Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T01:56:14.408Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring the relationship between auditory hallucinations, trauma and dissociation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2020

Deborah Wearne*
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Guy J. Curtis
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Peter Melvill-Smith
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Kenneth G. Orr
Affiliation:
Marian Center, Perth, Australia
Annette Mackereth
Affiliation:
Marian Center, Perth, Australia
Leon Rajanthiran
Affiliation:
Marian Center, Perth, Australia
Sean Hood
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Winston Choy
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Flavie Waters
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Western Australia, Perth; and Clinical Research Centre, Graylands Campus, North Metropolitan Health Service, Mental Health, Perth, Australia.
*
Correspondence: Deborah Wearne. Email: dw.maiahouse@optusnet.com.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

It is clinically imperative to better understand the relationship between trauma, auditory hallucinations and dissociation. The personal narrative of trauma has enormous significance for each individual and is also important for the clinician, who must use this information to decide on a diagnosis and treatment approach.

Aims

To better understand whether dissociation contributes in a significant way to hallucinations in individuals with and without trauma histories.

Method

Three groups of participants with auditory hallucinations were recruited, with diagnoses of: schizophrenia (without trauma) (n = 18), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD, n = 27) and comorbid schizophrenia and PTSD (SCZ+PTSD), n = 26). Clinician-administered measures included the PTSD Symptoms Scale Interview (PSSI-5), the Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale (CADSS) and the Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales (PSYRATS).

Results

Dissociative symptoms were significantly higher in participants with trauma histories (PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups) and significantly correlated with hallucinations in trauma-exposed participants, but not in participants with schizophrenia (without trauma history). Hallucination severity was correlated with the CADSS amnesia subscale score, but depersonalisation and derealisation were not.

Conclusions

Dissociation may be a mechanism in trauma-exposed individuals who hear voices, but it does not explain all hallucinatory experiences. The SCZ+PTSD group were in an intermediary position between schizophrenia and PTSD on dissociative and hallucination measures. The PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups experienced dissociative phenomena much more frequently than the schizophrenia group, with a significant trend towards the amnesia subtype of dissociation.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Auditory hallucinations and the experience of trauma attend individuals who suffer a broad variety of psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).Reference Waters and Fernyhough1 An association between hallucinations and trauma has been documented in PTSD, where two-thirds of civilian trauma survivors and half of traumatised military veterans report hearing voices.Reference Brewin and Patel2 Such findings have led to suggestions that trauma might be a direct cause of auditory hallucinations.Reference Luhrmann, Alderson-Day, Bell, Bless, Corlett and Hugdahl3,Reference Longden, Madill and Waterman4 Because dissociation is a common response to trauma, one hypothesis is that trauma causes hallucinations through the process of dissociation, which either impairs the integration of traumatic experience with components of the self identityReference Longden, Madill and Waterman4,Reference Moskowitz and Corstens5 or causes a failure of contextual integration of present and past experiences, resulting in involuntary intrusions.Reference Steel, Fowler and Holmes6 In support, robust associations between voice hearing and dissociation (r = 0.52) have been confirmed in a meta-analysis involving 31 studies,Reference Pilton, Varese, Berry and Bucci7 suggesting that dissociation (and particularly depersonalisationReference Perona-Garcelan, Carrascoso-Lopez, Garcia-Montes, Ductor-Recuerda and Jimenez8) may be a mediating factor between trauma and certain types of hallucinatory experiences. However, heterogeneity in the results, methodological limitations and a lack of direct evidence in schizophrenia have led to calls for more research in this area. Furthermore, recent research has questioned whether trauma is necessarily involved in all hallucinations, or whether instead there may be different causal pathways for different types of hallucination.Reference Luhrmann, Alderson-Day, Bell, Bless, Corlett and Hugdahl3

The present study sought to broaden the understanding of the relationship between dissociation, auditory hallucinations and trauma, using carefully selected participant samples and validated measures. The experience of hallucinations and dissociative state symptoms were examined in three groups of participants with auditory hallucinations and with different diagnoses and trauma histories: schizophrenia (without trauma/PTSD), PTSD with dissociation, and comorbid schizophrenia and PTSD (SCZ+PTSD). Levels of dissociation and hallucination scores were compared between groups, and the strength of the relationship between dissociation and hallucinations was assessed using correlation analyses. Subtypes of symptoms of dissociation were examined using regression analysis. This information can be used to inform clinical management of complex cases involving hearing voices, dissociation and trauma.

Method

Participants

Participants (n = 71) were recruited from psychiatric services in Perth, Western Australia. Clinical diagnosis was established according to DSM-59 criteria by their treating psychiatrist. Inclusion criteria included the presence of auditory hallucinations, a diagnosis of schizophrenia and/or PTSD with dissociation, age >18 years and capacity to give informed consent. Exclusion criteria included involuntary status or comorbid substance misuse.

Measures

All validated measures were completed with all participants in one session by the treating clinician, and in the following order:

  1. (1) PTSD Symptoms Scale Interview (PSSI-5):Reference Foa, McLean, Zang, Zhong, Rauch and Porter10 this is a validated semi-structured 17-item scale which assesses PTSD diagnosis and severity on the basis of DSM-5 criteria. All participants were assessed for lifetime traumatic events where trauma was experienced, witnessed or confronted and accompanied by an intense fear, helplessness or horror. The absence/presence of trauma experience as assessed by the PSSI-5 was used to assign participants with schizophrenia to diagnostic subgroups. Participants presenting with complex and multiple traumas were asked to focus on their worst traumatic experience for the assessment.

  2. (2) Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales (PSYRATS), hallucinations subscale:Reference Haddock, McCarron, Tarrier and Faragher11 this instrument comprises 11 items designed to quantify multiple dimensions regarding hallucinations, including frequency, duration, location, loudness, belief regarding origin, negative content, controllability, distress and disruption to life.

  3. (3) Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale (CADSS), subjective items:Reference Bremner, Krystal, Putman, Southwick, Marmar and Charney12 this instrument measures present state dissociative symptoms with 23 items under clinician guidance. Subjective items were used to explore the phenomenology of the experience. Participants were given explicit instructions to focus their attention on their experience of hearing voices while completing this measure. This scale immediately followed the PSYRATS to encourage this focus of attention. Subscales assessed were amnesia (2 items, 14 and 15), derealisation (12 items, 1, 2, 8–13, 16–19) and depersonalisation (5 items, 3–7);Reference Bremner, Krystal, Putman, Southwick, Marmar and Charney12 items 20–23 were not included in the subscale analysis.

  4. (4) Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS):Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler13 This widely used scale measures positive and negative psychosis symptom severity and general psychopathology with 28 items on a 7-point scale.

Ethics approval

Signed informed consent was obtained from all participants. Ethics approval was obtained from St John of God Hospital (ID 1459) and Joondalup Health Campus Committee (ID 1834) research ethics committees and the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) (DW01847).

Statistical analyses

Demographic and clinical variables and total scores were analysed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables and chi-squared for categorical variables. The association between hallucinations and dissociation was examined using group comparisons, correlations and regressions. Data were sufficiently normally distributed to satisfy statistical assumptions for parametric tests and Pearson's product correlations were conducted. To explore which dissociation subtypes affected hallucination severity, regressions were conducted. Using Gignac & Szodorai'sReference Gignac and Szodorai14 determination that correlations ≥0.3 can reasonably be considered large, we examined the number of CADSS items that correlated ≥0.3 with PSYRATS scores. We then conducted stepwise regressions of the PSYRATS scores on CADSS items that correlated ≥0.3. Finally, to examine whether dissociation (CADSS) mediated the relationship between trauma (PSSI-5) and voice hearing (PSYRATS) for the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD participants, mediation analysis was run using PROCESS model 4 by HayesReference Hayes15 in SPSS with the three CADSS subscales as potential mediators and 5000 bootstrapped resamples.

Results

On the basis of the treating psychiatrists’ clinical knowledge and instrument scores, three diagnostic groups of participants were identified: schizophrenia without trauma/PTSD (n = 18), PTSD with dissociation but no psychosis (n = 27) and comorbid schizophrenia and PTSD (SCZ+PTSD, n = 26). Table 1 shows that there were non-significant group differences in age and gender between the three groups and that the two PTSD groups did not differ significantly in mean age at which trauma was experienced.

Table 1 Demographic and questionnaire scorea comparisons among the three participant groups

CADSS, Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; PANSS-P, positive symptoms, PANSS-N, negative symptoms; PANSS-G, general psychopathology; PSSI-5, 5-item PTSD Symptoms Scale Interview; PSYRATS, Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

a. Subscript a > b > c indicate that means in same row are significantly different P < 0.05.

b. F-ratio d.f. = 1.51.

*P < 0.05 based on Tukey's least significant difference (LSD) post hoc test.

Trauma scores and type (PSSI-5)

Both the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups had severe PTSD (average scores >46), although PSSI-5 scores were significantly higher in the PTSD than in the SCZ+PTSD group (Table 1). The schizophrenia group scored 0, denoting an absence of trauma history. The type of worst trauma experience was similar between the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups. In the PTSD group, 70% of the trauma was experienced in childhood and 84% of the trauma was sexual abuse. The 30% of adult trauma was represented by adult rape, witness of accidental death and refugee war trauma. In the SCZ+PTSD group, 66% of the trauma was childhood abuse, of which 76% was sexual abuse. The 34% of adult trauma included witness of accidental death, experience of sexual or physical abuse and refugee war trauma.

Positive and negative symptoms (PANSS)

The SCZ+PTSD and schizophrenia groups had significantly greater clinical severity of symptoms on the PANSS than the PTSD group.

Hallucination severity (PSYRATS)

The PSYRATS scores were similar between groups, although total scores tended to be higher in the schizophrenia and SCZ+PTSD groups compared with the PTSD group, the differences just reaching statistical significance (P < 0.05).

Dissociation (CADSS)

Total dissociation scores were significantly higher for the PTSD group than the SCZ+PTSD group, which in turn were significantly higher than for the schizophrenia group. On the CADSS depersonalisation subscale, the PTSD group scored significantly higher than the SCZ+PTSD group, but the amnesia and derealisation subscale scores were equivalent in the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups (Table 1).

Correlations: hallucination severity (PSYRATS) and trauma severity (PSSI-5)

The PSYRATS and PSSI-5 scores were positively correlated in the PTSD (r = 0.55, P < 0.001) and SCZ+PTSD (r = 0.82, P < 0.001) groups, such that higher trauma scores were clearly associated with increased severity of hallucinations.

Correlations: hallucination severity (PSYRATS) and dissociation subscales (CADSS)

Total CADSS scores correlated significantly with PSYRATS scores in the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups, but not in the schizophrenia group (Table 2). In the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups, there were significant correlations between the PSYRATS scores and the CADSS subscale of amnesia, but the correlation between PSYRATS scores and CADSS subscales of depersonalisation and derealisation did not reach significance. There were no significant correlations in the schizophrenia group using both total and subscale CADSS scores.

Table 2 Correlations between variables by the three participant groups

CADSS, Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale; PSYRATS, Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

a. Bold denotes significant correlations, P < 0.05.

Regression: dissociation subscale (CADSS) and hallucination severity (PSYRATS) scores

CADSS items were entered in order of correlation strength into three stepwise regressions (one per diagnostic category). Only three CADSS items correlated ≥0.3 with PSYRATS scores for participants in the schizophrenia group, whereas nine and seven CADSS items respectively met this criterion for the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups. Table 2 shows significant correlations between the PSYRATS and the CADSS amnesia subscale in the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups. In the PTSD participants, CADSS item 14 (β = 0.44) (Have there been things which have happened during this assessment that now you can't account for?) and item 21 (β = 0.53) (Do you feel confused about who you really are?) predicted 48.3% of the variance in PSYRATS scores (F[2,24] = 13.14, P < 0.001). In the SCZ+PTSD participants, CADSS item 15 (β = 0.61) (Have you spaced out, or in some other way lost track of what was going on during this experience?) alone predicted 35% of the variance in PSYRATS scores (F[1,24] = 14.46, P = 0.001) while no further items significantly added to the prediction of PSYRATS scores in this group.

In the schizophrenia group, there were no significant correlations with any of the CADSS subscale scores (Table 2), although two individual items from the CADSS derealisation and depersonalisation subscales predicted 51.1% of the variance in PSYRATS scores (F[2,16] = 9.3, P = 0.003): item 1 (β = 0.60) (Do things seem to be moving in slow motion?) and item 6 (β = −0.42) (Do you feel disconnected from your own body?).

Does dissociation mediate the relationship between trauma and voice hearing?

A mediation analysis examined whether dissociation (CADSS) mediated the relationship between trauma (PSSI-5) and voice hearing (PSYRATS) in the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups together. The effect of PSSI-5 on PSYRATS was significant (c = 0.62, t[52] = 3.89, P = 0.003), and this was partially mediated by CADSS amnesia (c′ = 0.54, t[52] = 2.82, P = 0.069; a 1b 1 = 0.22 (bootstrapped 95 CI: 0.10–0.42)). CADSS derealisation and depersonalisation were not significant mediators of the relationship between trauma and voice hearing.

Discussion

This study examined whether dissociation plays a central role in the experience of hallucinations, by examining state dissociation experiences in three groups of participants who hear voices but differ in terms of trauma experience. The levels of dissociation in the PTSD and the SCZ+PTSD groups were significantly higher than in the schizophrenia group (who had no trauma history). The findings also showed a significant correlation between the severity of dissociation and severity of experience of hallucinations in the trauma-exposed groups (PTSD and SCZ+PTSD), but not in the schizophrenia group.

Trauma, dissociation and hallucinations

The PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups showed increased dissociation, specifically amnesia, linked to an increase in severity of hallucinations, supporting the mediating role of dissociation in individuals who have been exposed to trauma. There is now evidence that childhood adversity has occurred at significantly higher rates in people with psychosis. Studies have reported a 47% rate of childhood sexual abuse in populations with psychotic illnessReference Bendall, Jackson and Hulbert16 and that early trauma exposure significantly increases the risk for psychosis.Reference Morrison, Frame and Larkin17 However, although trauma can never been ruled out, not all individuals who experience hallucinations report trauma.Reference Luhrmann, Alderson-Day, Bell, Bless, Corlett and Hugdahl3

In contrast, the subset of participants with schizophrenia without trauma in our study showed a lack of correlation between dissociation and hallucination scores, suggesting that trauma (and the process of dissociation) is not always necessary for auditory hallucinations to occur, as has been previously suggested.Reference Longden, Madill and Waterman4 This result provides potential support for the theory that there are multiple pathways to hearing voices.Reference Brewin and Patel2 Outside of the psychosocial stressors and personal narratives of voice hearers, neurobiological mechanisms in schizophrenia clearly link to auditory hallucinations, including frontotemporal connectivity alterations affecting language processing networks and loss of sensory anchors acting to increase dependence on prior expectations.Reference Allen, Modinos, Hubl, Shields, Cachia and Jardri18

Comorbid schizophrenia and PTSD

A substantial proportion of our participants had comorbid schizophrenia and PTSD (SCZ+PTSD). This is consistent with the literature, which suggests that prevalence rates of PTSD in schizophrenia spectrum disorders are fairly high, varying between 0 and 55%, with 78% of papers reporting >10%.Reference Dallel, Cancel and Fakra19 This group had significantly higher scores on the PANSS and its three subscales, in line with studies showing that the severity of trauma and illness factors can result in increased symptom severity and increased comorbidity.Reference Dallel, Cancel and Fakra19 Yet this group had intermediary results in the severity of dissociation and hallucinations. This introduces the interesting concept of a subgroup of participants who experienced trauma-linked dissociative hallucinations, as well as auditory hallucinations that directly link to their psychotic illness. This concept supports the research in the field suggesting that both biological and psychological treatment strategies are clinically useful in schizophrenia.Reference Longden, Madill and Waterman4

CADSS dissociation subscales: amnesia, derealisation and depersonalisation

The results from our analyses of the dissociation subscales of the CADSS were less clear-cut, which was possibly due to limited statistical power, particularly in the schizophrenia group. In the PTSD and SCZ+PTSD groups, however, there was a significant correlation between hearing voices and amnesia subscale scores. Regression analysis suggested that dissociative hallucinations were best predicted by symptoms of amnesia.

There was a non-significant trend with depersonalisation. In the literature, depersonalisation symptoms have been linked to auditory hallucinationsReference Longden, Madill and Waterman4,Reference Moskowitz and Corstens5 in which the voice is seen to represent a component of self-identity which has been dissociated owing to failure of integration of a traumatic experience.Reference Longden, Madill and Waterman4 Depersonalisation also plays a central role in the concept of loss of internal anchors described by Allen et al.Reference Allen, Coyne and Console20

Nevertheless, in this study the evidence of the role of amnesia in mediating voice hearing was considerably stronger, particularly given that mediation analysis is a statistical tool that attempts to describe causality.Reference Kosuke, Keele and Tingley21 Steel et alReference Steel, Fowler and Holmes6 have developed a contextual integration model to explain the experience of involuntary intrusion in trauma. This model provides the best fit for the results in this study, where trauma fundamentally disrupts the ability to encode and integrate incoming sensory information with co-occurring data stored in the hippocampus. Clinically, this weakened ability to integrate information would present as amnesia and the sensory involuntary intrusions could present as hallucinations. The dissociative amnesia may represent an attempt of the unconscious to forget, and the potential failure of this suppressive defence process invites the experience of hallucinations as involuntary sensory intrusions.

Regression analysis using item scores in the schizophrenia group showed that hallucinations correlated with a sense of detachment from the world (things moving in slow motion) and had an inverse relationship to depersonalisation (disconnected from their own body). Although changes in the sense of body including a loss of ‘bodily-self’ are a common finding in schizophrenia,Reference Stanghellini22 in our study this depersonalisation experience was negatively correlated with hearing voices. In contrast, seeing the world as distant or detached, consistent with the derealisation, was positively correlated. This finding is reflected in the clinical situation, where auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia are often described as ‘real’Reference Aggemaes23 and can be experienced with a clear sense of the self ‘having a conversation’, whereas the world is seen as distant and detached.

Dissociative hallucination process model

We would suggest that individuals undergo two psychological processes when experiencing dissociative hallucinations (Fig. 1). The first is a loss of integration of consciousness, with either detachment (the subjective experience of an altered mental state) or compartmentalisation (a failure to control that which is normally under voluntary control), as described in Cardena's original model of dissociation.Reference Cardena, Lynn and Rhue24 The second process might involve the involuntary intrusion of sensory, affective or cognitive content into conscious awareness, in order to describe the experience of dissociative hallucinations.Reference Steel, Fowler and Holmes6,Reference Krause-Utz, Frost, Winter and Elzinga25 These intrusions could be trauma specific, as in dissociative flashbacks, or general, as in dissociative hallucinations. We would see dissociative hallucinations as occurring on a continuum from the experience of internal voices to external, uncontrollable, ego-alien negative voices. Voices may begin with a link to negative self-talk and memories of abuse but can lose that link as anchors fade.

Fig. 1 Mechanisms of symptom experience in dissociation. Adapted from CardenaReference Cardena, Lynn and Rhue24 and Steel et alReference Steel, Fowler and Holmes6.

Limitations

This research has limitations and methodological problems. The subjective experience of dissociation is difficult to operationalise, and despite our efforts to link the symptoms to hearing voices, it is potentially vulnerable to contamination. The use of the DSM-5 definition of trauma in PTSD set specific criteria but potentially leaves out many other trauma experiences. We asked participants specifically about their trauma memories, but recognise that retrospective descriptions of trauma are open to contamination. Individuals with schizophrenia are exposed to trauma, and dissecting comorbidity can be difficult in complex cases. Confounding variables in all participants may have included other diagnoses, such as major depression or generalised anxiety disorder, as only comorbid substance misuse was specifically excluded. It is also important to note that the PSYRATS examines the severity of existing hallucinations, but the process of development of these hallucinations cannot be directly measured.

Sample size was also a significant problem. The small number of participants, especially in the schizophrenia group, affected the statistical power of analyses, particularly when considering correlations among subscales. Small sample size means that sampling variability may affect inferences drawn about the represented population. For example, the participants with PTSD had PSSI-5 scores that represented a severe degree of PTSD symptomology. This increases the likelihood of multiple and complex trauma and means that the data are not necessarily representative of all individuals with PTSD. Group assignment was not random and required clinical accuracy regarding diagnosis, although this problem was mediated by the fact that all the psychiatrists involved had significant (between 10 and 30 years’) clinical experience and were dealing with patients who were clinically known to them.

Clinical and research implications

If experiences of auditory hallucinations have more than a single causative pathway, it is clinically valid to try to distinguish them. The description of hallucinatory experiences can be similar between individuals who have PTSD and schizophrenia, so it is essential to consider contextual variables such as dissociation symptoms. Dissociation is a universal defence mechanism in response to trauma but our groups described differing experiences of it. There is therefore a need to encourage further research into the role of dissociation, particularly amnesia, as a potential mediating factor between hallucinations and trauma.

Data availability

Data is available from the authors on request.

Author contributions

All authors meet ICMJE criteria for authorship.

Declaration of interest

None.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.31.

References

Waters, F, Fernyhough, C.Hallucinations: a systemic review of points of similarity and difference across diagnostic classes. Schizophr Bull 2017; 43: 3243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewin, CR, Patel, T. Auditory pseudohallucinations in United Kingdom war veterans and civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2010; 71: 419–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luhrmann, TM, Alderson-Day, B, Bell, V, Bless, JJ, Corlett, P, Hugdahl, K, et al. Beyond trauma: a multiple pathways approach to auditory hallucinations in clinical and nonclinical populations. Schizophr Bull 2019; 45: S2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longden, E, Madill, A, Waterman, G. Dissociation, trauma and the role of lived experience: toward a new conceptualization of voice hearing. Psychol Bull 2012; 138: 2876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, A, Corstens, D.Auditory hallucinations: psychotic symptom or dissociative experience? J Psychol Trauma 2007; 6: 3563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steel, C, Fowler, D, Holmes, E.Trauma-related intrusions and psychosis: an information processing account. Behav Cog Psychother 2005; 33: 139–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilton, M, Varese, F, Berry, K, Bucci, S. The relationship between dissociation and voices: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2015; 189: 138–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perona-Garcelan, S, Carrascoso-Lopez, F, Garcia-Montes, JM, Ductor-Recuerda, MJ, Jimenez, AM. Dissociative experiences as mediators between childhood trauma and auditory hallucinations. J Traumatic Stress 2012; 25: 323–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn). (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013.Google Scholar
Foa, E, McLean, C, Zang, Y, Zhong, J, Rauch, S, Porter, K, et al. Psychometric properties of the posttraumatic stress disorder symptom scale interview for DSM-5 (PSSI-5). Psychol Assess 2016; 28: 1159–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haddock, G, McCarron, J, Tarrier, N, Faragher, E. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales (PSYRATS). Psychol Med 1999; 29: 879–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, J, Krystal, J, Putman, W, Southwick, S, Marmar, C, Charney, D, et al. Measurement of dissociative states with the Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale (CADSS). J Trauma Stress 1998; 11: 125–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, SR, Fiszbein, A, Opler, LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13: 261–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gignac, GE, Szodorai, ET. Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Pers Individ Differ 2016; 102: 74–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (2nd edn). Guilford, 2018.Google Scholar
Bendall, S, Jackson, H, Hulbert, C. Childhood trauma and psychosis: a review of the evidence and directions for psychological interventions. Aust Psychol 2010; 45: 299306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, AP, Frame, L, Larkin, W. Relationships between trauma and psychosis: a review and integration. Br J Clin Psychol 2003; 42: 331–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, P, Modinos, G, Hubl, D, Shields, G, Cachia, A, Jardri, R, et al. Neuroimaging auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: from neuroanatomy to neurochemistry and beyond. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 695703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallel, S, Cancel, A, Fakra, E. Prevalence of post traumatic stress disorder in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a systematic review. J Neuropsychiatry 2018; 8: 1027–37.Google Scholar
Allen, J, Coyne, L, Console, D. Dissociative detachment relates to psychotic symptoms and personality decompensation. Compr Psychiatry 1997; 38: 327–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosuke, I, Keele, L, Tingley, D. A general approach to causal mediation analysis. Psychol Methods 2010; 15: 309–34.Google Scholar
Stanghellini, G. Embodiment and schizophrenia. World Psychiatry 2009; 8: 56–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aggemaes, A. The experienced reality of hallucinations and other psychological phenomena. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1972; 48: 220–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardena, E. The domain of dissociation. In Dissociation: Theoretical, Clinical and Research Perspectives (eds Lynn, SJ, Rhue, RW): 1531. Guilford, 1994.Google Scholar
Krause-Utz, A, Frost, R, Winter, D, Elzinga, BM. Dissociation and alterations in brain function and structure: implications for borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2017; 19(1): 6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic and questionnaire scorea comparisons among the three participant groups

Figure 1

Table 2 Correlations between variables by the three participant groups

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Mechanisms of symptom experience in dissociation. Adapted from Cardena24 and Steel et al6.

Supplementary material: File

Wearne et al. supplementary material

Wearne et al. supplementary material

Download Wearne et al. supplementary material(File)
File 7.5 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.