Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T12:19:18.459Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of weather conditions on cheetah monitoring with scat detection dogs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 August 2021

Noreen M. Mutoro*
Affiliation:
Evolutionary Zoology, Department of Biosciences, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunner Str. 34, A-5020Salzburg, Austria Action for Cheetahs in Kenya, PO Box 1611, KE-00606Nairobi, Kenya Faculty of Information Management and Media, Karlsruhe University of Applied Sciences, D-76133Karlsruhe, Germany
Jonas Eberle
Affiliation:
Evolutionary Zoology, Department of Biosciences, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunner Str. 34, A-5020Salzburg, Austria
Jana S. Petermann
Affiliation:
Experimental Ecology, Department of Biosciences, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunner Str. 34, A-5020Salzburg, Austria
Gertrud Schaab
Affiliation:
Faculty of Information Management and Media, Karlsruhe University of Applied Sciences, D-76133Karlsruhe, Germany
Mary Wykstra
Affiliation:
Action for Cheetahs in Kenya, PO Box 1611, KE-00606Nairobi, Kenya
Jan Christian Habel
Affiliation:
Evolutionary Zoology, Department of Biosciences, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunner Str. 34, A-5020Salzburg, Austria
*
Author for correspondence: Noreen M. Mutoro, Email: noreen.mutoro@stud.sbg.ac.at
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Knowledge on cheetah population densities across their current range is limited. Therefore, new and efficient assessment tools are needed to gain more knowledge on species distribution, ecology and behaviour. Scat detection dogs have emerged as an efficient and non-invasive method to monitor elusive and vulnerable animal species, like cheetahs, due to the dog’s superior olfactory system. However, the success of locating scat using detection dogs can be significantly improved under suitable weather conditions. We examined the impact of temperature, humidity and wind speed on detection rates of scat from cheetahs during a scat detection dog survey in Northern Kenya. We found that average wind speed positively influences the scat detection rate of detection dogs working on leash. Humidity showed no significant influence. Temperature showed a strong negative correlation with humidity and thus was excluded from our model analyses. While it is likely that wind speed is especially invalid for dogs working off leash, this study did not demonstrate this. Wind speed could thus influence the success of monitoring cheetahs or other target species. Our findings help to improve the survey and thus maximise the coverage of study area and the collection of target samples of elusive and rare species.

Type
Short Communication
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Wildlife monitoring is crucial for effective nature conservation as it provides valuable information on the distribution, abundance and demography of species and local populations (Yoccoz et al. Reference Yoccoz, Nichols and Boulinier2001). Conservation and management of rare and wide-ranging species requires frequent monitoring using standardised techniques and protocols that are cost and time efficient (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011). Non-invasive sampling is a preferred technique to collect presence data and biological samples. Such data and samples may give insights into species and population trends, population viability, genetic structures and physiological stress (Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Betsch, Wultsch, Mesa, Scott Mills, Boitani and Powell2012). Results obtained from standardised monitoring schemes can be used as an early warning system in species conservation (Zemanova, Reference Zemanova2019). However, it is particularly difficult to collect data of elusive species, which occur in low densities, such as most species at higher trophic levels like top predators (Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007a).

Various non-invasive monitoring techniques exist to observe free-ranging mammals, such as direct observation (Broekhuis et al. Reference Broekhuis, Bisset, Chelysheva, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018), camera traps (Fabiano et al. Reference Fabiano, Boast, Fuller, Sutherland, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018) or spoor surveys based on foot prints or faeces (Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Betsch, Wultsch, Mesa, Scott Mills, Boitani and Powell2012). The success of spoor surveys strongly relies on species abundance and the availability of a suitable substrate to detect footprints (Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Betsch, Wultsch, Mesa, Scott Mills, Boitani and Powell2012; Boast et al. Reference Boast, van Bommel, Andresen, Fabiano and Nyhus2018). Detection dogs have emerged as an alternative non-invasive method to monitor elusive, rare and endangered animal species (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011). This method is based on the dog’s superior olfactory system (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017) and has been successfully used for many species, including cheetahs Acinonyx jubatus (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017), koalas Phascolarctos cinereus (Cristescu et al. Reference Cristescu, Foley, Markula, Jackson, Jones and Fère2015) and non-human primates (Orkin et al. Reference Orkin, Yang, Yang, Yu and Jiang2016). This method includes the collection of both, presence data and biological samples for subsequent analyses (e.g. population genetics and/or physiology) (Wasser et al. Reference Wasser, Davenport, Ramage, Hunt, Parker, Clarke and Stenhouse2004; Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007a; Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017).

The populations of cheetahs in Eastern Africa are among the largest remnant populations of this vulnerable carnivore (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017; Marker et al. Reference Marker, Cristescu, Morrison, Flyman, Horgan, Sogbohossou, Bissett, van der Merwe, de Matos Machado, Fabiano, van der Meer, Aschenborn, Melzheimer, Young-Overton, Farhadinia, Wykstra, Chege, Samna, Amir, Sh Mohanun, Paulos, Nhabanga, M’soka, Belbachir, Ashenafi, Nghikembua, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018). Kenya’s cheetah population is estimated at about 1,200 individuals (Durant et al. Reference Durant, Mitchell, Groom, Pettorelli, Ipavec, Jacobson, Woodroffe, Böhm, Hunter, Becker, Broekhuis, Bashir, Andresen, Aschenborn, Beddiaf, Belbachir, Belbachir-Bazi, Berbash, de Matos Machado, Breitenmoser, Chege, Cilliers, Davies-Mostert, Dickman, Ezekiel, Farhadinia, Funston, Henschel, Horgan, de Iongh, Jowkar, Klein, Lindsey, Marker, Marnewick, Melzheimer, Merkle, M’soka, Msuha, O’Neill, Parker, Purchase, Sahailou, Saidu, Samna, Schmidt-Küntzel, Selebatso, Sogbohossou, Soultan, Stone, van der Meer, van Vuuren, Wykstra and Young-Overton2017). The species occurs in low densities (Marker et al. Reference Marker, Cristescu, Morrison, Flyman, Horgan, Sogbohossou, Bissett, van der Merwe, de Matos Machado, Fabiano, van der Meer, Aschenborn, Melzheimer, Young-Overton, Farhadinia, Wykstra, Chege, Samna, Amir, Sh Mohanun, Paulos, Nhabanga, M’soka, Belbachir, Ashenafi, Nghikembua, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018). Most of the individuals occur beyond protected areas (Durant et al. Reference Durant, Mitchell, Ipavec and Groom2015, Reference Durant, Mitchell, Groom, Pettorelli, Ipavec, Jacobson, Woodroffe, Böhm, Hunter, Becker, Broekhuis, Bashir, Andresen, Aschenborn, Beddiaf, Belbachir, Belbachir-Bazi, Berbash, de Matos Machado, Breitenmoser, Chege, Cilliers, Davies-Mostert, Dickman, Ezekiel, Farhadinia, Funston, Henschel, Horgan, de Iongh, Jowkar, Klein, Lindsey, Marker, Marnewick, Melzheimer, Merkle, M’soka, Msuha, O’Neill, Parker, Purchase, Sahailou, Saidu, Samna, Schmidt-Küntzel, Selebatso, Sogbohossou, Soultan, Stone, van der Meer, van Vuuren, Wykstra and Young-Overton2017). Cheetahs rarely rub against features for territorial and social communication. Thus, the collection of hairs as biological samples is ineffective (Schmidt-Küntzel et al. Reference Schmidt-Küntzel, Wultsch, Boast, Braun, Van der Weyde, Wachter, Brummer, Walker, Forsythe, Marker, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018). The use of scat detection dogs for cheetah monitoring improves success rates in monitoring schemes (Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Betsch, Wultsch, Mesa, Scott Mills, Boitani and Powell2012). However, success rates of scat detection with dogs might depend on various abiotic conditions, such as temperature, humidity and wind speed (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Ralls, Hurt, Adams, Parker, Davenport, Smith and Maldonados2003; Wasser et al. Reference Wasser, Davenport, Ramage, Hunt, Parker, Clarke and Stenhouse2004; Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011).

In this study, we examined the influence of temperature, humidity and wind speed on the detection rates of scat from cheetahs, using two dogs during uncontrolled field surveys (field surveys that used natural scat frequencies instead of experimental manipulations) in two national reserves in northern Kenya. The dogs searched on-leash along linear search transects. The dogs were leashed because of the presence of various dangerous animals like sympatric carnivores and large herbivores in our study area. We measured temperature, humidity and wind during each campaign. The results of our study provide data on the best weather conditions suited for scat detection dogs worked on leash and help to improve future cheetah monitoring with scat detection dogs.

Material and methods

Study area

As study area we selected the Buffalo Springs (0.52°N; 37.62°E) and Samburu (0.62°N; 37.53°E) National Reserve in Northern Kenya. The two reserves cover approximately 296 km2 and are separated by the Ewaso Nyiro River. The area lies at an altitude of 800–1200 m and is topographically characterised by rugged hills and water courses (Wittemyer, Reference Wittemyer2001). Rainfall is localised and highly variable in the region with bimodal distribution during the long rains in March–April and short rains in October–November (Wittemyer, Reference Wittemyer2001; Ogara et al. Reference Ogara, Nduhiu, Andanje, Oguge, Nduati and Mainga2010; Ihwagi et al. Reference Ihwagi, Chira, Kironchi, Vollrath and Douglas-Hamilton2011). The distribution of vegetation highly depends on the availability of water. Riverine woodlands along the banks of Ewaso Nyiro River are dominated by Acacia elatior and Hyphaene coriacea, while the saline soils of low lying pans adjacent to the river are dominated by salt bush, Salsola droides. In the dry regions more distant from the river, Acacia-Commiphora semiarid scrub woodland and Acacia wooded grassland are growing (Wittemyer, Reference Wittemyer2001).

Data collection

Linear search transects with an average length of 1.96 ± 0.69 km were set in areas where recent cheetah sightings were available (sightings from the past one month before respective campaigns). Cheetah scat detection was performed with a detection dog team, consisting of two detection dogs, a main dog handler and one orienteer (see Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017). The orienteer directed the main handler along the predefined transect during searches. The two detection dogs, a male Border Collie Rottweiler mix and a female Belgian Malinois/German Shepherd mix, were locally trained to locate cheetah scat in the field. Each dog had more than three years of training on cheetah scat using both wild samples opportunistically collected in the field and samples from cheetahs in captive facilities. The two dogs worked on leash (with 15 feet long leashes) and alternating. The dog team used a linear search strategy where the detection dogs searched one-way along a linear transect into the wind. The detection dogs searched into the wind to increase their detection rate, which was limited by working them on leash, resulting in reduced freedom to search for scents from multiple directions (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011; Cristescu et al. Reference Cristescu, Foley, Markula, Jackson, Jones and Fère2015). We employed linear search strategy to optimise the area covered and detection of scat samples from female, juvenile and other non-territorial male (floaters) cheetahs in the study area which do not use scent marking sites (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017; Schmidt-Küntzel et al. Reference Schmidt-Küntzel, Wultsch, Boast, Braun, Van der Weyde, Wachter, Brummer, Walker, Forsythe, Marker, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018). Searches were conducted in June and July of the year 2019 during the morning (06h30 – 10h30). Each dog worked for a maximum of two consecutive days on a transect depending on the condition of the dog, weather and the presence of other wildlife on the transect (please see the Appendix). During the searches, the detection dogs were given six to seven minute breaks after every 15 to 30 minutes. Each detection dog was fitted with a GPS unit (Garmin Alpha TT15 E-collar) to track their search effort (distance and speed) (please see the Appendix). We recorded the number of samples found during each survey. Out of these data, we subsequently calculated the detection rate as the number of scats found per 10 km searched, for each survey (cf. Schmidt-Küntzel et al. Reference Schmidt-Küntzel, Wultsch, Boast, Braun, Van der Weyde, Wachter, Brummer, Walker, Forsythe, Marker, Nyhus, Marker, Boast and Schmidt-Küntzel2018).

Weather conditions

We recorded temperature (°C), relative humidity (%) and wind speed (meter/second) at the beginning and at the end of each search using a handheld weather station (Ambient Weather Wn-4) (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011). Based on these data, we calculated mean temperature, humidity, wind speed (from average speed) and maximum wind speed for each search (please see the Appendix) (cf. Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007a).

Statistics

We tested for correlation of all pairs of weather variables using the rcorr-function in the R-package corrplot v. 0.84 (Wei & Simko, Reference Wei and Simko2017). For variable pairs with a significant Pearson correlation coefficient greater than 0.7, only one biologically reasonable variable was chosen for the model to avoid multicollinearity (Table 1). We then applied a generalised linear mixed effect model (GLMM) as implemented in the glmer-function in the R-package lme4 v. 1.1-21 (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) to test the effect of humidity and average wind speed on the detection rate. A Poisson distribution was chosen to model the count of detected scat. Therefore, we calculated the scat detection rate per 10 km. We included detection dog as a random effect. All analyses were done in R v. 3.6.1.

Table 1. Correlation coefficients and P values for weather conditions, such as mean temperature (°C), humidity (%) and wind speed (m/s) during uncontrolled field searches using detection dogs in Northern Kenya. Significant P values are given in bold

Results and discussion

There was a strong positive effect of mean average wind (P < 0.01, χ2 = 29.83) on the dogs’ scat detection rate. This result contradicts with previous studies showing no significant influence of wind (speed and direction) on the dogs’ detection success (Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007a; Nussear et al. Reference Nussear, Esque, Heaton, Cablk, Drake, Valentin, Yee and Medica2008; Leigh & Dominick, Reference Leigh and Dominick2015; Hoffman et al. Reference Hofmann, Marker and Hondong2021). The significant effect of mean wind speed during our survey may have resulted from working the dogs on-leash and along a linear transect into the wind, when they had less freedom to search for scents from multiple directions (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011). According to Reed et al. (Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011), a non-significant effect of wind speed and direction on detection dogs performance mainly occurs when the dogs are worked off-leash and are able to move freely around the transect line in order to compensate for any effect of wind.

We found no significant effect of mean relative humidity (P = 0.98, χ² = 0.0004) on the dogs’ scat detection rates during the searches. We deduce from the high correlation with humidity (Table 1) that there is no effect of mean temperature on the dogs’ scat detection rates. Our results are consistent with results from other studies (Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007a; Cablk et al. Reference Cablk, Sagebiel, Heaton and Valentin2008; Nussear et al. Reference Nussear, Esque, Heaton, Cablk, Drake, Valentin, Yee and Medica2008; Leigh & Dominick, Reference Leigh and Dominick2015) showing no significant variation in dogs’ detection rates depending on humidity or temperature.

Although most studies could not demonstrate an effect of weather conditions on detection rates (Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007a; Nussear et al. Reference Nussear, Esque, Heaton, Cablk, Drake, Valentin, Yee and Medica2008; Leigh & Dominick, Reference Leigh and Dominick2015), weather conditions may impact scent detection and influence the time required to search a site (Long et al. Reference Long, Donovan, Mackay, Zielinsku and Buzas2007b). Wind speed and direction for instance affect how the target scent is dispersed, but highly variable wind may disperse scent and make it more difficult for a dog to follow it to its source (Shivik, Reference Shivik2002; Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011). This is compatible with our findings that wind speed can influence detection rates of scats when the dogs are worked on-leash. Relative humidity and air temperature may influence the evaporation rate of the target’s scent source (Cablk et al. Reference Cablk, Sagebiel, Heaton and Valentin2008), while high temperatures for instance can increase the dogs’ rates of panting, which reduces their scenting efficiency (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Ralls, Hurt, Adams, Parker, Davenport, Smith and Maldonados2003). It is therefore important to record environmental variables during surveys and empirically examine the relationships between environmental conditions and detection rates (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011).

Scat detection dogs have been proven to be more effective than spoor-based survey methods in cheetah monitoring (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Durant, Watson, Parker, Gottelli, M’soka, Droge, Nyirenda, Schuette, Dunkley and Brummer2017). Our results also show that a linear search strategy can be effectively used in scat detection dog surveys to search areas for which cheetahs are known (Gutzwiller, Reference Gutzwiller1990). Most studies recommend working detection dogs off-leash because the dogs can move freely around the handler and search for scents from multiple directions (Reed et al. Reference Reed, Bidlack, Hurt and Getz2011). However, detection dogs ought to be worked on-leash in high risk areas such as national reserves that have high densities of dangerous animals like sympatric carnivores and large herbivores. We would recommend that the handler considers the direction and speed of the wind to ensure maximum detection of the target species. A grid search can also be used to cover relatively smaller areas but with high intensity compared to a linear search strategy (Nussear et al. Reference Nussear, Esque, Heaton, Cablk, Drake, Valentin, Yee and Medica2008).

We acknowledge that our field surveys used natural scat frequencies instead of controlled experimental manipulations (see Cristescu et al. Reference Cristescu, Foley, Markula, Jackson, Jones and Fère2015). As a result, the number of samples in the field was not known. Limitation of this study is that this design may result in imperfect detection rates of the target species’ scat because the number of samples found is a combination of number samples present and effectiveness of the dogs’. Additionally, species identity of the samples found was not further confirmed by other means such as molecular analysis. Therefore, not all samples found may be attributable to cheetah, and the number of scats detected may thus be overestimated. These two factors may affect the accuracy of the scat detection rate, which may in turn limit its use for the analysis of the effect of weather parameters on the detection of cheetah scats. Nevertheless, we argue that under the assumption that scat density was comparable throughout our study area or for the various searches, the results of our study are likely to hold true or be confirmed in the future. Another possible limitation of this study is weather variables were only taken at the start and end of the four hour search. This is a long time period during which variables such as wind speed in particular may vary significantly and may not reflect the conditions at the time at which the dog detects the samples. For future studies, we recommend experimental surveys that quantitatively test the effect of weather conditions on scat dog performance during linear or grid search strategies and when the dogs are worked on-leash or off-leash. This information can then be used to design scat detection dog surveys that maximise coverage of the study area and detection of target samples.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Kenya Wildlife Service (KWS), National Council for Science, Technology and Innovation (NACOSTI) and the County Governments of Samburu and Isiolo for authorisation to conduct cheetah monitoring with scat detection dogs.

Funding

Funding was provided by the Katholischer Akademischer Ausländer Dienst (KAAD), by the Cheetah Conservation Fund (CCF), the American Association of Zoo Keepers, Utah Hogle and Frenso Chaffe Zoos through the Action for Cheetahs in Kenya (ACK) project.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical statement

None.

Appendix

Appendix

Table A.1. Transects surveyed for scat by the detection dogs in Buffalo Springs (BSNR) and Samburu National Reserve (SNR) Northern Kenya, in June and July 2019. Temperature, humidity and wind speed are provided as average

Table A.2. Lengths of linear transects and number of days surveyed for scat by the detection dogs in Buffalo Springs (BSNR) and Samburu National Reserve (SNR) Northern Kenya, in June and July 2019

References

Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, MS, Durant, SM, Watson, FGR, Parker, M, Gottelli, D, M’soka, J, Droge, E, Nyirenda, M, Schuette, P, Dunkley, S and Brummer, R (2017) Using dogs to find cats: detection dogs as a survey method for wide-ranging cheetah. Journal of Zoology 302, 184192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boast, LK, van Bommel, L, Andresen, L and Fabiano, E (2018) Spoor tracking to monitor cheetah populations. In Nyhus, P (ed.), Cheetahs: biology and conservation. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 427435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broekhuis, F, Bisset, C and Chelysheva, E (2018) Field methods for visual and remote monitoring of the cheetah. In Nyhus, P, Marker, L, Boast, LK and Schmidt-Küntzel, A (eds), Cheetahs: biology and conservation. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 447455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cablk, ME, Sagebiel, JC, Heaton, JS and Valentin, C (2008) Olfaction-based detection distance: a quantitative analysis of how far away dogs recognize tortoise odor and follow it to source. Sensors 8, 22082222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cristescu, RH, Foley, E, Markula, A, Jackson, G, Jones, D and Fère, C (2015) Accuracy and efficiency of detection dogs: a powerful new tool for koala conservation and management. Scientific Reports 5, 8349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durant, S, Mitchell, N, Ipavec, A and Groom, R (2015) Acinonyx jubatus. The IUCN red list of threatened species: e.T219A50649567.Google Scholar
Durant, SM, Mitchell, N, Groom, R, Pettorelli, N, Ipavec, A, Jacobson, AP, Woodroffe, R, Böhm, M, Hunter, LTB, Becker, MS, Broekhuis, F, Bashir, S, Andresen, L, Aschenborn, O, Beddiaf, M, Belbachir, F, Belbachir-Bazi, A, Berbash, A, de Matos Machado, IB, Breitenmoser, C, Chege, M, Cilliers, D, Davies-Mostert, H, Dickman, AJ, Ezekiel, F, Farhadinia, MS, Funston, P, Henschel, P, Horgan, J, de Iongh, HH, Jowkar, H, Klein, R, Lindsey, PA, Marker, L, Marnewick, K, Melzheimer, J, Merkle, J, M’soka, J, Msuha, M, O’Neill, H, Parker, M, Purchase, G, Sahailou, S, Saidu, Y, Samna, A, Schmidt-Küntzel, A, Selebatso, E, Sogbohossou, EA, Soultan, A, Stone, E, van der Meer, E, van Vuuren, R, Wykstra, M and Young-Overton, K (2017) The global decline of cheetah Acinonyx jubatus and what it means for conservation. PNAS 114, 528533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano, E, Boast, LK, Fuller, AK and Sutherland, C (2018) The use of remote camera trapping to study cheetahs: Past reflections and future directions. In Nyhus, P, Marker, L, Boast, LK and Schmidt-Küntzel, A (eds), Cheetahs: biology and conservation. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 415425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutzwiller, KJ (1990) Minimizing dog-induced biases in game bird research. Wildlife Society Bulletin 18, 351356.Google Scholar
Hofmann, T, Marker, L and Hondong, H (2021) Detection success of cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) scat by dog-human and human-only teams in a semi-arid savanna. Namibian Journal of Environment 5, Section A: 111.Google Scholar
Ihwagi, FW, Chira, RM, Kironchi, G, Vollrath, F and Douglas-Hamilton, I (2011) Rainfall pattern and nutrient content influences on African elephants’ debarking behaviour in Samburu and Buffalo Springs National Reserves, Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 50, 152159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, MJ, Betsch, J, Wultsch, C, Mesa, B and Scott Mills, L (2012) Non-invasive sampling for carnivores. In Boitani, L and Powell, R (eds), Carnivore ecology and conservation: A handbook of techniques. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4869.Google Scholar
Leigh, KA and Dominick, M (2015) An assessment of the effects of habitat structure on the scat finding performance of a wildlife detection dog. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 6, 745752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, RA, Donovan, TM, Mackay, P, Zielinsku, WJ and Buzas, JS (2007a) Effectiveness of scat detection dogs for detecting forest carnivores. Journal of Wildlife Management 71, 20072017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, RA, Donovan, TM, Mackay, P, Zielinsku, WJ and Buzas, JS (2007b) Comparing scat detection dogs, cameras, and hair snares for surveying carnivores. Journal of Wildlife Management 71, 20182025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marker, L, Cristescu, B, Morrison, T, Flyman, MV, Horgan, J, Sogbohossou, EA, Bissett, C, van der Merwe, V, de Matos Machado, IB, Fabiano, E, van der Meer, E, Aschenborn, O, Melzheimer, J, Young-Overton, K, Farhadinia, MS, Wykstra, M, Chege, M, Samna, A, Amir, OG, Sh Mohanun, A, Paulos, OD, Nhabanga, AR, M’soka, JLJ, Belbachir, F, Ashenafi, ZT and Nghikembua, MT (2018) Cheetah rangewide status and distribution, In Nyhus, P, Marker, L, Boast, LK and Schmidt-Küntzel, A (eds), Cheetahs: biology and conservation. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 3354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nussear, KE, Esque, TC, Heaton, JS, Cablk, ME, Drake, KK, Valentin, C, Yee, JL and Medica, PA (2008) Are wildlife detector dogs or people better at finding desert tortoises (Gopherus agassizii)? Herpetological Conservation and Biology 3, 103115.Google Scholar
Ogara, WO, Nduhiu, JG, Andanje, AS, Oguge, N, Nduati, WD and Mainga, OA (2010) Determination of carnivores prey base by scat analysis. African Journal of Environmental Science and Technology 4, 540546.Google Scholar
Orkin, JD, Yang, Y, Yang, C, Yu, DW and Jiang, X (2016) Cost-effective scat-detection dogs: unleashing a powerful new tool for international mammalian conservation biology. Scientific Reports 6, 34758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, S, Bidlack, AL, Hurt, A and Getz, MG (2011) Detection distance and environmental factors in conservation detection dog surveys. Journal of Wildlife Management 75, 243251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt-Küntzel, A, Wultsch, C, Boast, LK, Braun, B, Van der Weyde, L, Wachter, B, Brummer, R, Walker, EW, Forsythe, K and Marker, L (2018) Laboratory: Scat-detection dogs, genetic assignment, diet and hormone analyses. In Nyhus, P, Marker, L, Boast, LK and Schmidt-Küntzel, A (eds), Cheetahs: biology and conservation. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 437446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shivik, JA (2002) Odor-adsorptive clothing, environmental factors, and search-dog ability. Wildlife Society Bulletin 30, 721727.Google Scholar
Smith, DA, Ralls, K, Hurt, A, Adams, B, Parker, M, Davenport, B, Smith, MC and Maldonados, JE (2003) Detection and accuracy rates of dogs trained to find scats of San Joaquin kit foxes (Vulpes macrotis mutica). Animal Conservation 6, 339346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasser, SK, Davenport, B, Ramage, ER, Hunt, KE, Parker, M, Clarke, C and Stenhouse, G (2004) Scat detection dogs in wildlife research and management: application to grizzly and black bears in the Yellowhead ecosystem, Alberta, Canada. Canadian Journal of Zoology 82, 475492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wei, T and Simko, V (2017) R package “corrplot”: Visualization of a Correlation Matrix (Version 0.84). https://github.com/taiyun/corrplot Google Scholar
Wittemyer, G (2001) The elephant population of Samburu and Buffalo Springs National Reserves, Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 39, 357365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoccoz, NG, Nichols, JD and Boulinier, T (2001) Monitoring of biological diversity in space and time. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 16, 446453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zemanova, MA (2019) Poor implementation of non-invasive sampling in wildlife genetics studies. Rethinking Ecology 4, 119132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Correlation coefficients and P values for weather conditions, such as mean temperature (°C), humidity (%) and wind speed (m/s) during uncontrolled field searches using detection dogs in Northern Kenya. Significant P values are given in bold

Figure 1

Table A.1. Transects surveyed for scat by the detection dogs in Buffalo Springs (BSNR) and Samburu National Reserve (SNR) Northern Kenya, in June and July 2019. Temperature, humidity and wind speed are provided as average

Figure 2

Table A.2. Lengths of linear transects and number of days surveyed for scat by the detection dogs in Buffalo Springs (BSNR) and Samburu National Reserve (SNR) Northern Kenya, in June and July 2019