Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T11:39:04.563Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association of trauma, post-traumatic stress disorder and non-affective psychosis across the life course: a nationwide prospective cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2021

Judith Allardyce*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neuroscience, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences (Division of Psychiatry), University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland
Anna-Clara Hollander
Affiliation:
Dept of Global Public Health, Karolinksa Institutet, Solna, Sweden
Syed Rahman
Affiliation:
Dept of Global Public Health, Karolinksa Institutet, Solna, Sweden
Christina Dalman
Affiliation:
Division of Public Health Epidemiology, Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden Psykisk Hälsa, Centrum för epidemiologi och samhällsmedicin, Stockholm, Sweden
Stan Zammit
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neuroscience, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Centre for Academic Mental Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, England
*
Author for correspondence: Judith Allardyce, E-mail: judith.allardyce@ed.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

We aimed to examine the temporal relationships between traumatic events (TE), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and non-affective psychotic disorders (NAPD).

Methods

A prospective cohort study of 1 965 214 individuals born in Sweden between 1971 and 1990 examining the independent effects of interpersonal and non-interpersonal TE on incidence of PTSD and NAPD using data from linked register data (Psychiatry-Sweden). Mediation analyses tested the hypothesis that PTSD lies on a causal pathway between interpersonal trauma and NAPD.

Results

Increasing doses of interpersonal and non-interpersonal TE were independently associated with increased risk of NAPD [linear-trend incidence rate ratios (IRR)adjusted = 2.17 [95% confidence interval (CI) 2.02–2.33] and IRRadjusted = 1.27 (95% CI 1.23–1.31), respectively]. These attenuated to a relatively small degree in 5-year time-lagged models. A similar pattern of results was observed for PTSD [linear-trend IRRadjusted = 3.43 (95% CI 3.21–3.66) and IRRadjusted = 1.45 (95% CI 1.39–1.50)]. PTSD was associated with increased risk of NAPD [IRRadjusted = 8.06 (95% CI 7.23–8.99)], which was substantially attenuated in 5-year time-lagged analyses [IRRadjusted = 4.62 (95% CI 3.65–5.87)]. There was little evidence that PTSD diagnosis mediated the relationship between interpersonal TE and NAPD [IRRadjusted = 0.92 (percentile CI 0.80–1.07)].

Conclusion

Despite the limitations to causal inference inherent in observational designs, the large effect-sizes observed between trauma, PTSD and NAPD in this study, consistent across sensitivity analyses, suggest that trauma may be a component cause of psychotic disorders. However, PTSD diagnosis might not be a good proxy for the likely complex psychological mechanisms mediating this association.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Psychotic disorders affect approximately 3% of the population at some point in their life (Perälä et al., Reference Perälä, Suvisaari, Saarni, Kuoppasalmi, Isometsä, Pirkola and Lönnqvist2007). This places a substantial burden on patients and healthcare systems. Reducing this morbid and societal burden is further impeded by the limited effectiveness of current treatments, with many sufferers having recurrent episodes, persistent symptoms and/or residual impairment (Owen, Sawa, & Mortensen, Reference Owen, Sawa and Mortensen2016).

Extensive evidence for a link between traumatic experiences (TE) and the onset and persistence of psychosis has accumulated over the last decade (Coughlan & Cannon, Reference Coughlan and Cannon2017). Trauma involving interpersonal violence, neglect and intent to harm, appear to be the most salient in the development of psychosis (Arseneault et al., Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi2011; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, McLaughlin, Saha, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso and Esan2017a, Reference McGrath, Saha, Lim, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Andrade and Cardoso2017b; Spauwen, Krabbendam, Lieb, Wittchen, & Van Os, Reference Spauwen, Krabbendam, Lieb, Wittchen and Van Os2018; van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, Lataster, Smeets, Gunther, van Zelst, de Graaf and van Winkel2014), with the evidence pointing towards the different types or forms of interpersonal trauma having comparable association effect sizes (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke, Thompson and Zammit2019). Non-interpersonal TE (e.g. loss of a loved one or serious accidental injury) are less studied, but may also be risk factors for psychosis (Abel et al., Reference Abel, Heuvelman, Jörgensen, Magnusson, Wicks, Susser and Dalman2014; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Lim, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Andrade and Cardoso2017b). Most of the accumulating evidence has focused on childhood trauma with the role of exposure to TE later in the life-course receiving less consideration (Beards et al., Reference Beards, Gayer-Anderson, Borges, Dewey, Fisher and Morgan2013; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Lim, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Andrade and Cardoso2017b). Moreover, exposure to childhood and adulthood trauma are strongly correlated (Stain et al., Reference Stain, Brønnick, Hegelstad, Joa, Johannessen, Langeveld and Larsen2013), with TE tending to co-occur or cluster within individuals (Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Williams2010; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, McLaughlin, Saha, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso and Esan2017a) and possibly exerting a cumulative effect, related to both the number and type of trauma (Arseneault et al., Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi2011; Croft et al., Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke, Thompson and Zammit2019; Janssen et al., Reference Janssen, Krabbendam, Bak, Hanssen, Vollebergh, de Graaf and van Os2004; Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Keeley, Corcoran, Ramsay, Wasserman, Carli and Cannon2013).

History of traumatic or stressful life events are diagnostic criteria for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and acute stress reaction (ASR), and while research has mainly focused on PTSD, both have a high rate of comorbidity with psychotic disorders (Buckley, Miller, Lehrer, & Castle, Reference Buckley, Miller, Lehrer and Castle2008). Recent prospective studies have further shown that having a diagnosis of PTSD or ASR is associated with both an increased risk of psychotic disorders (Okkels, Trabjerg, Arendt, & Pedersen, Reference Okkels, Trabjerg, Arendt and Pedersen2016) and psychotic experiences (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Arseneault, Caspi, Fisher, Matthews, Moffitt and Danese2019).

It has been hypothesised that PTSD mediates the association between trauma and psychosis severity and functional impairment, for at least a proportion of individuals (Mueser, Rosenberg, Goodman, & Trumbetta, Reference Mueser, Rosenberg, Goodman and Trumbetta2002; Williams, Bucci, Berry, & Varese, Reference Williams, Bucci, Berry and Varese2018). The development of such explanatory models are needed to extend our understanding and explain the interrelationship between trauma, PTSD and psychotic disorder in order to inform the development of prevention strategies and novel trauma-informed interventions for psychosis (Brand, McEnery, Rossell, Bendall, & Thomas, Reference Brand, McEnery, Rossell, Bendall and Thomas2018). Whilst there is some evidence to support this hypothesis, there are several areas where the evidence is weak or missing and we attempt to address these here.

First, although longitudinal studies show consistent evidence of an association between traumatic and psychotic experiences, there are few studies of psychotic disorders. While psychotic experiences in individuals with and without clinically diagnosed psychotic disorders are phenomenologically similar, there are large differences in their associated emotional valence, distress and functional impairment (Daalman et al., Reference Daalman, Diederen, Derks, van Lutterveld, Kahn and Sommer2012; Lawrie, Hall, McIntosh, Owens, & Johnstone, Reference Lawrie, Hall, McIntosh, Owens and Johnstone2010) and possibly their aetiology. For example, paranoid ideation is a common consequence of abuse, but these persecutory thoughts may have a different aetiology to the persecutory experiences and delusions which characterise psychotic disorders. Furthermore, psychotic experiences may be better conceptualised as markers of general psychopathology, rather than as specific risk factors for psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Caspi, Poulton, Meier, Houts, Harrington and Moffitt2013; Stochl et al., Reference Stochl, Khandaker, Lewis, Perez, Goodyer, Zammit and Jones2015).

While studies of psychotic experiences show an effect gradient and stronger associations with interpersonal than non-interpersonal trauma, there are currently no longitudinal studies which examine dose-response relationships or compare the effects of non-interpersonal and interpersonal TE on the incidence of psychotic disorders. Studies of psychotic disorder have almost all used cross-sectional data (Varese et al., Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer and Bentall2012), which hampers causal inference due to difficulties untangling causal effects from reverse causation and residual confounding in retrospective (post-outcome) assessment of exposures.

Second, although PTSD is common in people with a psychotic disorder (Seow et al., Reference Seow, Ong, Mahesh, Sagayadevan, Shafie, Chong and Subramaniam2016), only two studies have examined whether people with PTSD have an increased risk of subsequently developing a psychotic disorder, only one of which examined the extent to which PTSD symptoms mediate the association between trauma and psychotic disorder (although these were not clinician diagnoses) (Strelchuk et al., Reference Strelchuk, Hammerton, Wiles, Croft, Turner, Heron and Zammit2020). A few studies have examined whether PTSD symptoms mediate the relationship between trauma and psychotic experiences rather than psychotic disorder (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Bucci, Berry and Varese2018), but apart from one (Strelchuk et al., Reference Strelchuk, Hammerton, Wiles, Croft, Turner, Heron and Zammit2020), the rest have all have been cross-sectional in design and unable to determine direction of effect.

To date, no longitudinal studies have examined exposure to trauma, PTSD, and psychotic disorder in a unitary sample to address these limitations. Therefore, we use data from the Swedish national register to examine whether: (i) interpersonal and non-interpersonal traumatic events (TE) are associated with subsequent non-affective psychotic disorders (NAPD), (ii) PTSD is associated with subsequent NAPD, and (iii) the extent to which the association between TE and NAPD is mediated by PTSD.

Methods

Study cohort

The data used in this study was extracted from Psychiatry Sweden (PS) – a comprehensive linked national record register, which was developed specifically for mental health research. Record linkage brings together two or more records relating to the same individual so that information from multiple sources can be joined to produce richer data for research purposes in public health and epidemiological research. Record linkage matches a person' records via a unique identifier. Sweden has a strong track record for performing record linkage for research purposes owing to its routinely collected and well-maintained national administrative health data sets.

We identified all individuals born in Sweden between 1 January 1971 and 31 December 1990, who were resident in Sweden on their 16th birthday from the total population register (RTP) (N = 1 965 214). This cohort was then followed up from their 16th birthday until they either received a first-ever diagnosis of the outcome of interest, censorship due to death, emigration, or the study end-date (31 December 2016), whichever came first.

The Swedish National register diagnoses are well recognised as having good validity for psychiatric epidemiological studies (Ludvigsson et al., Reference Ludvigsson, Andersson, Ekbom, Feychting, Kim, Reuterwall and Olausson2011). As PTSD has high co-morbidity with other psychiatric disorders, further validation studies have been carried out which show its reliability and validity is adequate for large scale epidemiological studies in the Swedish population during the time period of these analyses (Hollander et al., Reference Hollander, Askegård, Iddon-Escalante, Holmes, Wicks and Dalman2019).

Assessment of NAPD and PTSD

The cohort was linked to Sweden' National Patient Registers which used International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Ninth Revision (World Health, 1978) for the period 1st January 1987–31st December 1996, and Tenth Revision (World Health, 2004) from 1 January 1997 onwards. Data for NAPD (coded as ICD-9: 295, 297, 298.2–298.9; ICD-10: F20–F29) is reliable, with complete inpatient coverage for the whole study period. Inpatient admissions are reliably coded for PTSD (ICD-10: F43.1) from 1 January 1997. Complete outpatient coverage for NAPD and PTSD were available from 1 January 2006. We also examined acute stress reaction (ASR: ICD-9: 308, ICD-10: F43.0) in a sensitivity analysis (see below).

Trauma exposure

Probable TE was identified using ‘reason for contact’ coding extracted from the national patient register. We derived two pre-specified categories of exposure for TE which are used extensively in trauma research: non-interpersonal and interpersonal TE (Aas et al., Reference Aas, Dazzan, Mondelli, Melle, Murray and Pariante2013). Non-interpersonal TE was defined as the presence of either (i) death of a first degree relative (mother, father, full sibling, child) or of a partner/spouse, or (ii) injury due to accidents without violence (for further details and ICD codes, see online Supplementary S Notes 1: Definition using ICD codes for Traumatic Experiences). For the former, relatives were identified from the Multigenerational Register and linked to the Cause of death Register to extract the date of deaths of first-degree relatives. Partners were defined as husbands, wives, common law spouses, registered partners, or another adult living in the same household as the proband, ascertained from the quinquennial Population and Housing Censuses of 1985 and 1990 and from the annual LISA (Longitudinal Integration Database for Health Insurance and Labour Market Studies) returns thereafter. Records from any identified partner were linked to the Cause of Death Register with their death included as an exposure if it occurred during the period of shared residency. Interpersonal TE were defined as injury due to war, terrorism, iatrogenic/medical misadventure, and violence/assaults (further details and ICD codes: online Supplementary S Notes 1: Definition using ICD codes for TE).

Covariates

Age (5-year age-bands), calendar-period (5-year bands) and sex were obtained from the RTP. Paternal age at off-spring' conception was dichotomised at > age 39, guided by recent meta-analyses supporting a cut off between 30 and 39 years of age (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Messias, Miettunen, Alaräisänen, Järvelin, Koponen and Kirkpatrick2011). Second-generation migrant status was extracted from Multi-Generational Registers linked to RTP/STATIV. Compulsory school best educational attainment was extracted from LISA, and parents' socioeconomic status at participant' birth from quinquennial censuses (1970–1990). Parental lifetime history of severe mental illness (SMI) was indexed in three separate binary variables as a proxy for familial liability to: (i) NAPD (as above), (ii) mania (296.1–296.3, F30–31) and (iii) depression (296.2, 300.4, F32–39).

Statistical analyses

Data were analysed using STATA 15 (StataCorp, 2017). Piecewise exponentiated survival models were fitted within the generalised linear modelling framework using log-linear Poisson Regression (Royston, Reference Royston and Lambert2011). Age, calendar-period, non-interpersonal and interpersonal TE were all considered as time-dependent variables (Clayton & Hills, Reference Clayton and Hills2013). The numbers of interpersonal and non-interpersonal TE before the age of 16 were entered at baseline. All TEs after age 16 were handled as time-varying covariates entering the model at the time they were first coded in the health service records. Using episode splitting the person-time exposure load was partitioned more precisely for each level (count) of exposure (for more information see online Supplementary S Note 2: Methods: exponentiated survival models and time-dependent variables). Sex, paternal age, compulsory school educational attainment, second-generation migrant status, socioeconomic status, and parental lifetime history of SMI were all entered as time-fixed covariates.

We examined the effect of interpersonal and non-interpersonal TE as they accrued, until failure (time when a person either experienced the outcome of interest or was censored), on the first-ever diagnosis of NAPD and PTSD, estimating incidence rate ratios (IRRs) in separate time to first-event analyses. The initial forced models included age, sex (and their interaction) and calendar period. We extended this model controlling for each confounder in turn and finally included all the covariates, with the fully adjusted model including mutual adjustment for the alternative type of traumatic event. A complementary set of analyses using the same statistical approach was used to examine the effect of PTSD on risk of NAPD. TE were modelled both as indicator variables to estimate category-specific estimates, and as a continuous exposure to estimate a linear-trend estimate across categories.

To evaluate the robustness of our findings we conducted a series of sensitivity analyses: (1) To assess possible reverse causation due to undetected prodromal psychosis which may increase exposure to TE (Carmen, Rieker, & Mills, Reference Carmen, Rieker and Mills1984), we imposed induction lengths of 1–5 years between date of TE and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD and estimated lagged time-dependent IRRs; (2) To examine whether any observed association between TE and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD was driven by factors associated with childhood trauma, we restricted analyses to TE occurring after age 16, and subsequently adjusted for recorded childhood trauma; (3) To address possible misclassification of prodromal psychosis as PTSD, we performed a series of analyses imposing induction lengths of 1–5 years between first-ever diagnosis of PTSD and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD; (4) To evaluate whether the association of PTSD with NAPD was stronger (more specific) than for other stress-related disorders, we repeated these analyses using ASR instead of PTSD.

Finally, mediation analysis within a counterfactual framework (Robins & Greenland, Reference Robins and Greenland1992) was performed using the inverse odds weighting (IOW) method (Hossin, Koupil, & Falkstedt, Reference Hossin, Koupil and Falkstedt2019; Nguyen, Osypuk, Schmidt, Glymour, & Tchetgen Tchetgen, Reference Nguyen, Osypuk, Schmidt, Glymour and Tchetgen Tchetgen2015; Tchetgen Tchetgen, Reference Tchetgen Tchetgen2013; VanderWeele & Vansteelandt, Reference VanderWeele and Vansteelandt2014) to estimate natural direct and indirect effects (Pearl, Reference Pearl2000) when PTSD is included on the pathway from interpersonal TE to first-ever NAPD. We chose, a priori, to examine interpersonal TE in the mediation model as they have been consistently shown to be more strongly associated with both PTSD (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso and Koenen2017) and NAPD (Arseneault et al., Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi2011). Interpersonal TE was dichotomised to optimise the effectiveness of this weighted approach (Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Osypuk, Schmidt, Glymour and Tchetgen Tchetgen2015; VanderWeele & Vansteelandt, Reference VanderWeele and Vansteelandt2014) and entered as a time-dependent variable at the time of the first trauma-related code in the health register. Standard errors were bootstrapped (500 replications) and percentile-based confidence intervals (CIs) derived. Models were adjusted for the full battery of confounders described above.

Results

Cohort characteristics

1 873 593 individuals with no missing data (>95% original cohort) were included in the analyses (baseline characteristics reported in online Supplementary Table S1: Cohort characteristics by non-affective psychosis status). From 1987–2016 (36 290 751 person-years at-risk) 17 653 individuals (0.94%) had a first-ever diagnosis of NAPD and 11 225 (0.6%) had a first-ever diagnosis of PTSD. Non-interpersonal TE was commoner than interpersonal TE, with 299 569 persons (16%) exposed to 1 or more non-interpersonal TE across the follow-up period compared to 14 528 (0.78%) exposed to interpersonal TE, consistent with other register-based studies (Webb et al., Reference Webb, Antonsen, Carr, Appleby, Pedersen and Mok2017).

Relative risk for NAPD by background TE exposure

Non-interpersonal and interpersonal TE were independently associated with risk of NAPD, which persisted after adjustment for confounders (Table 1, section A, Fig. 1). IRRs for NAPD stratified by interpersonal TE exposure were substantially larger than for non-interpersonal TE, with non-overlapping CIs. There was a monotonic increase in IRRs with increasing number of interpersonal TE.

Fig. 1. Coefficient plot: IRR for psychiatric outcomes according to number of interpersonal TE across life-course. IRRs are estimated adjusted for age, sex (and their interaction), calendar period, paternal lifetime history of NAPD, depressive disorders and bipolar-disorder, socioeconomic status, paternal, age at conception, compulsory school education attainment, second-generation migrant status for age, sex, period, paternal age, second-generation migrant status, paternal history of depressive disorder, NAPD and BD, socioeconomic status and best school educational attainment, NAPD, non affective psychotic disorders, ASR acute stress reaction, PTSD post-traumatic stress disorder.

Table 1. Association between TE and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD and PTSD

a Full Model: adjusted for age, sex (and their interaction), calendar period, paternal lifetime history of NAPD, depressive disorders and bipolar disorder, socioeconomic status, paternal. age at conception, compulsory school education attainment, second-generation migrant status. This model is mutually adjusted for interpersonal and non-interpersonal TEs.

Relative risk for PTSD by background TE exposure

Interpersonal and non-interpersonal TE were independently associated with increased risk for PTSD (Table 1, section B). Risk was substantially higher for interpersonal than non-interpersonal TE after mutual adjustment and after controlling for confounding.

PTSD and risk of NAPD

PTSD was associated with an approximately 15-fold increased risk for NAPD (Table 2). The effect-estimate was attenuated after adjusting for covariates, although there remained strong evidence of increased risk of NAPD after adjustment for all measured confounding variables [lRR = 8.06 (95% CI 7.23–8.99)].

Table 2. Associations between PTSD/ASR and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD

Full model: adjusted for age, sex (and their interaction), calendar period, paternal lifetime history of NAPD, depressive disorders and bipolar-disorder, socioeconomic status, paternal. age at conception, compulsory school education attainment, second-generation migrant status.

Mediation analyses

There was little evidence that PTSD mediated the association between interpersonal TE and NAPD (natural indirect (mediation) effect: IRR = 0.92; percentile CI 0.80–1.07; Table 3).

Table 3. Mediation of the association between interpersonal TE and NAPD by PTSD

a Adjusted for age, sex (and their interaction), calendar period, paternal lifetime history of NAPD, depressive disorders and bipolar-disorder, socioeconomic status, paternal. age at conception, compulsory school education attainment, second-generation migrant status.

b Obtained by applying the inverse odds weights with full complement of adjustments described.

Sensitivity analyses

(1) Imposing induction lengths of 1–5 years between TE exposure and first ever NAPD to assess the role of reverse causation, IRRs remained substantively unchanged, with only a relatively small attenuation in 5-year lagged time-dependent IRRs for both non-interpersonal and interpersonal traumas (online Supplementary Table S2). (2) The association between both interpersonal and non-interpersonal TE and NAPD persisted when trauma exposure was restricted to traumas experienced after age 16, and after adjusting for childhood TE (online Supplementary Table S3). (3) 5-year lagged time-dependent IRRs for NAPD after a diagnosis of PTSD (Table 2) were attenuated by approximately 50%, although remained substantial (lagged IRR5yr 4.62, 95% CI 3.65–5.87). (4) ASR associations with both TE and NAPD were weaker than for PTSD, though results were substantively similar (online Supplementary Table S4).

Discussion

Trauma and NAPD

Using data from a Swedish nationwide birth cohort we show clear temporal relationships between exposure to trauma and subsequent risk of NAPD. Our analyses account for both type and number of TE, in a time-to-event (survival) framework. We adjust for the alternative trauma-type to try and disentangle non-interpersonal and interpersonal trauma effects, which tend to co-occur (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Williams2018), and find that non-interpersonal and interpersonal traumas are independently associated with increased NAPD risk. The specific effect for interpersonal trauma is substantially stronger however, and shows a clearer effect gradient, consistent with published reports of higher rates of psychotic experiences in adolescents exposed to trauma involving intention to harm, threat and hostility (Arseneault et al., Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi2011). While non-interpersonal trauma is less studied, the small/modest independent increase in risk of NAPD observed is consistent with other (Abel et al., Reference Abel, Heuvelman, Jörgensen, Magnusson, Wicks, Susser and Dalman2014; Arseneault et al., Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi2011), although not all (Moriyama et al., Reference Moriyama, Drukker, Gadelha, Pan, Salum, Manfro and Polanczyk2018) studies.

Evidence of association between trauma and risk of NAPD remained strong after adjustment for a range of measured confounders, but estimates were attenuated, particularly for interpersonal trauma, suggesting these association are more prone to residual confounding. Individuals who develop NAPD have (on average) higher rates of subtle cognitive and neurodevelopmental abnormalities which may share genetic liability with NAPD and influence exposure to trauma. While parental history of psychiatric disorders will not capture all relevant shared genetic risk, our estimates for interpersonal trauma remained large and almost unchanged after adjustment for family history, making it less likely that observed associations are substantially attributable to genetic confounding. Systematic reviews of family and twin studies suggest genetic effects have moderate influences on environmental exposures (Kendler & Baker, Reference Kendler and Baker2007), though adjusting for schizophrenia-related polygenic risk did not attenuate associations between trauma and psychotic experiences in another cohort study (Croft et al., Reference Croft, Heron, Teufel, Cannon, Wolke, Thompson and Zammit2019).

Our results were substantively unchanged when we examined trauma exposure in adulthood and adjusted for childhood trauma, consistent with the allostatic load hypothesis which suggests it is trauma accumulation across the life-course which increases psychosis risk.

Trauma, PTSD and NAPD

Whilst we replicate findings from a Danish study showing that traumatic stress disorders are associated with increased risk of subsequent NAPD (Okkels et al., Reference Okkels, Trabjerg, Arendt and Pedersen2016), we extend this by showing a stronger effect for PTSD than ASR and by examining the extent to which PTSD mediates the association between trauma and psychosis risk. Despite strong associations between interpersonal trauma and both PTSD and NAPD, and between PTSD and subsequent NAPD, we found little support for the hypothesis that PTSD mediates the relationship between trauma and psychosis. However, people with PTSD do not always present to secondary care (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Arseneault, Caspi, Fisher, Matthews, Moffitt and Danese2019) and where they do they are likely to receive treatment for their PTSD symptoms; therefore our results may under-estimate the mediation effect of PTSD symptoms.

This study provides novel information that can help inform our understanding of how TE can lead to the development of psychosis. The absence of evidence for PTSD mediation in our study suggests that the conceptualisation of trauma leading to PTSD symptoms which, over time, develop into psychosis might need reframing, although are findings are not necessarily inconsistent with the hypothesis that traumatic pathology has a causal role in the development of psychosis. The potentially causal relationship between trauma and psychotic disorders are likely to be complex and a measure of PTSD diagnosis may not be the best proxy for the multiple pathways and psychological mechanisms underpinning this association. Examination of potentially specific trauma-symptom associations (Hardy, Reference Hardy2017) or of specific types of TE such as sexual abuse which we have not measured in this study, might provide a more sophisticated understanding of this relationship.

An alternative hypothesis suggests there is a spectrum of multifaceted, dimensional psychopathological responses to trauma, ranging in severity and need for care, and it is the balance between the timing, severity, and repetitiveness of trauma exposure within the context of an individual' specific liability (genetic, psychological and social) which leads to differential psychopathological expressions of response to trauma (Morrison, Frame, & Larkin, Reference Morrison, Frame and Larkin2003). That is, while ASR, PTSD, and NAPD diagnostic categories are clinically useful, they may well be artificial constructs lying along trauma-related phenomenological continua rather than representing discrete/separate psychopathological processes.

Strengths and limitations

This study used data from a large national cohort, representative of the Swedish population and therefore is less likely to be affected by selection bias. TE were recorded prospectively and independently of outcome using contemporaneous register data, hence eliminating recall bias. Furthermore, we used a Poisson framework in our analyses so that time could be considered a covariate rather than an outcome, to adjust for potential systematic variation in estimated rates due to secular changes in diagnostic approaches and other period effects. We also conducted a series of sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our findings. For example, although the average psychosis prodrome is approximately 2 years (Loebel et al., Reference Loebel, Lieberman, Alvir, Mayerhoff, Geisler and Szymanski1992), we observe a relatively small attenuation of estimates in the 5-year lagged models, making it unlikely reverse causation explains the associations we observe.

However, our results should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. First, although we adjust for a range of administratively available covariates in the analyses, some residual confounding is probable, and a well-recognised issue in all register-based studies (Mortensen, Allebeck, & Munk-Jørgensen, Reference Mortensen, Allebeck and Munk-Jørgensen1996). There might also be residual confounding for some of the measures we adjust for, such as socio-economic status and educational attainment as these are relatively lacking in detail. Given that both are more strongly associated with inter-personal than non-interpersonal trauma (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Kessler, Chilcoat, Schultz, Davis and Andreski1998), there might be more scope for residual confounding affecting estimates for inter-personal trauma in our study.

Second, as we use administrative data originally collected for health service purposes (such as providing valid health data and statistical information to support health service development) to measure trauma, our estimates reflect effects of trauma requiring hospital contact. This approach will have underestimated exposure to all the traumas that would meet criteria for trauma as defined by DSM or ICD diagnostic manuals, missing events such as childhood abuse or neglect, and witnessing or learning of TE as they occurred in others, as well as other traumas that would not have led to a hospital contact.

Furthermore, interpersonal TE may be misclassified as accidents where intent is undetermined/unknown (Ballard, Kalb, Vasa, Goldstein, & Wilcox, Reference Ballard, Kalb, Vasa, Goldstein and Wilcox2015), and whilst we coded medical misadventures as interpersonal traumas it could equally be argued that these are non-interpersonal, though the number of individuals coded as such was small. Therefore, while the hospital contact codes we have used in this study are likely to indicate a level of trauma that is clinically relevant, our estimates might not extrapolate well to all traumas, and be affected by misclassification bias. If non-differential, this would likely lead to an under-estimate of effects. However, if propensity for hospital contact following a trauma differed between those who eventually developed PTSD or NAPD compared to those who did not, this could have led to either over- or under-estimation of effects.

Similar issues apply to our outcome measures as we were unable to identify individuals in the population with symptoms of PTSD or NAPD who either received no treatment or only received treatment in primary or private care. However, using clinically determined diagnoses makes these results readily generalisable to clinical settings, even if not to the whole population. We were unable to examine complex PTSD that is perhaps a more pertinent mediator for the effects of trauma on psychosis as this diagnosis has only been introduced in ICD-11 (Cloitre, Garvert, Brewin, Bryant, & Maercker, Reference Cloitre, Garvert, Brewin, Bryant and Maercker2013).

Third, PTSD and NAPD have high levels of comorbidity (Achim et al., Reference Achim, Maziade, Raymond, Olivier, Mérette and Roy2009) and their psychopathology substantially overlap (Alsawy, Wood, Taylor, & Morrison, Reference Alsawy, Wood, Taylor and Morrison2015; Scott, Nurcombe, Sheridan, & McFarland, Reference Scott, Nurcombe, Sheridan and McFarland2007). For example, hallucinations in NAPD are analogous to flashbacks in PTSD, suspicion resembles hypervigilance and negative symptoms overlap with the numbing and avoidance seen in PTSD, which may make it difficult to differentiate PTSD and NAPD phenomenologically (Jessop, Scott, & Nurcombe, Reference Jessop, Scott and Nurcombe2008). If this diagnostic uncertainty or comorbidity is coded or handled differently across individual clinicians and hospital sites, it may reduce inter-diagnostic discrimination and reliability and result in misclassification of PTSD, which is already recognised to be under-detected in secondary care settings (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Lewis, Dawson, Colley, McCann, Piekarski and Bisson2018). Fourth, our analytical approach required all cohort members to have been born in Sweden and survive to 16 years, with trauma exposure in childhood entered at baseline to prevent immortal time bias (time-dependent bias); however, this method precludes the examination of critical or sensitive periods of trauma exposure during childhood. Finally, we used dichotomised measures in the mediation analysis to optimise the IOW approach (Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Osypuk, Schmidt, Glymour and Tchetgen Tchetgen2015; VanderWeele & Vansteelandt, Reference VanderWeele and Vansteelandt2014) but this will have resulted in information loss regarding exposure dosage, and similarly to the absence of information on the provision or success of any treatment provided for PTSD, could potentially have led to underestimation of the mediation effect in our study.

Implications

The large effect sizes observed when examining the associations between TE, PTSD and NAPD in this study, and our study design and sensitivity analyses to minimise effects of confounding, reverse causation and bias (though see limitations above) is consistent with trauma being a component cause in the onset of psychotic disorders. However, our mediation results suggest that a diagnostic category of PTSD is unlikely to substantially mediate this relationship. It is unclear therefore whether PTSD symptoms lie on the causal pathway to psychosis, or whether PTSD and psychosis lie on a spectrum of trauma-related psychopathological expression. A clearer conceptualisation of how psychosis can develop following trauma is likely to provide novel opportunities for improving treatment outcomes in psychotic disorders (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Rodriguez, Carr, Aas, Trotta, Marino and Murray2020; Compean & Hamner, Reference Compean and Hamner2019).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003287

Financial support

This work was supported by (ACH& RS, Forskningsradet om Hälsa, Arbetsliv och Välfärd (FORTE) 2017-00632), (CD, Swedish Research Council Formas and (JA, Wellcome Trust fellowship 209176/Z/17/Z). SZ is supported by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at University Hospitals Bristol and Weston NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Bristol.

Conflict of interest

No known conflict of interest applies to all authors.

References

Aas, M., Dazzan, P., Mondelli, V., Melle, I., Murray, R. M., & Pariante, C. M. (2013). A systematic review of cognitive function in first-episode psychosis, including a discussion on childhood trauma, stress, and inflammation. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 182.Google Scholar
Abel, K., Heuvelman, H., Jörgensen, L., Magnusson, C., Wicks, S., Susser, E., … Dalman, C. (2014). Severe bereavement stress during the prenatal and childhood periods and risk of psychosis in later life: Population based cohort study. BMJ, 348, f7679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achim, A. M., Maziade, M., Raymond, É, Olivier, D., Mérette, C., & Roy, M.-A. (2009). How prevalent are anxiety disorders in schizophrenia? A meta-analysis and critical review on a significant association. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(4), 811821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alameda, L., Rodriguez, V., Carr, E., Aas, M., Trotta, G., Marino, P., … Murray, R. M. (2020). A systematic review on mediators between adversity and psychosis: Potential targets for treatment. Psychological Medicine, 50(12), 19661976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720002421CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alsawy, S., Wood, L., Taylor, P. J., & Morrison, A. P. (2015). Psychotic experiences and PTSD: Exploring associations in a population survey. Psychological Medicine, 45(13), 28492859. doi: 10.1017/S003329171500080XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Fisher, H. L., Polanczyk, G., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2011). Childhood trauma and children's emerging psychotic symptoms: A genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(1), 6572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, E. D., Kalb, L., Vasa, R. A., Goldstein, M., & Wilcox, H. C. (2015). Self-harm, assault and undetermined intent injuries among pediatric emergency department visits. Pediatric Emergency Care, 31(12), 813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beards, S., Gayer-Anderson, C., Borges, S., Dewey, M. E., Fisher, H. L., & Morgan, C. (2013). Life events and psychosis: A review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(4), 740747. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brand, R. M., McEnery, C., Rossell, S., Bendall, S., & Thomas, N. (2018). Do trauma-focussed psychological interventions have an effect on psychotic symptoms? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 195, 1322. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., Kessler, R. C., Chilcoat, H. D., Schultz, L. R., Davis, G. C., & Andreski, P. (1998). Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: The 1996 detroit area survey of trauma. JAMA Psychiatry, 55(7), 626632. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.7.626Google ScholarPubMed
Buckley, P. F., Miller, B. J., Lehrer, D. S., & Castle, D. J. (2008). Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(2), 383402. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carmen, E. H., Rieker, P. P., & Mills, T. (1984). Victims of violence and psychiatric illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141(3), 378383. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.3.378Google Scholar
Clayton, D., & Hills, M. (2013). Statistical models in epidemiology. New York: OUP Oxford.Google Scholar
Cloitre, M., Garvert, D. W., Brewin, C. R., Bryant, R. A., & Maercker, A. (2013). Evidence for proposed ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD: A latent profile analysis. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 4, 112. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.20706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compean, E., & Hamner, M. (2019). Posttraumatic stress disorder with secondary psychotic features (PTSD-SP): Diagnostic and treatment challenges. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 88, 265275. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coughlan, H., & Cannon, M. (2017). Does childhood trauma play a role in the aetiology of psychosis? A review of recent evidence. BJPsych Advances, 23(5), 307315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, J., Heron, J., Teufel, C., Cannon, M., Wolke, D., Thompson, A., … Zammit, S. (2019). Association of trauma type, age of exposure, and frequency in childhood and adolescence with psychotic experiences in early AdulthoodAssociation of trauma in childhood and adolescence with psychotic experiences in early AdulthoodAssociation of trauma in childhood and adolescence with psychotic experiences in early adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(1), 7986. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daalman, K., Diederen, K., Derks, E. M., van Lutterveld, R., Kahn, R. S., & Sommer, I. E. (2012). Childhood trauma and auditory verbal hallucinations. Psychological Medicine, 42(12), 24752484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, H. L., Caspi, A., Poulton, R., Meier, M. H., Houts, R., Harrington, H., … Moffitt, T. E. (2013). Specificity of childhood psychotic symptoms for predicting schizophrenia by 38 years of age: A birth cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 43(10), 20772086. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712003091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. G., McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P. A., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 113123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, A. (2017). Pathways from trauma to psychotic experiences: A theoretically informed model of posttraumatic stress in psychosis. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 697697. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollander, A.-C., Askegård, K., Iddon-Escalante, C., Holmes, E. A., Wicks, S., & Dalman, C. (2019). Validation study of randomly selected cases of PTSD diagnoses identified in a Swedish regional database compared with medical records: Is the validity sufficient for epidemiological research? BMJ Open, 9(12), e031964. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-031964CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hossin, M. Z., Koupil, I., & Falkstedt, D. (2019). Early life socioeconomic position and mortality from cardiovascular diseases: An application of causal mediation analysis in the Stockholm public health cohort. BMJ Open, 9(6), e026258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janssen, I., Krabbendam, L., Bak, M., Hanssen, M., Vollebergh, W., de Graaf, R., & van Os, J. (2004). Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109(1), 3845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jessop, M., Scott, J., & Nurcombe, B. (2008). Hallucinations in adolescent inpatients with post-traumatic stress disorder and schizophrenia: Similarities and differences. Australasian Psychiatry, 16(4), 268272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelleher, I., Keeley, H., Corcoran, P., Ramsay, H., Wasserman, C., Carli, V., … Cannon, M. (2013). Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: Cause, effect, and directionality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(7), 734741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Baker, J. H. (2007). Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 37(5), 615626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., Cardoso, G., … Koenen, K. C. (2017). Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 8(sup5), 1353383. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., … Williams, D. R. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(5), 378385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., … Williams, D. R. (2018). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO world mental health Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(5), 378385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrie, S. M., Hall, J., McIntosh, A. M., Owens, D. G., & Johnstone, E. C. (2010). The ‘continuum of psychosis’: Scientifically unproven and clinically impractical. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(6), 423425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, S. J., Arseneault, L., Caspi, A., Fisher, H. L., Matthews, T., Moffitt, T. E., … Danese, A. (2019). The epidemiology of trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder in a representative cohort of young people in England and Wales. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(3), 247256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loebel, A. D., Lieberman, J. A., Alvir, J. M., Mayerhoff, D. I., Geisler, S. H., & Szymanski, S. R. (1992). Duration of psychosis and outcome in first-episode schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 11831188.Google ScholarPubMed
Ludvigsson, J. F., Andersson, E., Ekbom, A., Feychting, M., Kim, J.-L., Reuterwall, C., … Olausson, P. O. (2011). External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 116. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-450CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J. J., McLaughlin, K., Saha, S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., … Esan, O. (2017a). The association between childhood adversities and subsequent first onset of psychotic experiences: A cross-national analysis of 23 998 respondents from 17 countries. Psychological Medicine, 47(7), 12301245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Lim, C. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Andrade, L. H., … Cardoso, G. (2017b). Trauma and psychotic experiences: Transnational data from the world mental health survey. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 211(6), 373380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, B., Messias, E., Miettunen, J., Alaräisänen, A., Järvelin, M.-R., Koponen, H., … Kirkpatrick, B. (2011). Meta-analysis of paternal age and schizophrenia risk in male versus female offspring. Schizophrenia bulletin, 37(5), 10391047. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriyama, T. S., Drukker, M., Gadelha, A., Pan, P. M., Salum, G. A., Manfro, G. G., … Polanczyk, G. V. (2018). The association between psychotic experiences and traumatic life events: The role of the intention to harm. Psychological Medicine, 48(13), 22352246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, A. P., Frame, L., & Larkin, W. (2003). Relationships between trauma and psychosis: A review and integration. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42(4), 331353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortensen, P. B., Allebeck, P., & Munk-Jørgensen, P. (1996). Population-based registers in psychiatric research. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 50(sup36), 6772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueser, K. T., Rosenberg, S. D., Goodman, L. A., & Trumbetta, S. L. (2002). Trauma, PTSD, and the course of severe mental illness: An interactive model. Schizophrenia Research, 53(1–2), 123143. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00173-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, Q. C., Osypuk, T. L., Schmidt, N. M., Glymour, M. M., & Tchetgen Tchetgen, E. J. (2015). Practical guidance for conducting mediation analysis with multiple mediators using inverse odds ratio weighting. American Journal of Epidemiology, 181(5), 349356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okkels, N., Trabjerg, B., Arendt, M., & Pedersen, C. B. (2016). Traumatic stress disorders and risk of subsequent schizophrenia Spectrum disorder or bipolar disorder: A nationwide cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(1), 180186. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, M. J., Sawa, A., & Mortensen, P. B. (2016). Schizophrenia. The Lancet, 388(10039), 8697. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01121-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearl, J. (2000). Causality: Models, reasoning and inference (Vol. 29). Cambridge: Springer.Google Scholar
Perälä, J., Suvisaari, J., Saarni, S. I., Kuoppasalmi, K., Isometsä, E., Pirkola, S., … Lönnqvist, J. (2007). Lifetime prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disorders in a general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(1), 1928. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robins, J. M., & Greenland, S. (1992). Identifiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 1, 143155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royston, P. L. , Lambert, P.L. (2011). Poisson Models. In Flexible parametric survival analysis using STATA: Beyond the Cox model (pp. 4757). Lakeside Drive, College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Scott, J. G., Nurcombe, B., Sheridan, J., & McFarland, M. (2007). Hallucinations in adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder and psychotic disorder. Australasian Psychiatry, 15(1), 4448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seow, L. S. E., Ong, C., Mahesh, M. V., Sagayadevan, V., Shafie, S., Chong, S. A., & Subramaniam, M. (2016). A systematic review on comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 176(2), 441451. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.05.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spauwen, J., Krabbendam, L., Lieb, R., Wittchen, H.-U., & Van Os, J. (2018). Impact of psychological trauma on the development of psychotic symptoms: Relationship with psychosis proneness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 188(6), 527533. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.011346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stain, H. J., Brønnick, K., Hegelstad, W. T., Joa, I., Johannessen, J. O., Langeveld, J., … Larsen, T. K. (2013). Impact of interpersonal trauma on the social functioning of adults with first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(6), 14911498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StataCorp (2017). Stata statistical software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.Google Scholar
Stochl, J., Khandaker, G. M., Lewis, G., Perez, J., Goodyer, I. M., Zammit, S., … Jones, P. B. (2015). Mood, anxiety and psychotic phenomena measure a common psychopathological factor. Psychological Medicine, 45(7), 14831493. doi: 10.1017/s003329171400261xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strelchuk, D., Hammerton, G., Wiles, N., Croft, J., Turner, K., Heron, J., & Zammit, S. (2020). PTSD as a mediator of the relationship between trauma and psychotic experiences. Psychological Medicine, 19. doi: 10.1017/s0033291720004821Google ScholarPubMed
Tchetgen Tchetgen, E. J. (2013). Inverse odds ratio-weighted estimation for causal mediation analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 32(26), 45674580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanderWeele, T., & Vansteelandt, S. (2014). Mediation analysis with multiple mediators. Epidemiologic Methods, 2(1), 95115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Nierop, M., Lataster, T., Smeets, F., Gunther, N., van Zelst, C., de Graaf, R., … van Winkel, R. (2014). Psychopathological mechanisms linking childhood traumatic experiences to risk of psychotic symptoms: Analysis of a large, representative population-based sample. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(Suppl_2), S123S130. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varese, F., Smeets, F., Drukker, M., Lieverse, R., Lataster, T., Viechtbauer, W., … Bentall, R. P. (2012). Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: A meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 661671. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, R. T., Antonsen, S., Carr, M. J., Appleby, L., Pedersen, C. B., & Mok, P. L. H. (2017). Self-harm and violent criminality among young people who experienced trauma-related hospital admission during childhood: A Danish national cohort study. Lancet Public Health, 2(7), e314e322. doi: 10.1016/s2468-2667(17)30094-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J., Bucci, S., Berry, K., & Varese, F. (2018). Psychological mediators of the association between childhood adversities and psychosis: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 175196. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.05.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health, O (1978). International classification of diseases : [9th] ninth revision, basic tabulation list with alphabetic index. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
World Health, O. (2004). ICD-10 : International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems : Tenth revision (2nd ed.). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Zammit, S., Lewis, C., Dawson, S., Colley, H., McCann, H., Piekarski, A., … Bisson, J. (2018). Undetected post-traumatic stress disorder in secondary-care mental health services: Systematic review. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 212(1), 1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Coefficient plot: IRR for psychiatric outcomes according to number of interpersonal TE across life-course. IRRs are estimated adjusted for age, sex (and their interaction), calendar period, paternal lifetime history of NAPD, depressive disorders and bipolar-disorder, socioeconomic status, paternal, age at conception, compulsory school education attainment, second-generation migrant status for age, sex, period, paternal age, second-generation migrant status, paternal history of depressive disorder, NAPD and BD, socioeconomic status and best school educational attainment, NAPD, non affective psychotic disorders, ASR acute stress reaction, PTSD post-traumatic stress disorder.

Figure 1

Table 1. Association between TE and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD and PTSD

Figure 2

Table 2. Associations between PTSD/ASR and first-ever diagnosis of NAPD

Figure 3

Table 3. Mediation of the association between interpersonal TE and NAPD by PTSD

Supplementary material: File

Allardyce et al. supplementary material

Allardyce et al. supplementary material

Download Allardyce et al. supplementary material(File)
File 45.6 KB