressors (stressful and control conditions), and six movement parameters were included as additional covariates. To estimate the general effects of stress induction, contrast images of stress v. control over two sessions were analyzed using a one-sample t test (Henze et al., Reference Henze, Konzok, Kreuzpointner, Bärtl, Peter, Giglberger and Wüst2020). In addition, to test the neural habituation between blocks under different conditions, the interaction between stress and block was calculated using a 2 × 4 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with stress and block as within-subject variables.

To further calculate the level of brain changes over the entire stress induction period, we modeled each stress block separately and extracted a total of four brain response estimates per participant. Based on the whole-brain-wise ANOVA, ROIs were identified using masks from the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) template, and the selected ROIs were primarily located in the medial prefrontal cortex–limbic areas. Mean amplitude levels within each block were extracted using the selected ROI template as a mask, and the amplitude difference between the first and last stimulation blocks was measured to estimate neural habituation during stress induction (Blackford, Allen, Cowan, & Avery, Reference Blackford, Allen, Cowan and Avery2013; Plichta et al., Reference Plichta, Grimm, Morgen, Mier, Sauer, Haddad and Meyer-Lindenberg2014).

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (version 22). As subjective and endocrine stress reactivities were measured repeatedly throughout the experiment, we calculated the areas under the curve with respect to ground (AUCg) and areas under the curve with respect to increases (AUCi) to reflect the level of the stress response, as recommended by previous researchers (Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid, & Hellhammer, Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer2003). Specifically, AUCg is an index of total subjective or cortisol response during stress, while AUCi is an index of subjective or cortisol response with respect to increase (online Supplementary Fig. S1). Participants were categorized as responders if an increase in the cortisol level was detected (⩾1 nmol/L) either immediately or 20 min after stress relative to baseline (Hamer, O'Donnell, Lahiri, & Steptoe, Reference Hamer, O'Donnell, Lahiri and Steptoe2010), afterward, the neural habituation pattern differences between cortisol responders and non-responders were explored. A correlation analysis between neural habituation and cortisol response was tested to analyze the relationship between neural habituation and cortisol response. Furthermore, a correlation analysis between neural habituation, depression, and resilience was conducted. We also defined moderate models with neural habituation as the independent variable, stress reactivity as the dependent variable, and resilience as the moderating variable. All p values in statistical analysis were corrected using the false-discovery rate (FDR) approach.

Validation analysis

The low stability between different stress paradigms has long been criticized (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Chen, McEwen, Forsyth, Gee, Bearden and Mirzakhanian2021). Therefore, we used an independent sample under the MIST paradigm to validate the results (48 participants; 17–22 years old, 24 women). Information about the MIST paradigm has been previously reported in another article (Ren et al., Reference Ren, Zhao, Li, Liu, Ren, Pruessner and Yang2022). Similar to the ScanSTRESS paradigm, the MIST paradigm also uses social evaluative threat and uncontrollability to induce acute stress. During stress induction, salivary cortisol was collected seven times, and the daily stress inventory was used as an index of chronic stress to reflect mental health levels (online Supplementary I and Fig. S2). The analysis procedures were the same as those in ScanSTRESS.

Results

Study population

Three participants were excluded because of head motion execution (Zuo et al., Reference Zuo, Anderson, Bellec, Birn, Biswal, Blautzik and Milham2014), and another two participants were excluded because of outliers (more than 3 standard deviations outside the mean) in the cortisol data (Gump et al., Reference Gump, Stewart, Reihman, Lonky, Darvill, Parsons and Granger2008). A total of 72 participants (32 women) were included in the final analysis.

Subjective, endocrine, and neural responses to stress

Regarding the behavioral and endocrine levels, time was determined to be a significant intra-subject variable in subjective stress self-reports (F (4, 68) = 42.33, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.713). The post-hoc analysis revealed that participants' subjective stress levels increased significantly after stress induction [p time2−time1 < 0.001, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.955–1.539], attained the highest levels of subjective stress after stress induction (p time3−time2 < 0.001, 95% CI 0.275–0.780), and then decreased significantly after stress induction (p time3−time4 < 0.001, 95% CI 1.370–2.019). Time was also found to be a significant variable for salivary cortisol levels (F (4, 68) = 3.30, p =0.016, η p2 = 0.162). Post-hoc analysis revealed that participants' salivary cortisol levels increased significantly after stress induction (p time2−time1 < 0.05, 95% CI 0.007–0.066, p time3−time1 < 0.05, 95% CI 0.006–0.076), remained a high level after stress induction, and then decreased significantly (p time4−time5 < 0.05, 95% CI 0.015–0.056). The overall change in stress reactivity is shown in Fig. 2a.

Figure. 2. Validation of stress induction in the ScanSTRESS paradigm. (a) Subjective stress report and salivary cortisol secretion during ScanSTRESS. (b) Neural response to stress induction in the ScanSTRESS paradigm (stress > control, FDR-corrected p < 0.05).

The whole-brain analysis (two-tailed combined FDR-corrected q < 0.05; Fig. 2b) revealed similar activation and deactivation patterns seen in previous studies (Akdeniz et al., Reference Akdeniz, Tost, Streit, Haddad, Wüst, Schäfer and Meyer-Lindenberg2014; Sandner et al., Reference Sandner, Lois, Streit, Zeier, Kirsch, Wüst and Wessa2020), indicating that acute stress-induced activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and temporal areas and deactivation in the vmPFC, posterior cingulate cortex, and insula. Detailed information is provided in online Supplementary Table S1.

Behavioral correlations

Correlation analysis showed that depression was positively correlated with the total subjective stress feelings during stress (SSAUCg, r = 0.27, p = 0.021, 95% CI 0.043–0.492) and negatively correlated with cortisol secretion with respect to increase (CortiAUCi, r = −0.23, p = 0.051, 95% CI −0.467 to 0.044). There was no correlation between stress response and age and any of the other variables (online Supplementary Table S2).

Neural habituation during repeated stress induction

ANOVA at the whole-brain level (two-tailed combined FDR-corrected q < 0.05) revealed a significant interaction between stress and block within the prefrontal and limbic areas, including the dlPFC, vmPFC, anterior cingulate cortex, insula, hippocampus, and amygdala (Fig. 3a). The detailed information is presented in online Supplementary Table S3. In addition, the activation value in each block was extracted using the F test map as a mask, and the neural activity declined significantly during the four stress blocks while remaining stable during the four control blocks (Fig. 3b). Group-level analysis revealed that compared to responders, non-responders showed a greater neural decline during stress induction (online Supplementary Fig. S3). Finally, activation changes between the first and last stress blocks were used as the neural habituation index.

Figure. 3. Neural habituation between stressful conditions in the ScanSTRESS paradigm (FDR-corrected p < 0.05). (a) The interaction between block and stress. (b) Neural habituation during four blocks in the areas investigated.

Correlation between neural habituation, endocrine stress response, and mental health

Twenty-two ROIs located in the prefrontal cortex and limbic areas were selected from the AAL template, and the F map derived from the ANOVA was also defined as an ROI. Correlation results showed that neural habituation at the whole-brain level as well as the ROI level was accompanied by a blunted total cortisol secretion during stress (CortiAUCg). Besides, CortiAUCi is negatively correlated with ROI which is located in the dlPFC and vmPFC. Lastly, neural habituation of some ROIs located in the prefrontal cortex was negatively correlated with resilience, and neural habituation of ROIs located in the limbic area was positively correlated with depression (Fig. 4).

Figure. 4. Correlation between neural habituation (first block–last block) and stress reactivity as well as behavioral data in the ScanSTRESS paradigm. Neural habituation on the whole-brain level as well as regions of interest located in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) [including superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG)], ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [including the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), SFG, orbital part (orb), SFGmorb, SFG medial part (med), IFGorb, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)], and limbic area (insula) are accompanied by a blunted CortiAUCg, and neural habituation in the left dlPFC (SFG) and left vmPFC (IFGorb) are negatively correlated with CortiAUCi. Besides, neural habituation in the right amygdala and right hippocampus is positively correlated with the level of depression level. Lastly, the neural habituation in left dlPFC (mainly the SFG) and vmPFC (including the left SFGmed, left MFGorb, and both IFGorbs) is negatively correlated with the level of resilience. CortiAUCg and CortiAUCi indicate the areas under the curve with respect to ground and areas under the curve with respect to increases for salivary cortisol, respectively. * indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.05.

Moderation analysis

With neural habituation (whole-brain level and 22 ROIs) as the independent variable, cortisol response (CortiAUCg and CortiAUCi) as the dependent variable, and resilience as the moderate variable, results showed that the level of resilience tend to moderate the relationship between the dlPFC and vmPFC neural habituation and blunted stress response (online Supplementary Table S4). Only one model with vmPFC [right middle frontal gyrus (MFG), orbital part (orb)] habituation as the independent variable could survive the FDR correction, indicating that CortiAUCi was significantly lower in the high vmPFC habituation group among participants with low resilience (B = −3. 458, p < 0.001), but not among those with high resilience (B = 1.960, p = 0.054) (FDR-corrected q < 0.05; online Supplementary Fig. S4).

Validation analysis

Stress induction in MIST paradigm resulted in similar subjective stress feeling and endocrine increases (online Supplementary Fig. S5) as well as neural activation (online Supplementary Fig. S6). Participants' subjective chronic stress levels were negatively correlated with acute endocrine stress responses. Compared to cortisol responders, non-responders showed more profound neural habituation (online Supplementary Fig. S7). Furthermore, when the same ROI was selected, neural habituation at the whole-brain level and ROI level could signal blunted cortisol response (FDR-corrected q < 0.05; Fig. 5). Neural habituation in the right insula was positively correlated with subjective chronic stress level (p = 0.039, uncorrected). Notably, this result did not survive FDR correction, and it was reported solely for the purpose of completeness, and should be interpreted with caution. Neither depression nor resilience was measured in the MIST paradigm.

Figure. 5. Correlation analysis between neural habituation and stress reactivity in the MIST. ROIs were selected based on the ScanSTRESS results. Neural habituation on the whole-brain level, as well as regions of interest located in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) [including superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG)], ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [including the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) medial part (med), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) triangular part (tri), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)], and limbic area (Insula), are accompanied by blunted CortiAUCi. Besides, neural habituation in the right insula is positively correlated with the level of chronic stress (p = 0.039, uncorrected). CortiAUCg and CortiAUCi indicate the areas under the curve with respect to ground and areas under the curve with respect to increases for salivary cortisol, respectively. * indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.05, ** indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.01, *** indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.001.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the neural mechanism of the acute HPA axis response from a neural habituation perspective. Our results showed that neural responses in the limbic area and prefrontal cortex, such as the dlPFC, vmPFC, hippocampus, and amygdala, showed decreased activity during repeated stress induction. The level of neural habituation in these areas could signal a blunted endocrine-stress response. Moreover, the level of neural habituation was negatively correlated with the resilience level but positively correlated with the depression level, highlighting the maladaptive role of the blunted stress response. Furthermore, the level of resilience could moderate the relationship between neural habituation and blunted stress, which underlies the mechanism by which resilience can generate protective effects on stressful events. Notably, the same neural habituation tendency and relationship to blunted stress response are also found in the MIST paradigm, which suggests that the neural habituation level is also positively correlated with the chronic stress level (which is negatively correlated with the endocrine response). Overall, our study provides a novel perspective for considering the neural mechanism of the blunted stress response, offers a potential bridge between psychiatric disorders and blunted stress responses, and reveals how resilience could help the organism fight against the maladaptive response.

Perhaps the most profound and convincing theory for a blunted stress response is the motivational dysregulation theory, which considers blunted psychological stress reactivity as a marker of central motivational dysregulation (Carroll, David Batty, Mortensen, Deary, & Phillips, Reference Carroll, David Batty, Mortensen, Deary and Phillips2011; Carroll, Lovallo, & Phillips, Reference Carroll, Lovallo and Phillips2009). A review found that all the factors associated with a blunted stress response, such as addiction (Paris, Franco, Sodano, Frye, & Wulfert, Reference Paris, Franco, Sodano, Frye and Wulfert2010), depression (Phillips, Hunt, Der, & Carroll, Reference Phillips, Hunt, Der and Carroll2011), and anxiety (Bibbey, Ginty, Brindle, Phillips, & Carroll, Reference Bibbey, Ginty, Brindle, Phillips and Carroll2016), share a complaint of motivation dysregulation (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Ginty, Whittaker, Lovallo and de Rooij2017). In the laboratory stress test, motivational tasks (e.g. public speaking or cognitive tasks) are important components of stress induction (Shields, Reference Shields2017), by manipulating participants' performance on a high error level, giving negative feedback after each wrong answer, inducing feelings of uncontrollability, and threatening the social self (Noack, Nolte, Nieratschker, Habel, & Derntl, Reference Noack, Nolte, Nieratschker, Habel and Derntl2019), which could affect motivation level, and further lead to a significant psychological and endocrine stress response (Foley & Kirschbaum, Reference Foley and Kirschbaum2010). Empirical research from animal and human studies has shown that acute stress can disrupt motivation functions related to reward (Bai, Belin, Zheng, Liu, & Zhang, Reference Bai, Belin, Zheng, Liu and Zhang2017; Hollon, Burgeno, & Phillips, Reference Hollon, Burgeno and Phillips2015). More recently, a study also found that a threatening stimulus during acute stress can lead to motivational disengagement, and the level of motivation decline could signal blunted cardiovascular reactivity (Hase, aan het Rot, de Miranda Azevedo, & Freeman, Reference Hase, aan het Rot, de Miranda Azevedo and Freeman2020).

At the neural level, the limbic system and prefrontal cortex, such as the cingulate gyrus and amygdala, are key regions that support motivation (Ginty et al., Reference Ginty, Gianaros, Derbyshire, Phillips and Carroll2013; Phillips, Ginty, & Hughes, Reference Phillips, Ginty and Hughes2013). The dlPFC is vital for purchasing goals and interplays with motivation circuits, such as the ACC and vmPFC, to construct a network for goal regulation (Lee & Reeve, Reference Lee and Reeve2020; Spielberg et al., Reference Spielberg, Miller, Warren, Engels, Crocker, Banich and Heller2012). The hippocampus and amygdala are also essential areas for generating motivation (Cardinal, Parkinson, Hall, & Everitt, Reference Cardinal, Parkinson, Hall and Everitt2002; Jarrard, Reference Jarrard1973), and their activation could ensure a sufficient cortisol response (Khalili-Mahani, Dedovic, Engert, Pruessner, & Pruessner, Reference Khalili-Mahani, Dedovic, Engert, Pruessner and Pruessner2010). Previous studies have suggested that deactivation in these areas during acute stress could signal a blunted stress response (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Ginty, Whittaker, Lovallo and de Rooij2017; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Ginty and Hughes2013). In the current study, rather than deactivation, the activity decline in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus was accompanied by the blunted endocrine stress response. This may reflect a reduced effort and motivation in answering the questions correctly. Taken together, we suggested that neural habituation during acute stress could reflect a motivation disengagement during repeated failures and negative feedback.

In line with our hypothesis, the results showed that mental health (depression and chronic stress) related to blunted stress was positively correlated with neural habituation levels. We validated these results using two paradigms, respectively. In the ScanSTRESS study, we found greater right hippocampus and amygdala habituation in people with a higher level of depression, whereas in the MIST study, we found greater right insula habituation in people with a higher level of chronic stress. Depression and chronic disorders are associated with prefrontal and limbic system dysregulation (Dwivedi et al., Reference Dwivedi, Roy, Lugli, Rizavi, Zhang and Smalheiser2015; Ren et al., Reference Ren, Zhao, Li, Liu, Ren, Pruessner and Yang2022; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Dai, Chen, Liu, Xin, Zhou and Becker2021; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Yang, Tang, Zhou, Cheng, Zhou and Xie2013). For example, the depression level could alter the prefrontal cortex and limbic system function both at rest and during a task (Mujica-Parodi, Cha, & Gao, Reference Mujica-Parodi, Cha and Gao2017; Pannekoek et al., Reference Pannekoek, Veer, Van Tol, Van der Werff, Demenescu, Aleman and Van der Wee2013), especially in reward-related tasks (Gold, Morey, & McCarthy, Reference Gold, Morey and McCarthy2015; Hu, Reference Hu2018). In addition, chronic stress can disrupt the brain functions responsible for motivation (Evans & Stecker, Reference Evans and Stecker2004; Harmon, Greenwald, McFarland, Beckwith, & Cromwell, Reference Harmon, Greenwald, McFarland, Beckwith and Cromwell2009). In the current study, we infer that neural habituation could reflect motivation dysregulation, which suggests that neural habituation might serve as a general neural mechanism to explain the relationships between these psychiatric disorders and a blunted stress response.

Interestingly, the results showed that neural habituation in the prefrontal cortex is negatively correlated with resilience. Resilience can help individuals regulate negative emotions and generate adaptive coping behaviors during acute stress (Min, Yu, Lee, & Chae, Reference Min, Yu, Lee and Chae2013; Tugade & Fredrickson, Reference Tugade and Fredrickson2007). Animal and human studies have identified that resilience can affect reward circuits and protect motivation functions (Der-Avakian, Mazei-Robison, Kesby, Nestler, & Markou, Reference Der-Avakian, Mazei-Robison, Kesby, Nestler and Markou2014; Vythilingam et al., Reference Vythilingam, Nelson, Scaramozza, Waldeck, Hazlett, Southwick and Ernst2009). Besides, since motivation and resilience are highly synergistic, some researchers have reported that motivation maintenance ability could be considered a direct indicator of resilience (Martin, Reference Martin2002; Resnick, Reference Resnick2011). The prefrontal cortex, especially the vmPFC, is a key component of stress resilience (Holz et al., Reference Holz, Tost and Meyer-Lindenberg2020; Sinha et al., Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Todd Constable and Seo2016). A negative correlation between prefrontal habituation and resilience may underlie the protective mechanism of resilience, thus maintaining a relatively high motivation level despite uncontrollable, unpredictable, and threatening situations. Furthermore, the results also showed that resilience moderated the relationship between the vmPFC (right MFG, orb) habituation and endocrine stress response. Specifically, vmPFC habituation and a blunted endocrine response only existed in the low resilience group; interestingly, the high resilience group showed increased rather than decreased activity during acute stress. This phenomenon validates the positive correlation between an increase in vmPFC activity and resilience in previous research (Sinha et al., Reference Sinha, Lacadie, Todd Constable and Seo2016), which means resilience could reduce vmPFC habituation, even regulate vmPFC activity to increase adversity and provide a sufficient stress response.

In general, our research used a neural habituation perspective in consideration of the neural activity and motivation function during stress conditions, which not only provided solid evidence for the motivational dysregulation theory but also holds great methodological importance. Importantly, the current study recommended employing neural habituation rather than mean amplitude to measure neural activity during stress due to the following reasons. First, since the results showed significant neural activation differences across different sessions and blocks, using the mean amplitude to measure the neural activity during stress may decrease the sensitivity in detecting individual differences (Plichta et al., Reference Plichta, Grimm, Morgen, Mier, Sauer, Haddad and Meyer-Lindenberg2014). Second, the difficulty of building stable connections between neural systems and other systems, such as endocrine and psychological systems, has been reported in previous research (Henze et al., Reference Henze, Konzok, Kreuzpointner, Bärtl, Peter, Giglberger and Wüst2020). This inconsistency hinders researchers from unveiling interactions between different systems. Our results suggest that neural habituation could build stable relationships with the endocrine system. They indicate that neural habituation could serve as a potential neural marker that bridges the stress response at the neural, endocrine, physiological, and psychological levels. Third, the inconsistent results between different stress paradigms is another obstacle in the stress area (Berretz, Packheiser, Kumsta, Wolf, & Ocklenburg, Reference Berretz, Packheiser, Kumsta, Wolf and Ocklenburg2021; Noack et al., Reference Noack, Nolte, Nieratschker, Habel and Derntl2019), and low general ability makes it impossible to compare results between different paradigms. Our results showed consistency across different paradigms, offering an indicator of horizontal contrast between different paradigms. Lastly, when dividing participants into cortisol responders and non-responders, there is a huge difference in neural habituation between responders and non-responders. Besides, the non-responder group contains more males than females. Former research has reported stronger endocrine response during stress in female participants (Van Paridon, Timmis, Nevison, & Bristow, Reference Van Paridon, Timmis, Nevison and Bristow2017; Weekes et al., Reference Weekes, Lewis, Goto, Garrison-Jakel, Patel and Lupien2008), our study may underlie a neural mechanism to explain this gender difference.

This study has some limitations. First, the whole-brain level significant change between different sessions in the ScanSTRESS paradigm was not replicated in the MIST paradigms, potentially due to differences in task duration and difficulty. Future research should investigate these factors' influence on neural habituation during stress. Second, neural activity increased during control blocks, which may be due to participants gaining a sense of control and coping experience from high accuracy during the control task. Further research is needed to identify the psychological components of control blocks. Finally, the study did not measure resilience in the MIST paradigm, which may limit the generalizability of moderate results. Additionally, measuring hair cortisol levels could provide more insight into the relationship between neural habituation and the endocrine stress system in chronic stress.

Conclusions

These findings offer a general neural mechanism for the blunted endocrine stress response and the relationship between mental disorders and the blunted stress response, as well as how protective factors such as resilience could protect against this maladaptive process.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001666

Acknowledgments

We thank Jiwen Li and Mengxue Lan for their helpful advice and data collection.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31971019, 32271133), the National Key Research Development Program of China (STI 2030—Major Projects: 2022ZD0211000), and the Social Science Foundation of Chongqing (2021YC029), China.

Conflict of interest

The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

All participants provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Southwest University, China (No. H22008).

Footnotes

*

These authors equally contributed to this work.

References

Akdeniz, C., Tost, H., Streit, F., Haddad, L., Wüst, S., Schäfer, A., … Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2014). Neuroimaging evidence for a role of neural social stress processing in ethnic minority-associated environmental risk. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(6), 672680. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, Y., Belin, D., Zheng, X., Liu, Z., & Zhang, Y. (2017). Acute stress worsens the deficits in appetitive behaviors for social and sexual stimuli displayed by rats after long-term withdrawal from morphine. Psychopharmacology, 234(11), 16931702. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4571-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., Kumsta, R., Wolf, O. T., & Ocklenburg, S. (2021). The brain under stress – A systematic review and activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of changes in BOLD signal associated with acute stress exposure. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 124(September 2020), 8999. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bibbey, A., Ginty, A. T., Brindle, R. C., Phillips, A. C., & Carroll, D. (2016). Blunted cardiac stress reactors exhibit relatively high levels of behavioural impulsivity. Physiology and Behavior, 159, 4044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackford, J. U., Allen, A. H., Cowan, R. L., & Avery, S. N. (2013). Amygdala and hippocampus fail to habituate to faces in individuals with an inhibited temperament. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(2), 143150. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bratt, A. M., Kelley, S. P., Knowles, J. P., Barrett, J., Davis, K., Davis, M., & Mittleman, G. (2001). Long term modulation of the HPA axis by the hippocampus: Behavioral, biochemical and immunological endpoints in rats exposed to chronic mild stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(2), 121145. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00033-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvi, J. L., Chen, F. R., Benson, V. B., Brindle, E., Bristow, M., De, A., … Granger, D. A. (2017). Measurement of cortisol in saliva: A comparison of measurement error within and between international academic-research laboratories. BMC Research Notes, 10(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-017-2805-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao, H., Chen, O. Y., McEwen, S. C., Forsyth, J. K., Gee, D. G., Bearden, C. E., … Mirzakhanian, H. (2021). Cross-paradigm connectivity: Reliability, stability, and utility. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 15, 614629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J., & Everitt, B. J. (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(3), 321352. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00007-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, D., David Batty, G., Mortensen, L. H., Deary, I. J., & Phillips, A. C. (2011). Low cognitive ability in early adulthood is associated with reduced lung function in middle age: The Vietnam experience study. Thorax, 66(10), 884888. https://doi.org/10.1136/thoraxjnl-2011-200104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, D., Ginty, A. T., Whittaker, A. C., Lovallo, W. R., & de Rooij, S. R. (2017). The behavioural, cognitive, and neural corollaries of blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 7486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, D., Lovallo, W. R., & Phillips, A. C. (2009). Are large physiological reactions to acute psychological stress always bad for health? Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(5), 725743. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00205.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, D., Phillips, A. C., Hunt, K., & Der, G. (2007). Symptoms of depression and cardiovascular reactions to acute psychological stress: Evidence from a population study. Biological Psychology, 75(1), 6874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.12.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyr, N. E., & Romero, L. M. (2009). Identifying hormonal habituation in field studies of stress. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 161(3), 295303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2009.02.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dedovic, K., Duchesne, A., Andrews, J., Engert, V., & Pruessner, J. C. (2009). The brain and the stress axis: The neural correlates of cortisol regulation in response to stress. NeuroImage, 47(3), 864871. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Mahani, N. K., Engert, V., Lupien, S. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2005). The Montreal Imaging Stress Task: Using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30(5), 319325.Google ScholarPubMed
Der-Avakian, A., Mazei-Robison, M. S., Kesby, J. P., Nestler, E. J., & Markou, A. (2014). Enduring deficits in brain reward function after chronic social defeat in rats: Susceptibility, resilience, and antidepressant response. Biological Psychiatry, 76(7), 542549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwivedi, Y., Roy, B., Lugli, G., Rizavi, H., Zhang, H., & Smalheiser, N. R. (2015). Chronic corticosterone-mediated dysregulation of microRNA network in prefrontal cortex of rats: Relevance to depression pathophysiology. Translational Psychiatry, 5(11), e682e682. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., & Stecker, R. (2004). Motivational consequences of environmental stress. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(2), 143165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00076-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, P., & Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Human hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 9196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginty, A. T., Gianaros, P. J., Derbyshire, S. W. G., Phillips, A. C., & Carroll, D. (2013). Blunted cardiac stress reactivity relates to neural hypoactivation. Psychophysiology, 50(3), 219229. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, A. L., Morey, R. A., & McCarthy, G. (2015). Amygdala–prefrontal cortex functional connectivity during threat-induced anxiety and goal distraction. Biological Psychiatry, 77(4), 394403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gump, B. B., Stewart, P., Reihman, J., Lonky, E., Darvill, T., Parsons, P. J., & Granger, D. A. (2008). Low-level prenatal and postnatal blood lead exposure and adrenocortical responses to acute stress in children. Environmental Health Perspectives, 116(2), 249255. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.10391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamer, M., O'Donnell, K., Lahiri, A., & Steptoe, A. (2010). Salivary cortisol responses to mental stress are associated with coronary artery calcification in healthy men and women. European Heart Journal, 31(4), 424429. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehp386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmon, K. M., Greenwald, M. L., McFarland, A., Beckwith, T., & Cromwell, H. C. (2009). The effects of prenatal stress on motivation in the rat pup. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 12(3), 250258. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890802367265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hase, A., aan het Rot, M., de Miranda Azevedo, R., & Freeman, P. (2020). Threat-related motivational disengagement: Integrating blunted cardiovascular reactivity to stress into the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 33(4), 355369. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2020.1755819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henze, G. I., Konzok, J., Kreuzpointner, L., Bärtl, C., Peter, H., Giglberger, M., … Wüst, S. (2020). Increasing deactivation of limbic structures over psychosocial stress exposure time. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(7), 697704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.04.002.Google ScholarPubMed
Hollon, N. G., Burgeno, L. M., & Phillips, P. E. M. (2015). Stress effects on the neural substrates of motivated behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 14051412. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holz, N. E., Tost, H., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2020). Resilience and the brain: A key role for regulatory circuits linked to social stress and support. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(2), 379396. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0551-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howden, E. J., Perhonen, M., Peshock, R. M., Zhang, R., Arbab-Zadeh, A., Adams-Huet, B., & Levine, B. D. (2015). Females have a blunted cardiovascular response to one year of intensive supervised endurance training. Journal of Applied Physiology, 119(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00092.2015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, K. (2018). Neural activity to threat in ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with individual differences in anxiety and reward processing. Neuropsychologia, 117, 566573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, L. M. C., Gispen-de Wied, C. C., Gademan, P. J., De Jonge, R. C. J., Van Der Linden, J. A., & Kahn, R. S. (1998). Blunted cortisol response to a psychosocial stressor in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 33(1–2), 8794. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00066-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrard, L. E. (1973). The hippocampus and motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 79(1), 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalili-Mahani, N., Dedovic, K., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., & Pruessner, J. C. (2010). Hippocampal activation during a cognitive task is associated with subsequent neuroendocrine and cognitive responses to psychological stress. Hippocampus, 20(2), 323334. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, E. J., Pellman, B., & Kim, J. J. (2015). Stress effects on the hippocampus: A critical review. Learning and Memory, 22(9), 411416. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.037291.114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J. S., Han, S. Y., & Iremonger, K. J. (2019). Stress experience and hormone feedback tune distinct components of hypothalamic CRH neuron activity. Nature Communications, 10(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13639-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kogler, L., Müller, V. I., Chang, A., Eickhoff, S. B., Fox, P. T., Gur, R. C., & Derntl, B. (2015). Psychosocial versus physiological stress – Meta-analyses on deactivations and activations of the neural correlates of stress reactions. NeuroImage, 119, 235251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koning, A. S. C. A. M., Buurstede, J. C., Van Weert, L. T. C. M., & Meijer, O. C. (2019). Glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors in the brain: A transcriptional perspective. Journal of the Endocrine Society, 3(10), 19171930. https://doi.org/10.1210/js.2019-00158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krkovic, K., Clamor, A., & Lincoln, T. M. (2018). Emotion regulation as a predictor of the endocrine, autonomic, affective, and symptomatic stress response and recovery. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 94(December 2017), 112120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.04.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., … Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2011). City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature, 474(7352), 498501. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, W., & Reeve, J. (2020). Remembering pleasure and personal meaning from episodes of intrinsic motivation: An fMRI study. Motivation and Emotion, 44(6), 810818. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-020-09855-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, J. F., Akil, H., & Watson, S. J. (1999). Neural circuits mediating stress. Biological Psychiatry, 46(11), 14611471. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00266-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (2010). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in coping and resilience. Brain Research, 1355, 5260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. (2002). Motivation and academic resilience: Developing a model for student enhancement. Australian Journal of Education, 46(1), 3449. https://doi.org/10.1177/000494410204600104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarty, R. (2016). Learning about stress: Neural, endocrine and behavioral adaptations. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 19(5), 449475. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2016.1192120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Min, J. A., Yu, J. J., Lee, C. U., & Chae, J. H. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies contributing to resilience in patients with depression and/or anxiety disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(8), 11901197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.05.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Cha, J., & Gao, J. (2017). From anxious to reckless: A control systems approach unifies prefrontal-limbic regulation across the spectrum of threat detection. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 11, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2017.00018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noack, H., Nolte, L., Nieratschker, V., Habel, U., & Derntl, B. (2019). Imaging stress: An overview of stress induction methods in the MR scanner. Journal of Neural Transmission, 126(9), 11871202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-018-01965-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, J., Dimitrov, A., Oei, N. Y. L., Walter, H., Adli, M., & Veer, I. M. (2020). Association of naturally occurring sleep loss with reduced amygdala resting-state functional connectivity following psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 114(January), 104585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pagliaccio, D., Luby, J. L., Bogdan, R., Agrawal, A., Gaffrey, M. S., Belden, A. C., … Barch, D. M. (2014). Stress-system genes and life stress predict cortisol levels and amygdala and hippocampal volumes in children. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(5), 12451253. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pannekoek, J. N., Veer, I. M., Van Tol, M. J., Van der Werff, S. J. A., Demenescu, L. R., Aleman, A., … Van der Wee, N. J. A. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity abnormalities in limbic and salience networks in social anxiety disorder without comorbidity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 23(3), 186195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.04.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paris, J. J., Franco, C., Sodano, R., Frye, C. A., & Wulfert, E. (2010). Gambling pathology is associated with dampened cortisol response among men and women. Physiology and Behavior, 99(2), 230233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.04.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A., & McEwen, B. S. (2015). Stress habituation, body shape and cardiovascular mortality. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 56, 139150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, A. C., Ginty, A. T., & Hughes, B. M. (2013). The other side of the coin: Blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity are associated with negative health outcomes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, A. C., Hunt, K., Der, G., & Carroll, D. (2011). Blunted cardiac reactions to acute psychological stress predict symptoms of depression five years later: Evidence from a large community study. Psychophysiology, 48(1), 142148. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01045.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plichta, M. M., Grimm, O., Morgen, K., Mier, D., Sauer, C., Haddad, L., … Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2014). Amygdala habituation: A reliable fMRI phenotype. NeuroImage, 103, 383390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Khalili-Mahani, N., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Buss, C., … Lupien, S. (2008). Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: Evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63(2), 234240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(7), 916931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, M., Pruessner, J. C., Hellhammer, D. H., Bruce Pike, G., & Lupien, S. J. (2007). The associations among hippocampal volume, cortisol reactivity, and memory performance in healthy young men. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 155(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.12.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo, J., … Thompson, R. F. (2009). Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 135138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ren, X., Zhao, X., Li, J., Liu, Y., Ren, Y., Pruessner, J. C., & Yang, J. (2022). The hippocampal-ventral medial prefrontal cortex neurocircuitry involvement in the association of daily life stress with acute perceived stress and cortisol responses. Psychosomatic Medicine, 84(3), 276287. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000001058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resnick, B. (2011). The relationship between resilience and motivation. In B. Resnick, L. P. Gwyther, & K. A. Roberto (Eds.), Resilience in Aging: Concepts, Research, and Outcomes (pp. 199215). Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0232-0_13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roche, D. J. O., King, A. C., Cohoon, A. J., & Lovallo, W. R. (2013). Hormonal contraceptive use diminishes salivary cortisol response to psychosocial stress and naltrexone in healthy women. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 109, 8490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2013.05.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandner, M., Lois, G., Streit, F., Zeier, P., Kirsch, P., Wüst, S., & Wessa, M. (2020). Investigating individual stress reactivity: High hair cortisol predicts lower acute stress responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 118(March), 104660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharma, R., Smith, S. A., Boukina, N., Dordari, A., Mistry, A., Taylor, B. C., … Ismail, N. (2020). Use of the birth control pill affects stress reactivity and brain structure and function. Hormones and Behavior, 124, 104783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, G. S. (2017). Response: Commentary: The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(Dec), 25442551. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinha, R., Lacadie, C. M., Todd Constable, R., & Seo, D. (2016). Dynamic neural activity during stress signals resilient coping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(31), 88378842. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1600965113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sladky, R., Höflich, A., Atanelov, J., Kraus, C., Baldinger, P., Moser, E., … Windischberger, C. (2012). Increased neural habituation in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder revealed by fMRI. PLoS ONE, 7(11), e50050. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberg, J. M., Miller, G. A., Warren, S. L., Engels, A. S., Crocker, L. D., Banich, M. T., … Heller, W. (2012). A brain network instantiating approach and avoidance motivation. Psychophysiology, 49(9), 12001214. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01443.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, J. S., Kim, Y. J., Galatzer-Levy, I. R., Reddy, R., Ely, T. D., Nemeroff, C. B., … Ressler, K. J. (2017). Amygdala reactivity and anterior cingulate habituation predict posttraumatic stress disorder symptom maintenance after acute civilian trauma. Biological Psychiatry, 81(12), 10231029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.11.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2007). Regulation of positive emotions: Emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8(3), 311333. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-006-9015-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaishnavi, S., Connor, K., & Davidson, J. R. T. (2007). An abbreviated version of the Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC), the CD-RISC2: Psychometric properties and applications in psychopharmacological trials. Psychiatry Research, 152(2–3), 293297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.01.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Paridon, K. N., Timmis, M. A., Nevison, C. M., & Bristow, M. (2017). The anticipatory stress response to sport competition; A systematic review with meta-analysis of cortisol reactivity. BMJ Open Sport & Exercise Medicine, 3(1), e000261. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjsem-2017-000261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vythilingam, M., Nelson, E. E., Scaramozza, M., Waldeck, T., Hazlett, G., Southwick, S. M., … Ernst, M. (2009). Reward circuitry in resilience to severe trauma: An fMRI investigation of resilient special forces soldiers. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 172(1), 7577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.06.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weekes, N. Y., Lewis, R. S., Goto, S. G., Garrison-Jakel, J., Patel, F., & Lupien, S. (2008). The effect of an environmental stressor on gender differences on the awakening cortisol response. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(6), 766772. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, C. L., Babb, T. L., Halgren, E., Wang, M. L., & Crandall, P. H. (1984). Habituation of human limbic neuronal response to sensory stimulation. Experimental Neurology, 84(1), 7497. https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(84)90007-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, X., Dai, J., Chen, Y., Liu, C., Xin, F., Zhou, X., … Becker, B. (2021). Intrinsic connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and striato-limbic system respectively differentiate major depressive from generalized anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(4), 791798. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00868-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, C. G., Wang, X. D., Zuo, X. N., & Zang, Y. F. (2016). DPABI: Data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics, 14(3), 339351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Yang, D., Tang, G., Zhou, C., Cheng, K., Zhou, J., … Xie, P. (2013). Proteomics reveals energy and glutathione metabolic dysregulation in the prefrontal cortex of a rat model of depression. Neuroscience, 247, 191200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuo, X. N., Anderson, J. S., Bellec, P., Birn, R. M., Biswal, B. B., Blautzik, J., … Milham, M. P. (2014). An open science resource for establishing reliability and reproducibility in functional connectomics. Scientific Data, 1(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1038/sdata.2014.49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure. 1. ScanSTRESS paradigm and an overview of the experimental procedure. (a) In the stressful ‘performance phase’, participants were asked to solve challenging cognitive tasks (serial subtraction and mental rotation) under time pressure; in addition, two juries (a man and a woman) is presented on the screen to increase the social evaluative threat. Time limits were adapted to the individual's performance resulting in frequent failure; if the participants could not answer the question in time or correctly, there would be negative feedback shown on the screen as ‘work faster’ or ‘error’ insistently. In the meantime, one of the juries would press a red button in his/her hand and another jury would record the participant's failure; this could also increase participants' feelings of social threat. (b) In the control ‘relaxation phase’, participants went through similar although much easier tasks (find the matched figure or number) with abundant time, and the negative feedback from the screen and the juries were removed. Furthermore, the juries were instructed to look away to remove the social evaluative threat. (c) The order for conditions is counter-balanced, in the first run, the stimuli were given in the ‘control–stressful–control–stressful’ order; in the second run, the stimuli were given in the ‘stressful–control–stressful–control’ order. (d) After arriving at the laboratory, the participants were asked to rest for 30 min before entering the MRI scanner. During the scanning, a T1 image was acquired first, followed by a resting-state image. Immediately, the ScanSTRESS paradigm was used to induce a stress response for 22 min. Then, there was another resting-state scan and structural scan; lastly, participants were debriefed for 10 min before they left the laboratory. During this period, participants provided five saliva samples. The experimenter paused the scan to collect the second, third, and fourth samples. Participants were temporarily removed from the scanner for saliva sampling, after which they returned and completed head localization again. The first and fifth samples were collected outside the MRI scanner.

Figure 1

Figure. 2. Validation of stress induction in the ScanSTRESS paradigm. (a) Subjective stress report and salivary cortisol secretion during ScanSTRESS. (b) Neural response to stress induction in the ScanSTRESS paradigm (stress > control, FDR-corrected p < 0.05).

Figure 2

Figure. 3. Neural habituation between stressful conditions in the ScanSTRESS paradigm (FDR-corrected p < 0.05). (a) The interaction between block and stress. (b) Neural habituation during four blocks in the areas investigated.

Figure 3

Figure. 4. Correlation between neural habituation (first block–last block) and stress reactivity as well as behavioral data in the ScanSTRESS paradigm. Neural habituation on the whole-brain level as well as regions of interest located in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) [including superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG)], ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [including the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), SFG, orbital part (orb), SFGmorb, SFG medial part (med), IFGorb, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)], and limbic area (insula) are accompanied by a blunted CortiAUCg, and neural habituation in the left dlPFC (SFG) and left vmPFC (IFGorb) are negatively correlated with CortiAUCi. Besides, neural habituation in the right amygdala and right hippocampus is positively correlated with the level of depression level. Lastly, the neural habituation in left dlPFC (mainly the SFG) and vmPFC (including the left SFGmed, left MFGorb, and both IFGorbs) is negatively correlated with the level of resilience. CortiAUCg and CortiAUCi indicate the areas under the curve with respect to ground and areas under the curve with respect to increases for salivary cortisol, respectively. * indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.05.

Figure 4

Figure. 5. Correlation analysis between neural habituation and stress reactivity in the MIST. ROIs were selected based on the ScanSTRESS results. Neural habituation on the whole-brain level, as well as regions of interest located in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) [including superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG)], ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [including the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) medial part (med), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) triangular part (tri), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)], and limbic area (Insula), are accompanied by blunted CortiAUCi. Besides, neural habituation in the right insula is positively correlated with the level of chronic stress (p = 0.039, uncorrected). CortiAUCg and CortiAUCi indicate the areas under the curve with respect to ground and areas under the curve with respect to increases for salivary cortisol, respectively. * indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.05, ** indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.01, *** indicates FDR-corrected q < 0.001.

Supplementary material: File

Liu et al. supplementary material

Liu et al. supplementary material
Download Liu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.5 MB