For once in a book review, I will cut to the chase. I am really pleased I have read this book cover to cover and, although I do not work on speciation in birds, this is already an appreciated, re-read and loaned-out book on my shelf. Why? It provides both breadth and depth on speciation in birds, offering sufficient introductory material to grasp the current state of affairs as well as going on to propound the problems and unanswered questions in speciation, at least as they pertain to birds. In my view, it is an excellent text for graduate students, the concepts are clearly outlined with plenty of examples, supported with colour illustrations, and there is lots of material for discussion and to inspire further reading. If you, or a student, haven't thought much about speciation before, then this book should reward you dipping your toes in the water. If you do study speciation, here is an excellent addition to the recent books by GavriletsReference Gavrilets1 and Coyne and OrrReference Coyne and Orr2.
Just before you reach for your credit card, you may expect some qualification of my glowing endorsement. And there is a little. You need to know already what the big questions in speciation more generally are before beginning (e.g. What is a species? Does sympatric speciation occur?). In addition, the relative contribution birds have made to our understanding of speciation comes from the wealth of evidence provided rather than from explicit comparisons with other taxa. For example, it is not clear whether birds are useful models of speciation through sheer research effort, or because birds offer a broader range of speciation puzzles than, say, Drosophila.
Price makes it clear that to address questions of speciation in birds a greater emphasis on the contributions of behaviour is needed than there would be for many other taxa except, perhaps, Anolis lizards or Lake Victoria cichlids. The critical role of behaviour in avian speciation is highlighted by chapters on ‘Behavior and Ecology’ (addressing issues such as dispersal propensity and the likelihood of survival in novel environments, and contributions to the latter of larger brains and feeding innovations), four chapters on ‘Social Selection’, a chapter on ‘Species Recognition’ in which the possible influences of filial and sexual imprinting on assortative mating are addressed, and one on ‘Mate Choice’. Birds, therefore, clearly offer some of the best opportunities to examine the role that behaviour, and especially learning, play in pre-mating isolation mechanisms although Price also stresses that postmating isolation is still essential for species coexistence. On the other hand, the elegant laboratory studies investigating the genetics underlying speciation do not exist for birds as they are not the right group with which to ask this kind of question. Beyond this, Price also considers higher-level patterns. For example, although the birds provide some of the best examples of adaptive radiation (the Galapagos finches of course), the truth is flight has often meant that single lineages do not often radiate to fill new niches in each habitat, but rather multiple lineages colonise and evolve.
As you can see, my qualifications are, in fact, only apparent, rather than critical. This book has already led me into more thoughtful conversations about speciation with my colleagues than I have ever had, as well as stimulating thinking on under-explored connections between my own field (cognition) and another (speciation). How often can we say that about the books on our shelves?