Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4rdrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-19T11:51:23.109Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Working Memory and Language

An Overview of Key Topics

from Overview of the Handbook

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University

Summary

Working memory (WM) is our limited-capacity storage and processing (memory) system that permeates essential facets of our cognitive life such as arithmetic calculation, logical thinking, decision-making, prospective planning, language comprehension, and production. Since the very inception of WM in the early 1960s (Miller et al., 1960), its role in language acquisition and processing has been extensively investigated both empirically and theoretically by researchers from diverse fields of psychology and linguistics, accumulating an increasingly huge body of literature (e.g., see Baddeley, 2003; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993 for reviews of early studies). Notwithstanding, the field still lacks a comprehensive and updated profile of conceptualizing and implementing working memory in the broad domains of native and second language acquisition, processing, impairments, and training. In this chapter, we introduce a comprehensive handbook in which key areas of inquiry and practice in working memory and language are at the forefront and theoretical ingenuity and empirical robustness are integrated throughout.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

1.1 Introduction

Working memory (WM) is our limited-capacity storage and processing (memory) system that permeates essential facets of our cognitive life such as arithmetic calculation, logical thinking, decision-making, prospective planning, language comprehension, and production. Since the first elaboration on WM in the early 1960s (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Galanter and Pribram1960), its role in language acquisition and processing has been extensively investigated both empirically and theoretically by researchers from diverse fields of psychology and linguistics, accumulating an increasingly huge body of literature (e.g., see Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1993; Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2003 for reviews of early studies). Notwithstanding, the field still lacks a comprehensive and updated profile of conceptualizing and implementing working memory in the broad domains of native and second language acquisition, processing, and language impairments – a volume that is long overdue and expected by both WM theorists and language practitioners alike.

Indeed, after a careful examination of recently published titles on working memory and language learning, we find it a bit surprising that, given the enormous interest in this important and intriguing topic by scholars from multidisciplinary fields of cognitive psychology and language sciences, there has not yet been a volume that provides a comprehensive account of these accumulative research findings and empirical evidence. This is particularly so when we realized that Gathercole and Baddeley’s seminal monograph, Working memory and language, had been published in 1993, almost thirty years ago! The succeeding three decades following its publication have recorded an exponential growth of both empirical and theoretical investigations in either working memory or language learning, either separately as two independent disciplines or occasionally jointly investigated as one phenomenon of human cognition.

Separately, for example, we have seen, on the one hand, numerous empirical and theoretical papers addressing the nature, structure, and functions of WM per se. Plus, several edited volumes have offered comprehensive coverage of working memory theories and models in cognitive science and neuroscience. These have included, among others, Miyake and Shah’s (Reference Miyake and Shah1999) Models of working memory, Conway et al.’s (2005) Variation in working memory, and most recently, an edited volume by Logie et al. (Reference Logie, Camos and Cowan2021) entitled Working memory: State of science. At the other end of the pendulum, that is, language acquisition and processing, we have also seen numerous papers reporting both theoretical reviews and empirical studies in language learning per se; an apt example is the recent handbook compiled by the first editor, Schwieter and Benati (Reference Schwieter and Benati2019), though no such a volume has been set out to cover working memory and language issues specifically. Then, when working memory and language learning are considered as a joint venture, we have not found a comprehensive volume addressing the intricate relationship between working memory and the diverse and broad domains of first and second language learning, processing, and impairment.

As far as we know, there are only three other recent titles that are addressing a similar topic, and they have greatly inspired and helped pave the way for the current handbook. To begin with, Altarriba and Isurin’s edited volume (Reference Altarriba and Isurin2013), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches, has done an excellent job at showcasing some recent advances in studies on bilingual memory, language processing, and second language forgetting. They are also successful in integrating theoretical developments and real-world approaches to language learning from cognitive perspectives. Although this is very much appreciated in their title, we do feel that it was also a very concentrated and specialized book. It has served as a solid reference for bilingual processing and its interface with memory. That said, we still need a volume to speak more directly to the many other areas in which WM has been demonstrated to play a dynamic role in nuanced language research domains, such as first and second language acquisition, processing, bilingual development, not to mention the emerging domains of language impairment and cognitive training (e.g., Novick et al., Reference Novick, Bunting, Dougherty and Engle2019).

Then, in terms of working memory and first or second language learning, we have failed to identify any recent volumes reviewing this topic comprehensively and thoroughly. In terms of second language learning though, the monograph authored by Wen (Reference Wen2016), “Working memory and second language learning: Towards an integrated approach and its sister volume edited with collaborators (i.e., Wen et al., Reference Wen, Mota and McNeill2015, Working memory in second language acquisition and processing) feature some succinct reviews of working memory theories, assessment procedures, and empirical studies, thus providing important insights into the intricate relationship between working memory and second language acquisition and processing. They also provided inculcate thoughts regarding integrating working memory and second language acquisition research. But again, these two volumes have not been able to address adequately the intricate relationship between working memory and first language acquisition and processing; neither did they manage to cover the well-established areas of WM and linguistic theories and models, as well as the expanding literature of WM and bilingualism.

Given these obvious gaps in the research literature, we thus set out to compile this comprehensive handbook, with the goal in mind to fill up all these lacunae from previous research. Furthermore, we also aim for theoretical ingenuity and empirical robustness in our individual chapter reviews and devote independent sections to key areas of foundational theories, including working memory models and measures in cognitive psychology, as well as incorporating working memory within well-established linguistic theories and processing frameworks. As far as we know, many of these have not been done before. As such, we are hoping that the comprehensive coverage of key topics in all these essential areas in our handbook will benefit researchers and students not just from psychology and linguistics, but also readers from all other related fields of cognitive sciences to draw insights and inspirations from the chapters herein.

1.2 Organization of the Handbook

As briefly outlined above, we have thus compiled the handbook to cover all essential areas of the language sciences in which WM has been demonstrated to play a significant role. Specifically, the handbook is organized based upon the following seven parts (which somehow resonates with the very first impression of the “magical number seven” proposed by George Miller in 1956, another buzzword concept that has been associated with WM capacity that is widely known by almost everyone both inside and outside psychology).

  • Part I: Introduction

  • Part II: Theoretical Models and Measures

  • Part III: Linguistic Theories and Frameworks

  • Part IV: First Language Processing

  • Part V: Bilingual Acquisition and Processing

  • Part VI: Language Disorders, Interventions, and Instruction

  • Part VII: Conclusion

Following the present overview, Part I continues with a special introduction by Baddeley, arguably one of the most prominent scholars in WM. In the chapter, the author walks us through the history of various psycholinguistic models of language and memory and recounts how his classic WM model has evolved to address the arising challenges brought by language-related research. The chapter begins with a discussion of his seminal multicomponent model – which was developed during a time when Chomsky’s transformational grammar was at the forefront of psycholinguistics – and explores the link between short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM). After a brief discussion of the original tripartite WM model (Baddeley & Hitch, Reference Baddeley, Hitch and Bower1974), Baddeley summarizes related studies depicting the links between each component WM and language domains, the latest endeavors being focused on the relationship between the episodic buffer and binding. These reviews are also lending support to the distinction between verbal STM and WM. Toward the end, Baddeley acknowledges the different approaches between his own WM model and that of Popper’s view.

Part II contains major chapters dedicated to discussing prominent WM-language theories, frameworks, and methods. Chapter 3 by Coolidge and Wynn opens the section with an exhaustive overview of the evolution of WM and language. The authors make mention of the fact that one of the pros of Baddeley and Hitch’s multicomponent model is the fact that it encompasses all forms and types of memory, whereas prior to its development, language research primarily focused on acoustic or written language (i.e., a two-component model of memory). Throughout the chapter, the varying components of the multicomponent model including the phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, and the development of the central executive are discussed in relation to their roles in language/speech production. In Chapter 4, Papagno presents an updated view of the utilization of the phonological loop as a “language learning device” (Baddeley et al., Reference Baddeley, Gathercole and Papagno1998), a key thrust of the WM-language endeavors in the early decades. The chapter begins with an overview of the role of the phonological loop as outlined in Baddeley and Hitch’s multicomponent model of WM. Furthermore, the author also looks at various studies that investigate the functional role of the phonological loop (i.e., the ability to retain sequences of verbal items for a short period of time). The elaboration of the phonological loop is provided throughout the chapter as various studies are examined on healthy people, children learning their mother tongue, children and young adults learning a second language (L2), and, crucially, on neuropsychological patients with a selective deficit of auditory-verbal short-term memory. In all, the studies suggest that the primary function of the phonological loop is to hold new phonological representations in the memory long enough to build permanent representation. The author discusses how this explanation can be applied to what occurs during new language learning.

Chapter 5 by Adams, Forsberg, and Cowan presents a critical overview of the Embedded-Processes Model of WM and its implications for language use. The Embedded-Processes Model was initially proposed in Cowan (Reference Cowan1988) and was more formalized later in Cowan (Reference Cowan1999). The model posits that WM is a collection of mechanisms that permit information to remain in an activated state. The authors first outline the basic concepts necessary to fully understand the model and then delve deeper into how each of its components is relevant to language use. They also examine the use of WM in both children and adults who are learning an L2. The premise of the chapter is to provide the foundational concepts necessary to understand the critical role that attention and LTM play in relation to successful language utilization at all levels. Chapter 6 by Adams and Delaney reviews long-term WM (LT-WM) and language comprehension. The LT-WM model (Ericsson & Kinstch, Reference Ericsson and Kintsch1995) was developed to explain how individuals are able to effectively encode critical information into their LTM despite the somewhat limited capacity and constraints of WM. The authors go through the history of the LT-WM theory, starting with the rationale for its conception in the mid-1990s. Following this, there is an in-depth analysis of how the theory accounts for central phenomena in discourse comprehension and also how more recent work examining the theory proposes the involvement of syntactic processing. The chapter ends with a description of recent studies that review the relationship between neural activity and LT-WM in reasoning skills and language comprehension.

Purg, Ozimič, and Repovš in Chapter 7 offer insight into the cognitive neuroscience of WM and language. There is a notable close relationship between WM and language as cognitive systems and as such, there is overlap and integration of brain systems and networks that support these processes. Through an examination of theoretical models and empirical evidence provided by a diverse range of study types and research methods, the chapter focuses on the brain substrate which plays a role in WM and language processing from the perspectives of their interconnectedness, synergy, and integration. In Chapter 8, Hitch, Hurlstone, and Hartley focus on computational models of WM for language processing. The authors focus on explaining how the limited capacity of the phonological loop utilized in verbal WM deals with information about serial order using computational models of the immediate recall of unstructured sequences of words, letters, or digits that provided baseline findings. The comparison of these computational models and the findings exemplify a serial ordering mechanism known as competitive queuing, a process that is evidenced to operate under various forms of linguistic constraint. In all, the chapter suggests that competitive queuing can potentially be a mechanism for unifying theories of WM and language processing.

Chapter 9 by Camos and Barrouillet tracks the development of the Time-Based Resource Sharing (TBRS) model of WM for language (Barrouillet & Camos, Reference Barrouillet and Camos2015, Reference Barrouillet, Camos, Logie, Camos and Cowan2020). The model investigates the function and development of WM specifically via the integration of executive attention and the timed components of cognitive processes. The authors take an in-depth look at the distinctions between domain-specific systems of maintenance that supervise verbal information and the domain-general system relying on attention. Throughout the chapter, the authors demonstrate how verbal information is maintained in WM from childhood to adulthood, how linguistic features impact the short-term memory capacity of verbal information, and how WM mechanisms in the model impact the creation of true and false verbal LTM traces. Rönnberg, Holmer, and Rudner transition to reviewing the Ease of Language Understanding model (Rönnberg, Reference Rönnberg2003) in Chapter 10. The premise of this model is that speed and accuracy of the signal matching in relation to existing multimodal language representations are determining factors of the ease of language comprehension. Furthermore, the authors examine the difference in matching pace and language understanding from the point of predictions and postdictions in WM. Ultimately, the interaction between WM and LTM is critical to language understanding and the importance of its efficiency becomes apparent in cases where its breakdown results in cognitive decline and dementia.

Dehn’s Chapter 11 discusses assessing WM in youth. Given that language development, acquisition of academic skills, and performance of academic skills are heavily reliant on WM, assessing children’s WM is an essential consideration in cases in which it is recommended that children have neuropsychological and psychological evaluations. Children with a low WM capacity are prone to academic learning difficulties, specific learning disabilities (SLD), and deficits such as dyslexia (Alloway and Archibald, Reference Alloway and Archibald2008; Pickering, Reference Pickering, Alloway and Gathercole2006). The author also looks at the assessment measures that can be used to examine both storage and processing (e.g., the backward digit span test) and they also identify several assessments that are critically needed to measure WM function in children. In Chapter 12 by Burgoyne et al., the authors illustrate the importance of WM capacity and attention control measures for language researchers. The chapter investigates the origins of complex span measures of WM capacity to understand the underlying cognitive processes of language comprehension. Following this, there is a review of the evidence that supports an executive attention perspective to WM (Kane & Engle, 2002) – and a description of the relationship between WM capacity, attention control, and language comprehension. Specific attention is paid to how the functions supported by attention control contribute to performance across a variety of cognitive tasks. In closing, the authors provide recommendations and resources for researchers whose work involves measuring WM capacity and attention control.

Part III presents important theoretical contributions from general linguistics. In Chapter 13, Hawkins provides insight on whether or not grammars are shaped by WM. The author examines variation patterns across the world’s grammars specifically in relation to psycholinguistic WM models. The following arguments are made and supported with compelling evidence: (1) constrained capacity WM exceeds some limit, (2) more versus less WM models are reflected in preferred vs dispreferred structures, and (3) integrated WM models that interact with other factors facilitate processing ease. Then, in Chapter 14, Amici, Sanchez-Amaro, and Cacchione discuss the link between branching and WM within a cross-linguistic approach. The authors first review the differences in processing information habits retained beyond the linguistic domain across languages as this affects how humans process stimuli other than words in a sentence. The chapter reflects on previously conducted studies that explore the association between word order and attention allocation and further investigate the effects of branching habits beyond the linguistic domain. The concluding sections promote a stronger cross-cultural approach to the study of branching and WM.

In Chapter 15, O’Grady shifts focus toward WM and natural syntax. The chapter begins with a brief review of the birth of cognitive science with Miller’s (Reference Miller1956) paper on human memory and the storage of information. This seminal paper provided the foundation of the capacity limits of short-term memory being established at 7±2 units of information. Subsequent work by Yngve (Reference Yngve1960) builds on Miller’s work and suggested that these capacity limits apply to “immediate memory” in sentence production, a critical factor explaining the complexity of English syntax. The three lines of inquiry posited by Chomsky (Reference Chomsky1956) are also identified and reviewed. Finally, the author discusses the Performance-Grammar Correspondence Hypothesis (Hawkins, Reference Hawkins2014) and speaks about how his differing approach is more rooted in emergentist thought. Chapter 16 by Xu and Liu offers a review of research on the role of WM in shaping syntactic dependency structures. The chapter looks at the relationship between dependency distance, the constraint of WM, and the least effort principle. More specifically, the authors indicate that the latter two variables are typically organized in a way that reduces dependency distance, which ultimately shapes the patterns of word order in human languages and may account for the linguistic universals seen in language typology. Finally, the chapter examines whether syntactic structures are the result of self-adaptation of language systems as shaped by external constraints and motivations including WM.

Truscott and Sharwood Smith report on WM and the Modular Cognition Framework (Truscott & Sharwood Smith, Reference Truscott and Sharwood Smith2004) in Chapter 17. The authors emphasize the involvement of the framework in language development and WM. WM and its capacity are considered integral parts of the cognitive system and as such, the authors apply this view to the study of language through their discussion of processing, learning, metalinguistic knowledge, bilingual control, codeswitching, the issue of selective versus nonselective access in bilingual processing, optionality in SLA, and translation and interpreting. In Lu and Wen’s Chapter 18, the authors explore the role of WM and the language acquisition device (LAD; Chomsky, 1965). To resolve the issue of the elusive nature of the distinction between STM and WM limitations as they relate to language processing, the authors propose an integrated memory and chunking-based metric of comprehension complexity. They posit that during language processing, STM limitations of 7±2 chunks (Miller, Reference Miller1956) are sensitive to the instant chunk number (i.e., the number of information chunks that the parser has kept active in mind when processing a new word) whereas WM limitations of 4±1 (Cowan, Reference Cowan2001) are sensitive to the mean dependency distance (i.e., the sum of all instant chunk numbers divided by the number of words in the construction). They provide examples of their metric calculations and discuss some of their limitations.

Part IV reviews research on WM and first language (L1) processing and use. In Chapter 19 by Kim, the author reviews research on WM in word reading. This section provides a summary of the previous studies conducted on word reading from the perspective of Baddeley’s multicomponent WM model. The chapter also reviews studies on WM in Chinese word reading and offers clear directions for future research. Zahn, Horne, and Martin, in Chapter 20, examine the role of WM in language comprehension and production from neuropsychological perspectives. They address the role of verbal WM (specifically, the contributions of phonological and semantic WM buffers) in processes such as language production and comprehension, drawing on a mixture of data collected from brain-damaged and healthy individuals.

Pérez Muñoz and Bajo discuss WM and high-level text comprehension processes in Chapter 21. The authors review a substantial body of research concerning how cognitive processes are supported by WM during online L1 and L2 text comprehension. Pérez and Bajo argue that WM is particularly critical when text comprehension requires updating of the situation model through the inhibition of nonrelevant information in the L1, but this relationship is still unclear in the L2. Chapter 22 by Swets and Ivanova addresses WM and speech planning. The authors examine how WM fine-tunes the flexibility of speech planning strategies and review the role of WM in individual levels of planning. Ultimately, they conclude that speech planning is determined by a complex system of compensatory factors that includes WM. In Chapter 23, Olive reviews how novice and skilled writers engage WM. The chapter utilizes specific perspectives that analyze how writing processes engage both the executive and nonexecutive components of WM.

Part V synthesizes work on WM and L2 acquisition, processing, and use. Chapter 24 by Service and Simard reports on how measures of WM relate to L2 vocabulary. The authors aim to distinguish the differences between the intersecting effects of the components of WM, vocabulary knowledge, and methodological choices (i.e., measurement instruments) as they pertain to different aspects of L2 vocabulary knowledge. The authors critically examine previous research and bring to light their differing methodologies and implications of their results. The chapter concludes with a proposition of new perspectives on the interpretations of tasks typically used in WM and L2 vocabulary studies. In Chapter 25 by Leseman and Verhagen, the authors discuss WM and L2 grammar development in children. In reviewing previous work on statistical learning in first language acquisition and on cue competition in L2 learners, they suggest that the relationships between WM and L2 grammar are bidirectional and based on general mechanisms of perception, extraction, and integration of statistical information in the surrounding language. Specifically, they propose that children learn grammar in an L2 through chunking exemplars of constructions and that the resultant chunks in long-term memory, in turn, aid perception and attention, and enhance working memory capacity to facilitate further L2 learning.

McCormick and Sanz’s Chapter 26 examines WM and L2 grammar development among adults. The authors review a plethora of studies that explain the differences in developmental rates such as the “more is better” hypothesis. McCormick and Sanz argue that future research may benefit from examining the role of WM in cognitively demanding tasks, which happen to change during L2 learning. In Chapter 27, Cunnings critically evaluates the various models of WM explaining L2 sentence comprehension. He also reviews existing studies that examine how WM influences L2 sentence processing and offers a concluding discussion on individual differences in WM and sentence processing with suggestions as to how to best measure it. In Chapter 28, Leeser and Herman report on methodological issues in research on WM and L2 reading comprehension. They argue that decisions about study designs can have significant effects on research outcomes and provide suggestions as to how to rectify these issues. This chapter also discusses the importance of understanding reading comprehension processes and their various components as these are vital for the promotion of L2 literacy skills and linguistic development. Finally, the authors discuss the fundamental issues that must be taken into consideration for more productive research to be done in this field.

Skehan in Chapter 29 discusses WM and L2 speaking tasks. He examines the relationships between WM and performance on L2 speaking tasks. Based on the research discussed in the chapter, the author argues that WM plays a role in the formulation stage of speech production and proposes areas in which it would be the most helpful to research WM connections with L2 speaking tasks. In Chapter 30 by An and Li, the authors report on WM and L2 interaction. The chapter begins with an overview of WM as it pertains to L2 acquisition. Following this, a summary of the methods and findings from empirical research examining the role of WM in L2 interaction is noted. This synthesis focuses on three topics: (1) WM and interactional behaviors; (2) WM and task performance; and (3) WM and interaction-driven L2 gains. The chapter concludes with a discussion of the limitations, pedagogical implications, and future research directions.

In Chapter 31, Zheng and Kuang analyze WM in interpreting studies by conducting a systematic review of work from the 1970s to the 2010s. Special attention is paid to simultaneous interpreting (SI) and consecutive interpreting (CI). The primary goal of the chapter is to fill the gap in SI and CI research by reviewing major WM models of interpreting, determining the similarities and differences between SI and CI, and examining relevant empirical evidence that either validates or invalidates the models. New research directions that can be used to investigate SI and CI are proposed. Chapter 32 by Shin and Hu begins by first discussing a methodological synthesis of WM tasks in L2 research. The authors review the last 20 years of WM and L2 learning research in order to describe WM task designs, scoring methods, and reporting practices. The chapter contributes to the ongoing dialogue promoting methodological rigor of WM measurement in L2 research (Wen et al., Reference Wen, Juffs, Winke, Winke and Brunfaut2021).

Part VI turns to engage readers with a dialogue on WM, language disorders, interventions, and instruction. Chapter 33 by Swanson presents a critical overview of specific learning disorders due to a WM deficit. The author draws on recent studies that suggest that growth in the executive component of WM is significantly related to growth in reading and/or math for children with specific learning disorders in these areas. Chapter 34 by Montgomery, Gillam, and Evans reviews a new perspective on the connection between memory and sentence comprehension in children with developmental language disorder (DLD). The authors present the first conceptually integrated and empirically validated model of sentence comprehension for school-age children with DLD which takes into account and describes the structural relationship between the aforementioned abilities. Morgan’s Chapter 35 examines WM and deafness while focusing on the development of WM in children who are born deaf. The author utilizes studies of deaf users of spoken and signed languages from both the medical and social models of deafness and reviews how variance in WM capacities has previously been attributed to deaf and hearing children. The chapter concludes that deafness disrupts early language exposure and reduces rehearsal abilities, ultimately impacting the WM’s operating system.

Chapter 36 by Alloway, Carpenter, Robinson, and Frankenstein tracks the development of WM training in the classroom. The authors characterized WM training programs into two categories: those narrow or broad in scope. Furthermore, they also look at the efficacy of WM training as it pertains to transfer effects and report whether these effects are short- or long-term (maintenance effects). They also provide recommendations for implementing WM in the classroom. Holmes, Byrne, and Graham transition to WM and classroom learning in Chapter 37. The scholars discuss how minimal WM resources constrain classroom learning with a specific focus on the impacts of poor WM on the ability of children to follow learning-activity-relevant instructions and classroom management tasks. Finally, various methods that can be employed to assist these children are discussed, including an overview of current ideas about memory enhancement via brain stimulation and training and practical methods that can inform pedagogy.

In Chapter 38 by Sweller, Roussel, and Tricot, the authors discuss cognitive load theory and instructional design for language learning which begins with employing evolutionary education psychology to determine which categories of knowledge are instructionally relevant. Chapter 39 by Tullo and Jaeggi conducts a meta-analysis of studies on WM training that explore the mechanistic properties and effectiveness of WM training. The authors propose alternative approaches that may fill gaps that traditional interventions have left open.

Finally, in Part VII, the handbook concludes with Chapter 40, a summative piece written by the editors Wen and Schwieter. This first part of the chapter highlights and synthesizes multiple perspectives of WM from cognitive science as augmented by the major chapters of relevant sections in the handbook, which culminates in unifying characterizations of the construct. Then, the second part proposes an integrated account and taxonomy of WM (i.e., the phonological/executive model; Wen, Reference Wen2016 & Reference Wen, Wen, Skehan, Biedron, Li and Sparks2019) as a viable framework for language sciences and bilingualism research in the future.

1.3 Conclusion

In short, we hope that as collectively demonstrated throughout The Cambridge Handbook of Working Memory and Language, the broad fields of working memory and language sciences can now have a more interactive platform and thus can move closer not just to portray a much clearer picture of the emerging patterns of the interaction between WM and language processes, but also make a head start at more seamless integration that will benefit both disciplines and many others. So far, we have come to appreciate that the multiple components and functions of WM are playing critical albeit distinctive roles in essential aspects of language acquisition and processing and impairment in general (Wen, Reference Wen2016). But beyond these, we also realize that there is still much to explore further, particularly toward deeper integration between the two independent disciplines of working memory in cognitive science on the one hand and language sciences on the other. Such an ambition has constituted not just the rationale and motivations of this handbook in the very first place, but also represents the vision and mission of this handbook. To allow this to happen, we thus call on more researchers and students from across the multiple fields to join us to embark on this rewarding journey to further study and learn more about WM and language, which proves to be the ultimate joy and reward of editing this enormous handbook.

References

Alloway, T., & Archibald, L. (2008). Working memory and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 251262.Google Scholar
Altarriba, J., & Isurin, L. (2013). Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Atkinson, R., & Shiffrin, R. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. & Spence, J. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 89195). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189208.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 4789). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2015). Working memory: Loss and reconstruction. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2020). The time-based resource-sharing model of WM. In Logie, R., Camos, V., Cowan, N. (Eds), Working memory: State of the science (pp. 85115). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1956). Three models for the description of language. Institute of Radio Engineers Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 113124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A., Jarrold, C., Kane, M., Miyake, A., & Towse, J. (Eds.) (2007). Variation in working memory. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104(2), 163.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control, 20, 506.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behaviour and Brain Sciences, 24, 87185.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1993). Working memory and language. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. (2014). Cross-linguistic variation and efficiency. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 189217.Google Scholar
Logie, R., Camos, V., & Cowan, N. (Eds.). (2021). Working memory: State of the science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Novick, J., Bunting, M., Dougherty, M., & Engle, R. (2019). Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. (1956). The magical number of seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63 (3), 8197.Google Scholar
Miller, G., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. Holt.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pickering, S. (2006). WM in dyslexia. In Alloway, T. & Gathercole, S. (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 740). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rönnberg, J. (2003). Cognition in the hearing impaired and deaf as a bridge between signal and dialogue: A framework and a model. International Journal of Audiology, 42 (Suppl 1), S68S76.Google Scholar
Schwieter, J. W., & Benati, A. (2019). the cambridge handbook of language learning. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thiessen, E. D., & Erickson, L. C. (2013). Beyond word segmentation: A two-process account of statistical learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 239243.Google Scholar
Truscott, J., & Sharwood Smith, M. (2004). Acquisition by processing: A modular approach to language development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 120.Google Scholar
Wen, Z. (2016). Working memory and second language learning: Towards an integrated approach. Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Wen, Z. (2019). Working memory as language aptitude: The Phonological/Executive Model. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedron, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (Eds.), Language aptitude: Advancing theory, testing, research and practice (pp. 189214). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., Juffs, A., & Winke, P. (2021). Measuring working memory. In Winke, P. & Brunfaut, T. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and testing (pp. 167176). Routledge.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., Mota, M. & McNeill, M. (2015). Working memory in second language acquisition and processing. Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Yngve, Victor. (1960). A model and a hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 104, 444466.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×