Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T00:43:41.249Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behaviour and welfare of African lion (Panthera leo) cubs used in contact wildlife tourism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2023

Ann Wilson*
Affiliation:
AW Applied Behavioural Ecology and Ecosystem Research Unit, Department of Agriculture and Environmental Sciences, University of South Africa, Florida, South Africa
Clive JC Phillips
Affiliation:
CJCP Estonian University of Life Sciences, Institute of Veterinary Medicine and Animal Sciences, Kreutzwalki 1, Tartu 51014, Estonia and Curtin University Sustainability Policy (CUSP) Institute, Curtin University, Perth, Western Australia 6845, Australia
*
Author for correspondence: Ann Wilson, Email: cheata@unisa.ac.za
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Lion (Panthera leo) cubs are used in wildlife interaction tourism but the effects on cub welfare are unknown. We assessed the behaviour of three cohorts of lion cubs, twelve animals in total, at three different interaction facilities, using continuous and scan-sampling methodologies for the entire duration of cub utilisation for human interactions. Cubs spent most time inactive (62%), particularly sleeping (38%), but also spent a substantial amount of time playing (13%) and being alert (12%). A generalised linear mixed model revealed that cub behaviour was similar in two facilities but different from cubs in the third. In these two similar facilities, as human interactions increased, the time spent resting, sleeping and playing with other cubs decreased, and alert behaviour, grooming of humans and flight responses increased. In the third facility, cubs had an abnormal activity budget, with high levels of inactivity (80%) accompanied by a lack of response to human interactions. We conclude that in some facilities normal cub behaviour cannot be achieved and may be compromised by a high frequency of human interactions, which therefore needs to be controlled to limit adverse effects on cub behaviour.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

While education, research and conservation are common objectives of modern zoos, so too is entertainment, with visitors seeking to not only learn about and view the animals from a distance, but to also interact with them in close proximity (Fernandez et al. Reference Fernandez, Tamborski, Pickens and Timberlake2009). Interacting with wildlife has as a result become a part of many modern zoos and aquaria, with the public being willing to pay extra for the experience (Kreger & Mench Reference Kreger and Mench1995). Interacting with wildlife is not restricted to zoos but also exists within a commercial environment for recreational purposes (Moorhouse et al. Reference Moorhouse, D’Cruze and Macdonald2017), such as swimming with dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), tiger-petting (Panthera tigris), selfies with sloths (Bradypus variegatus) and elephant (Elephas maximus) riding, to name just a few. These practices are driven by tourist demand, many of whom find interacting with a wild animal enjoyable (Shani Reference Shani2009).

Lion (Panthera leo) cub interaction encounters is a particularly popular international tourism activity (Wilson & Phillips Reference Wilson and Phillips2021). While exact numbers are unknown, it has been estimated that up to 10,000 lions are used internationally, with 500,000 annual visitors (Moorhouse et al. Reference Moorhouse, D’Cruze and Macdonald2017). Such tourism would include interacting with lions through petting and feeding, photographic opportunities such as selfies and walking with lions and visiting zoos and sanctuaries. There are approximately 7,000 lions in captivity in South Africa (Nowark Reference Nowark2015), owing to the captive breeding and commercial utilisation (in addition to the tourism uses referred to, this includes hunting and the farming of lion for the lion bone trade) of the species. Ethical debates in the media have brought the attention of the public to the use of lion cubs for interactions (TripAdvisor 2016; Africa Geographic 2020; Getaway 2020). Although the impact of visitors on cub behaviour and welfare has not been studied, we reported the output of a stakeholder workshop and conjoint analysis survey, which identified and ranked welfare issues faced by lion cubs within this tourism interaction industry (Wilson & Phillips Reference Wilson and Phillips2021). While the key issue identified was a lack of governance of the industry, there are some other key issues relating to cub behaviour, specifically the cubs’ ability to choose their environment and escape from the interactions, keeper understanding of cub behaviour, and cubs’ sleep and social needs. This study, as a follow-up, attempts to answer what impact human interactions, and specifically the frequency of such interactions, have on cub behaviour and what that may mean for cub welfare.

Wild animals in their natural environments will typically exhibit avoidance behaviours when confronted by humans. However, when in captivity, wild animals exposed to frequent human presence may become tame, characterised by a decreased fear of humans (Hediger Reference Hediger1964). Over time, husbandry has the capacity to produce tamer individuals (McDougall et al. Reference McDougall, Reale, Sol and Reader2006), but this is highly dependent on management systems (Tennessen & Hudson Reference Tennessen and Hudson1981) which, in turn, has associated welfare implications (Dawkins Reference Dawkins1980). Poor husbandry is most likely to generate stress in untamed individuals (McDougall et al. Reference McDougall, Reale, Sol and Reader2006). Animals involved in zoo interaction experiences are exposed to people in a different way to animals in other contexts, such as farms and laboratories, which have small groups of familiar people taking care of them (Price Reference Price1984). Animals used in interactions are additionally exposed to large numbers of unfamiliar people, who behave in an unpredictable manner and respond to them uniquely (Hosey & Melfi Reference Hosey and Melfi2015).

Proximity to humans and extensive social interactions with them induce stress responses in many species (Fernandez et al. Reference Fernandez, Tamborski, Pickens and Timberlake2009). These responses not only differ between species but also between individuals within a species (Hosey Reference Hosey2008) and such individual differences should be considered in the management of these animals (Wielebnowski Reference Wielebnowski1999). It is even recommended that these individual responses should give rise to tailored welfare treatments for individuals that respond differently from the average (Richter & Hintze Reference Richter and Hintze2019). Adaptive behavioural responses of animals can relate directly to management strategies, with responses being shown by the animals to either real (fear) and/or perceived (anxiety) threats (Boissy Reference Boissy1995). Fear and anxiety in animals are undesirable emotional states which reduce welfare (Boissy Reference Boissy1995).

Visitors to zoos have variable effects on the animals’ welfare, known as the ‘visitor-effect’, with both behavioural and physiological components (Davey Reference Davey2007). The impact is not always negative such as is seen with avoidance and aggressive behaviours, or even neutral as in habituated animals, but may even be positive, such as in attention-seeking behaviours (Sherwen & Hemsworth Reference Sherwen and Hemsworth2019). However, this visitor-effect is not easy to evaluate (Davey Reference Davey2007) and, while visitor behaviour and animal behaviour are associated, it is difficult to show causality (Margulis et al. Reference Margulis, Hoyos and Anderson2003). This may be as a result of animals causing a behavioural response in humans and vice versa, known as ‘visitor attraction’ (Davey Reference Davey2006). This bi-directional relationship will usually have a primary direction and is taxon-specific (Margulis et al. Reference Margulis, Hoyos and Anderson2003). No studies of this visitor effect on the African lion were able to be sourced but one was conducted on the Asiatic lion (Suárez et al. Reference Suárez, Recuerda and Arias-de-Reyna2017). This indicated that there was no correlation between visitor density and Asiatic lion activity and concludes that the animals were affected by the mere presence of humans irrespective of their number, with solutions needed to address such negative effects.

To measure management effects on welfare, the cumulative effects of husbandry choices on the cub’s behaviour must be assessed. The principle of additivity of multiple concurrent stressors has been demonstrated in the livestock production industry (e.g. Hyun et al. Reference Hyun, Ellis, Riskowski and Johnson1998). At lion cub interaction facilities, the effect of keeper relationships, interaction numbers and frequency, interactor behaviour, the weaning age of the cubs, starting age of interactions and effects of training styles all have the potential to affect the behaviour of the cubs in a cumulative manner.

Animal keepers and their manner of handling farm animals has been linked to productivity, with a fear of humans, derived from rough and inappropriate handling, resulting in effects such as chronic stress, reduced reproduction rates in pigs (Sus scrofa), poultry and foxes (Vulpes vulpes) and decreased milk production in dairy cows (Bos taurus) (Carlstead Reference Carlstead2009; Zulkifli Reference Zulkifli2013). The quality of relationships between zoo animals and their keepers can affect reproductive success, with responses to positive keeper time and contact (Mellen Reference Mellen1991). Positive relationships can improve social behaviours amongst chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and decrease abnormal behaviour (Baker Reference Baker1994) as can positive reinforcement training (Pomerantz & Terkel Reference Pomerantz and Terkel2009). Behavioural responses of zoo animals towards the interacting public are also dependent on the quality of interactions (Hosey & Melfi Reference Hosey and Melfi2015). Learned helplessness is a potential negative response which can be expected in wildlife interactions and has been evident in wombats (Lasiorhinus latifrons) used in human interaction encounters (Hogan et al. 2011). Crowd size, visitor frequency and proximity to the zoo animal being interacted with all affect behavioural responses (Fernandez et al. Reference Fernandez, Tamborski, Pickens and Timberlake2009). Behaviours are influenced by the personalities of the individual animals (Gosling et al. Reference Gosling, Kwan and John2003), modified by their temperament and past experiences. Animals weaned early and provided with reconstituted milk are likely to be more motivated to interact with humans than those naturally suckled (Jago et al. Reference Jago, Krohn and Matthews1999), as are animals which are handled from an early age (Markowitz et al. Reference Markowitz, Dally, Gursky and Price1988). A young, still developing animal is especially sensitive to human stimulation and this impact, either positive or negative, can have a long-lasting impact on the animal, including its genetic potential (Zulkifli Reference Zulkifli2013). It has even been suggested that repetitive negative interactions may prevent the development of future positive interactions (Hosey & Melfi Reference Hosey and Melfi2015), and this is concerning for an animal facing a lifetime in captivity. Therefore, assessing the welfare of lion cubs used in interaction facilities requires investigation into the effects of management on their behaviour, with the aim of inferring their resultant emotional states.

The main objective of our study was to investigate the behaviour of lion cubs used in wildlife tourism interaction facilities and then to specifically examine behavioural responses to human interaction frequency, with the aim of detecting and describing any possible problem behaviours.

Material and methods

Three South African lion cub tourism facilities offering very different interaction experiences were used in the study. Names and locations have been withheld to preserve their anonymity. Ethical clearance was granted for the study by the University of South Africa’s College of Agriculture and Environmental Sciences, Animal Research Ethics Committee (reference number: 2017/CAES/053).

Continuous sampling was used across the three facilities, irrespective of their management styles. We recorded durations and time budgets of all behaviours, which were mutually exclusive (Hartmann Reference Hartmann1982). Each sampling day had two recording periods, 0900–1200 and 1300–1600h. The behaviour of each cub was recorded continuously for a 10-min period within each hour, totalling 60 min per cub per day. Study sites were visited three or four times per month for the period that the cubs were used for interactions, with data being collected during both weekdays (when the facilities received fewer interactors), and on weekends and public holidays (when there were increased numbers of interactors), in order to attempt to determine the relationships between interaction numbers and behaviours displayed by the cubs. Human interaction frequency relates to the number of human interactors which entered the lion cubs’ enclosure during the two recording periods within a given sampling day. The behaviour of all of the study cubs within each facility were also scan sampled at 10-min intervals during the same two recording periods as the continuous sampling. This resulted in 36 scans per cub per recording day. The scan sampling was however only used to add context and description to rare behaviours observed.

Cubs were sampled in a random order (determined before starting the day’s recordings and this order was kept for each subsequent recording), and to minimise observer influence, the observer arrived 30 min prior to data collection, to allow the cubs to habituate to her presence. The observer watched the behaviour from inside the enclosure, with a hide deemed unnecessary because the cubs were well habituated to the presence of humans on account of their daily contact with humans. The researcher never interacted with the cubs and moved around within the enclosure to ensure that she could see all areas accessible to the cubs. This was especially important during interaction sessions with several people in the enclosure at the same time. The cubs never approached the researcher, and this was considered a necessity for accurate data collection. Inter-rater differences were eliminated by only using one observer (Hartmann Reference Hartmann1982). Trial runs during the construction and testing of the ethogram were used to train the observer. Cubs could be easily identified, making use of unique distinguishing characteristics such as colour, size, sex, hair patterns etc.

Data collection commenced as new cubs became available and therefore did not occur for exactly the same time-period for the three facilities. The number of recording days was determined by the length of time cubs were used in the activities (Table 1). Facility A comprised three female sibling cubs that were recorded altogether on ten days (four weekdays, five weekend days and one public holiday) over a 13-week period during April to June 2017. Facility B had four cubs, three siblings (one female and two males) and a single cub (male) from another litter, that were recorded on 15 days (ten weekdays and five weekend days) over a 21-week period during May to October 2017. Facility C had five cubs, two siblings (one male and one female) and three single cubs all from other litters (two males and one female) that were recorded on 12 days (eight weekdays, three weekend days and one public holiday) in a 21-week period between August 2017 and March 2018. Cubs were all hand-reared, and bottle-fed within a few days of birth.

Table 1. The three lion cub facilities included in the study and the differences in the management of the lion cub interactions

An ethogram (Table 2) was used to record cubs’ common behaviours, constructed using the available lion ethology literature (Schaller Reference Schaller1972; Bertram Reference Bertram1975; Estes Reference Estes2012). A pilot study allowed for the testing of the ethogram and the adding of any new observed behaviours.

Table 2. Behaviour categories and detailed descriptors used to record duration of daily behaviour of the cubs

Statistical analysis

A Generalised Linear Mixed Model with a negative binomial distribution and a log-link function was used to ascertain any relationship between the variables tested. Facility and Behaviour, as well as the interaction between these two, were included as the main effects in the model as differences between the facilities were likely to influence the behaviour expressed by the cubs. The individual animal, its sex and the observation day were included as random factors (they were not the focus of the study), and cub age and total number of people that encountered the cub’s enclosure within each recording day were included as continuous predictors within the model (as likely to effect behaviour, with number of people being the focus of the study). Repeated measures of each animal were accounted for statistically by nesting observation day within individual animal effects. The counts of each behaviour category were used as the response variable in the model. The glmmTMB package in R (Brooks et al. Reference Brooks, Kasper, van Benthem, Magnusson, Berg, Nielsen, Skaug, Machler and Bolker2017) was used to perform the analyses. Model structures were compared using the Wald χ2 test, through analysis of deviance type 3 testing, using the ANOVA function in the ‘car’ package (Fox & Weisberg Reference Fox and Weisberg2019), to investigate patterns of predictor significance.

For all significant predictors, pair-wise comparisons were performed using the Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, to identify significant differences between factor states. All tests were two-tailed and considered significant at P < 0.05. P-values for the outcomes of the tests were adjusted for multiple test comparisons using the p.adjust() function in R (R Core Team 2013) and the Benjamini-Hochberg correction method (Benjamini & Hochberg Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). The continuous predictor, cub age, was associated with a significant effect across facilities and so each broad ethological grouping was correlated (Pearson’s correlation coefficient) and individually tested for significance.

A percentage activity budget (indicating the mean, SD and SEM) was constructed for all behaviours derived from the three facilities used in the study. All behaviour frequencies were tested against human interaction frequency, to ascertain strengths of correlations and significance within Facilities.

Results

Each facility was observed to manage their cubs and the interaction experience in a unique way. They differed in the number and type of staff responsible for the cubs’ care, the ages of cubs used, their duration and frequency of interactions, the manner of the interaction allowed, and the disciplining of the cubs by staff (Table 1).

An activity budget of the lion cubs studied at the three interaction facilities (Table 3) shows large variation in behaviour, with big SD values, but the low SEM values give confidence in the data (Barde & Barde Reference Barde and Barde2012).

Table 3. Activity budget as % of total time (sample mean; SD; SEM) of lion cubs (n = 12) used in three wildlife tourism interaction facilities

* Ingestion not a true representation as may have occurred outside the recording periods

Graphical depiction of the mean values for behaviour of the cubs at the three facilities (Figure 1) suggests that cubs in Facilities A and B were similar to each other in their behaviour, and Facility C differed from these two for certain behaviours. This visual interpretation is supported by the statistical analysis of the interaction between ‘Facility and Behaviour’, which was a significant predictor of the Generalised linear mixed model outcomes (Wald χ214 = 100.78; P < 0.001).

Figure 1. Sample mean (± SD) percentages of time spent in behaviour categories by the lion cubs across the three interaction facilities.

Wilcoxon rank sum tests applied to each pair of facilities revealed that Facilities A and B were significantly different to Facility C (P < 0.005), but not significantly different to each other, for behaviour categories inactive, play, attentive, locomotion and aggression. Grooming and abnormal behaviour categories were not significantly different between Facilities (Table 4). As such, Facility A and B are statistically grouped together as Facility pair AB when determining a response to human interaction frequency, whilst Facility C remains independent in its response.

Table 4. Wilcoxon rank sum test statistics for each pair of Facilities (AB, AC and BC) to determine significant differences across behaviour categories

Human interaction frequency

In Facilities pair AB there was a trend (r 79 = –0.19; P = 0.08) for sleeping behaviour, but not resting behaviour (r 79 = +0.16; P = 0.15), to decline with increased human interaction frequency. Sleeping times were significantly negatively associated with resting times at Facilities pair AB (r 79 = –0.27; P = 0.01) (Figure 2). Sleeping behaviour at Facility C did not correlate with human interaction frequency (r 50 = +0.03; P = 0.86), but a decrease in resting behaviour was significantly negatively associated with an increase in human interaction frequency (r 50 = –0.03; P = 0.03) (Figure 3).

Figure 2. Relationship between sleeping behaviour frequency of cubs and the resting behaviour frequency of cubs within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Figure 3. Relationship between resting behaviour frequency of cubs and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facility C.

Conspecific play behaviour decreased as human interaction numbers increased at Facilities pair AB (r 79 = –0.25; P = 0.02) (Figure 4). Self-play behaviour frequency at this facility did not correlate with human interaction number (r 79 = +0.14; P = 0.21) and nor did that of human play (r 79 = +0.09; P = 0.44). Conspecific play at Facility C had no significant relationship with human interaction number (r 50 = +0.22; P = 0.11), and neither did self-play (r 50 = +0.22; P = 0.71). Play with humans at Facility C correlated positively with an increase in human interaction number (r 50 = +0.27; P = 0.05).

Figure 4. Relationship between conspecific play behaviour frequency of cubs and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Of all the Attentive behaviours, only alert behaviour frequency had a significant relationship with human interaction frequency at Facilities pair AB (r 79 = +0.39; P < 0.001), increasing at higher interaction frequencies (Figure 5). Investigatory behaviour (r 79 = +0.01; P = 0.94) and excitement (r 79 = +0.11; P = 0.31) were not significantly related to human interaction frequency in Facilities pair AB. Cub behaviour in Facility C was not significantly related to human interaction frequency: alert (r 50 = +0.09; P = 0.52), investigatory (r 50 = +0.16; P = 0.31), excitement behaviours (r 50 = –0.14; P = 0.25).

Figure 5. Relationship between alert behaviour frequency of cubs and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Human grooming by cubs was positively correlated with increased human interaction frequency (r 79 = +0.26; P = 0.02) in Facilities pair AB. Neither autogrooming (r 79 = –0.13; P = 0.23) nor allogrooming (r 79 = –0.07; P = 0.54) were correlated with human interaction frequency. Cubs in Facility C showed no associations between autogrooming (r 50 = +0.03; P = 0.83), allogrooming (r 50 = +0.10; P = 0.47) or human grooming (r 50 = +0.02; P = 0.88) with human interaction frequency.

Increased human interaction frequency did not illicit an increase in either human-directed aggression (r 79 = +0.09; P = 0.37) or conspecific aggression (r 79 = +0.01; P = 0.90) in Facilities pair AB, or in Facility C (human-directed aggression [r 50 = +0.14; P = 0.31], conspecific-directed aggression [r 50 = +0.04; P = 0.80]). However, in Facilities pair AB (but not in Facility C [r 50 = +0.02; P = 0.90]), flight behaviour frequency correlated positively with human interaction frequency (r 79 = +0.55; P < 0.001) (Figure 6). This flight behaviour frequency in Facilities pair AB was correlated with human-directed aggression frequency in Facilities pair AB (r 79 = +0.35; P = 0.001).

Figure 6. Relationship between flight behaviour frequency and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Rare behaviours

Certain behaviours were too rarely recorded for statistical analysis and are only described. Pacing behaviour was determined as being different from the general movement behaviour category because it did not meet the requirements of purposefully moving from one location to an intended new one within the enclosure, as a result of its repeated pattern. It was also not included within the Attentive behaviour category due to the absence of any alert behaviour, investigatory or excited behaviour. It appeared to be unique to each facility in terms of causation and occurrence. Observations recorded during the scan-sampling method, and which did not form part of the data used in the General linear mixed model, are also included to add possible context to these rare behaviours. Facility A had only one incident of pacing, while pacing behaviour in Facility B only occurred when the cubs were confined to a travel crate (n = 2). One female cub at Facility B paced after being prevented by volunteers from performing non-nutritive sucking; she also paced during the time that a fire break was being burnt around the cubs’ enclosure. Cubs in Facility C exhibited the most pacing observations (n = 13) and for considerably longer periods of time (range 30–1,316 s). Most pacing behaviours were observed when cubs were separated from each other into an adjoining enclosure with a visual barrier between (n = 6). There was also deliberate pacing along a fence line (n = 2) with another pacing (n = 1) being displayed whilst a cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) was hissing in a neighbouring new enclosure. In another pacing incident, loud noises made by trucks and machinery, outside of the enclosure coincided with pacing action (n = 1). Finally, pacing was also seen in a cub (n = 1), who recognised the keeper’s voice outside that of the enclosure.

Non-nutritive sucking may also be an indicator of a potential problem behaviour. All three female cubs at Facility A exhibited this behaviour and it was restricted to the sucking of ears. This sucking was observed six times (range: 4–120 s) and was no longer observed in these cubs after 66 days of age. Cubs in Facility B accounted for most (n = 15) non-nutritive sucking, consisting of both ear (33%) and genitalia (67%) sucking (range: 2–265 s). This behaviour was actively stopped by management and volunteers if seen, suggesting that more and longer durations of sucking could have been observed if the cubs had been allowed. Non-nutritive sucking of ears was last observed in a cub at this Facility at 133 days of age, and at 168 days of age for genitalia sucking. Facility C only had one recorded attempt to ear suckle, but after 2 s the 108 day old cub spat out the ear.

Lastly, two rarely observed problem behaviours were observed. A female cub at Facility B trembled for 204 s when she was 91 days of age. This was during the period described earlier, in which she paced due to a fire break being made around the enclosure she was in. This behaviour was not observed again. Another cub in Facility C was observed biting the fence for 59 s at 85 days of age, and again at 197 days of age (this observation lacks duration as it was recorded during a scan-sampling session).

Discussion

Scientific literature on wild lion cub activity budgets is lacking, with behavioural studies of lions in the wild focusing almost exclusively on the adult lions within a pride. However, recognising that many behaviours are stimulus-driven (Stolba & Wood-Gush Reference Stolba and Wood-Gush1984), it is clear that wild lion cubs raised within a pride will experience vastly different internal and external stimuli to those raised in captivity, devoid of adult lion presence, but confronted by frequent interaction with humans. Consequently, resulting activity budgets from two very different stimuli bases would reflect equally different behavioural repertoires and frequencies. To compare activity budgets in this situation might therefore not serve to determine their state of welfare. The non-performance of a behaviour seen in the wild does not necessarily imply that welfare is compromised in the captive individual (Veasey et al. Reference Veasey, Waran and Young1996) and it can be conversely deduced that new behaviours not being observed in wild conspecifics may not imply welfare compromise. Veasey et al. (Reference Veasey, Waran and Young1996) cautions that other techniques should be used in conjunction with wild behaviour comparisons in assessing welfare.

The extant scientific literature provides little information on inactive behaviour of lion cubs. The cubs within our study spent on average 62% of their time inactive, with the majority of this time spent sleeping. However, given that the behaviours were only recorded during the day, our data collection is not necessarily entirely representative. There is no consensus on the activity budgets of wild adult lions, even though it has been the subject of extensive research. Wild lions are easily identified from their distinguishing characteristics, such as markings that come with age, but cubs are more difficult to identify individually in the wild (Bertram Reference Bertram1975), making behavioural recording difficult for cubs, in the absence of invasive marking techniques. Hanby et al. (Reference Hanby, Bygott, Packer, ARE and Arcese1995) reported that lions in Tanzania spent up to 80% of their time inactive, sleeping, lying down and sitting, mostly during the daytime. Hayward and Hayward (Reference Hayward and Hayward2006), however, found that lions in Addo National Park, South Africa, were active for 41% of each 24-h day, with one lioness active for as much as 54% of the time. They suggest that it is a popular misconception that lions sleep for 22 h per day. A study in Zimbabwe of self-sufficient (able to hunt for themselves) captive-born adults with their wild-born, sub-adult offspring, which were being prepared for release into the wild from a 403-acre fenced managed reserve, found that resting behaviour accounted for 61% of their time (range 52–69%). However, the activity budgets of these lions were probably influenced by research activities, such as playback of territorial vocalisation calls made by other lions (Dunston et al. Reference Dunston, Abell, Doyle, Evershed and Freire2016).

A study on lion cubs which collected data at 5-min intervals over three time-periods of 1 h each, during daylight hours, exists for comparison. Captive lion cubs in a sanctuary in Zimbabwe (Ncube & Ndagurwa Reference Ncube and Ndagurwa2010), mother-raised with no environmental enrichment, spent 68% of their time resting, i.e. much more than the cubs in our study. Within the same study, two separate orphaned lion cub groups not exposed to human interactions were provided with extensive environmental enrichment, which resulted in resting percentages of 37 and 40%, respectively. When comparing the inactive behaviour of the lion cubs in this study with other available literature, it is clear that even this relatively well understood and common behavioural category within ethological studies is unable to be compared due to limited relevant literature and/or an inconsistency in methodology. Dunston et al. (Reference Dunston, Abell, Doyle, Evershed and Freire2017) conducted the first study to compare activity budgets between captive-origin and wild prides under the same methodology and determined that wild cubs rested less (on average 42%) than their captive counterparts (an average of 52%). Even under such a study of matching methodology it should be noted that the captive lions were observed more as a group than their wild counterparts and that this would affect interaction-type behaviours recorded. It was also determined that natural environmental conditions, such as closed riverbeds compared to grassland-type habitats, have effects on wild cub behaviour. As such, the activity budget of the lion cubs used in this study contributes new information about their behaviour under captive human interaction-specific environmental conditions. It must be reiterated that the focus of this study was to compare the behaviour of the cubs with themselves and each other under different human interaction frequencies. Environmental conditions such as weather, time of day and possible changes to their environments and even differences between their environments may also have influenced their behaviours, but these factors were not included as additional random effects.

The cubs at Facilities pair AB decreased sleep as human interaction frequency increased, correlating with their resting behaviour frequency increase. Wechsler (Reference Wechsler1995) explains how an animal’s behaviour has goal-related function and even coping behaviours are adapted to achieve these goals. So, even though Facilities pair AB cubs were able to maintain a level of inactivity, it is not a coping response, given that sleep and rest serve different biological functions. At Facility C, the leading behaviour in terms of time budget allocation was Inactivity at 80%, even accounting for as much as 100% of one cub’s sample budget day. A wide range of behaviours is required in order to alleviate suffering, with behavioural deprivation being classified as a central problem in animal welfare (Dawkins Reference Dawkins1988), and inappropriate husbandry preventing animals from performing behaviours more typical of their species. Excessive sleep is identified as an avoidance strategy within the Stress Response Scale for humans, to avoid a stressful situation and its implications (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Horowitz and Wilner1984). Bixler et al. (Reference Bixler, Vgontzas, Lin, Calhoun, Vela-Bueno and Kales2005) determined depression to be the most significant risk factor associated with excessive sleep in people and that the association was stronger in the young. The Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rat (Rattus norvegicus) is a genetic model in which excessive sleep and even narcolepsy have been associated with depression (Allard et al. Reference Allard, Tizabi, Shaffery, Trouth and Manaye2004). MacLellan et al. (Reference MacLellan, Fureix, Polanco and Mason2021) reviewed evidence from several animal models of depression, as well as from animals experiencing poor welfare in captive conditions, and found that they exhibited depressive-like states, with low activity and unresponsiveness being one such state. Problem behaviours, such as excessive sleep, are reflective of poor mental states and poor welfare (Gonyou Reference Gonyou1994). Inactivity can also be a biological indicator of boredom in animals (Burn Reference Burn2017), which implies an awareness of self as the animal misses an opportunity to perform alternate behaviours (Wemelsfelder Reference Wemelsfelder, Fox and Mickley1985). The corresponding negative emotion therefore associated with this behavioural problem is depression which, were it to continue, would result in suffering (Dawkins Reference Dawkins1988). It would need to be ascertained whether the behavioural deprivation effect expressed by the cubs in Facility C is reversed once interactions are ceased or whether there is a long-lasting effect. Should it continue, then the malfunction-induced behaviour (Mason Reference Mason, Mason and Rushen2006) could morph into a stereotypical behaviour, with sleep acting as the continuous repetitive behaviour.

Play behaviour occurs once all primary needs have been met, similar to exploratory behaviour, indicating a positive internal state (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011). It encourages learning through behaviours adapted from usual contexts (Smith Reference Smith1982), it strengthens social attachments (Bekoff Reference Bekoff1977) and in correct contexts and quantities, reflects a positive emotional state (Barnett Reference Barnett1958). Play is a plausible indicator of welfare at a population level rather than at an individual level (Richter et al. Reference Richter, Kästner, Kriwet, Kaiser and Sachser2016), as it has been known to increase when conditions are stressful, such as in the absence of parental care or after a period of deprivation (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011). Play therefore needs to be recorded in all of its various forms, such as social and solitary play, as well as frequencies of play and duration, if it is to be used to distinguish between different welfare states (Ahloy-Dallaire et al. Reference Ahloy-Dallaire, Espinosa and Mason2018). Our cubs spent on average 13% of their time playing, evenly divided between conspecific play and self-play, with much less play with humans. Cubs in Facilities A and B decreased their conspecific play frequency as human interaction numbers increased. Human interactions may have prevented opportunities for conspecific play, yet human play did not correspondingly increase. Self-play at Facilities pair AB was not affected by human interaction increase, suggesting that human interaction numbers typically experienced at Facilities pair AB did not prevent learning within a solitary context, nor did they increase, as is often the case during stressful situations. Facility C cubs exhibited play behaviour at a frequency of less than one-third of those at Facilities pair AB. While their self and conspecific play were not affected by human interaction increase, their play with humans increased. It is possible that any increase of play with humans at Facility C may also be indicative of a stressful situation (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011), which would support the high levels of Inactivity being exhibited as a problem behaviour.

The behaviours within the Attentive behaviour category are biologically different but were grouped together, based on their focused state. Alert behaviour was the most common form of attentive behaviour expressed by the cubs. According to Wemelsfelder (Reference Wemelsfelder1991), an animal should be able to be attentive when required to react to unexpected events and even concentrate on goal-oriented tasks. An inability to respond appropriately to environmental stimuli has been linked with a poor variability of behaviours, with animals in impoverished environments becoming habitually inattentive. Wemelsfelder (Reference Wemelsfelder, Close, Dolins and Mason1989) suggests that this inattentiveness leads to an inability to adapt to an environment and can be a precursor to stereotypic behaviours (Wiepkema Reference Wiepkema, Wiepkema and van Adrichem1987). Facility C’s cubs were more than 50% less Alert than Facilities pair AB’s cubs. Ascertaining whether this was an effect of low behaviour variability, or simply a result of less time remaining after a large proportion had been assigned to inactive behaviour, is not as important as the resulting lack of alert behaviour, which is associated with poor welfare. The Investigatory behaviour shown by the cubs occurred very infrequently and with high variability. Its presence in an activity budget suggests positive welfare, given that the benefits of such behaviour outweigh any costs and risks to the individual (Renner Reference Renner1990). The lack of the behaviour might constitute a lack of behavioural diversity. Excitement behaviour is a positive anticipatory and reward-seeking behaviour; a response to a physiological need associated with high-arousal positive states (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010). When there is a high expectation of positive events, emotions akin to optimism are experienced (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010). The presence of excitement behaviour within budgets of lion cub activity is suggestive of positive emotional states, albeit infrequent and highly variable.

Grooming behaviour was not significantly different across the three facilities and yielded similar variability. Lions groom by licking and in affiliative behaviours, such as head rubbing (Matoba et al. Reference Matoba, Kutsukake and Hasegawa2013). Licking assists in reinforcing social bonds and has hygienic benefits (Schaller Reference Schaller1972), with head rubbing providing a tactile opportunity to show affection and communicate through scents (Bradshaw & Cameron-Beaumont Reference Bradshaw, Cameron-Beaumont, Turner and Bateson2000). Both forms of grooming between conspecific adults/subadults in captivity have been found to maintain and strengthen social bonds (Matoba et al. Reference Matoba, Kutsukake and Hasegawa2013), with cubs being the initiators of such social behaviour (Abell et al. Reference Abell, Kirzinger, Gordon, Kirk, Kokeŝ, Lynas, Mandinyenya and Youldon2013). The performance of allogrooming reflects a positive behaviour from a welfare perspective, and it was observed in the cubs. Autogrooming was the most dominant form of grooming by the cubs. It maintains health by keeping the individual clean but also aids thermoregulation, stimulates pheromones and decreases irritation and, as such, is vital for adaption and survival (Feusner et al. Reference Feusner, Hembacher and Phillips2009). As with allogrooming, some autogrooming appears to reflect a positive emotional and behavioural state, but excessive autogrooming is indicative of a negative one. Human grooming, an obviously unnatural behaviour when compared to the wild lion, is however reflective of a bond existing between cub and keeper. Such reciprocal relationships have been known to exist between humans and farm animals and probably result in improved quality of life for both (Hemsworth Reference Hemsworth2003). There are reports of wild felids in captivity who rub and lick their human keepers, including those who, unlike lions, are solitary in behaviour (Cameron-Beaumont et al. Reference Cameron-Beaumont, Lowe and Bradshaw2002). This behaviour is strongly associated with human-reared felids (Mellen Reference Mellen1988). The reason why an animal should groom an unfamiliar human is yet to be determined. Perhaps it is a reciprocal behavioural reaction on the part of the animal who perceives the petting action as grooming or the animal soliciting favour, if it deems the human as a dominant presence. Human-associated grooming increased with human interaction frequency, which simply reflects more opportunity. Human interaction frequency did not have any effect on auto- or allogrooming.

Aggression was the least frequent behaviour category expressed by the cubs, with conspecific aggression occurring just a little more than human-directed aggression. McGlone (Reference McGlone1986) suggests that aggression can be termed an abnormal behaviour when the form of aggression is not witnessed in the wild. But Dantzer and Mormede (Reference Dantzer and Mormede1983) believe that aggressive behaviour can be considered abnormal when it results from a lack of control over the environment. Broom (Reference Broom1991) explains how fear is difficult to cope with and aggression may be an animals’ response. Aggression may also be displayed as a result of frustration when another behaviour is unable to be performed (Roper Reference Roper1984). As such, a clear understanding of an aggressive response from a cub is required when evaluating its emotional state, be it towards conspecifics or humans. Cubs at Facilities pair AB exhibited increased flight behaviour as human interaction increased. Flight was also associated with human-directed aggression, thus linking the fight and flight responses. Both are viewed as responses to a state of fear (Boissy Reference Boissy1995). Animals need to have control over their environments through choice or manipulation, as it provides them with opportunities to avoid a negative stimulus. A lack of control results in fear, eliciting a flight, fight or undesirable behavioural response (McBride Reference McBride1984). None of the lion cubs had access to a retreat space where they could retain control over their environments.

Non-nutritive sucking was the predominant form of abnormal behaviour at Facilities A and B. A behaviour typically observed in intensely farmed bovine calves, non-nutritive sucking is reduced when previous nutritive sucking bouts are for a sufficient time and when the non-nutritive sucking did not occur as a result of low feed levels, for example, when a meal was skipped (Rushen & de Passillé Reference Rushen and de Passillé1995). Milk source (cows’ milk or artificial milk) does not appear to influence non-nutritive sucking in calves (de Passillé et al. Reference de Passillé, Rushen and Janzen1997). The desire to non-nutritively suckle was strongest for 10 min after having ingested milk. The satiating effect of the non-nutritive sucking was related to metabolic hormones (de Passillé & Rushen Reference de Passillé and Rushen1997). The survival of young depends on their successful suckling and de Passillé (Reference de Passillé2001) draws the link between this strong motivation and frustration if suckling is deprived, resulting in negative impacts on welfare. Given that the act of suckling is an appetitive behaviour, it should be provided for in captivity in order to alleviate stress (Carlstead & Shepherdson Reference Carlstead, Shepherdson, Moburg and Mench2000). The performance of non-nutritive sucking in the lion cubs, indicates a desire to perform the behaviour.

Pacing is known to be the most dominant (97%) form of stereotype amongst carnivores (Clubb & Mason Reference Clubb and Mason2003) and was the most significant form of abnormal behaviour expressed by the lion cubs but was extremely variable in its possible causations. Environments which induce stereotypy reduce animal welfare. Mason (Reference Mason, Mason and Rushen2006) suggests that stereotypy behaviour be classified as either: (i) a frustration-induced behaviour which is maladaptive, in that it is performed by a normal animal responding to an abnormal environment and can be reversed; or (ii) malfunction-induced behaviours which are associated with mental pathologies and impaired central nervous system functioning. Pacing, as a very common form of abnormal repetitive behaviour, was most evident at Facility C, when the cub cohort were split into adjoining enclosures and, as such, can be classified as a frustration-induced response to separation from conspecifics. This is supported by Bashaw et al. (Reference Bashaw, Kelling, Bloomsmith and Maple2007) who state that carnivore pacing is associated with their inability to control sensory access to social partners.

Shaking and trembling, as observed by a cub at Facility B, represent a state of freeze, a defensive response not characterised by flight nor fight, as each are mutually exclusive (Eilam Reference Eilam2005). The freeze state reflects hypervigilance, associated with fear whilst a decision to then either flee or fight is being weighted up (Bracha Reference Bracha2004).

The fence-biting behaviour expressed by a cub at Facility C is an abnormal behaviour. Not all abnormal behaviours are harmful to the animal but do reveal a problem (Cooper & Mason Reference Cooper and Mason1998). This behaviour presented itself very infrequently and, should it have then developed further into a repetitive abnormal behaviour, would have been reflective of a state of ongoing frustration (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickery2007).

Animal welfare implications

The welfare of animals used in tourism activities is widely understood to be compromised, and understanding the impact of such compromise is important if their welfare is to be addressed. An important aspect to consider is if workload contributes to such welfare compromise and to what extent this is the case. Human interaction frequency with petted lion cubs, in a tourism context, impacted upon several of the cubs’ behaviours causing, principally, a decrease in resting, sleeping and play and an increase in alert behaviour, grooming of humans and flight responses. Such behavioural responses did not reflect a coping response and indicate welfare compromise, even at a lower interaction frequency.

Acknowledgements

We appreciate access to the three lion interaction facilities, whose owners opened their facilities up to this research, and also to Dr Luke Duncan for statistical assistance. The research was part of a PhD programme funded by the National Department of Higher Education and Training, South Africa, through a partnered research initiative (AQIP) with the University of South Africa (UNISA). The funders played no part in study design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; writing of the paper; and/or decision to submit to Animal Welfare.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: AW, CJCP; Data curation: AW; Formal analysis: Luke Duncan; Funding acquisition: AW; Investigation: AW; Methodology: CJCP, AW; Project administration: AW, CJCP; Supervision: CJCP; Writing – original draft: AW; Writing – review and editing: CJCP.

References

Abell, J, Kirzinger, MWB, Gordon, Y, Kirk, J, Kokeŝ, R, Lynas, K, Mandinyenya, B and Youldon, D 2013 A social network analysis of social cohesion in a constructed pride: Implications for ex situ reintroduction of the African lion (Panthera leo). PLoS ONE 8(12): e82541. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082541 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Africa Geographic 2020 Lion cub petting & captive wildlife interactions: what tourists need to know. https://africageographic.com/stories/lion-cub-petting-captive-wildlife-interactions-what-tourists-need-to-know/Google Scholar
Ahloy-Dallaire, J, Espinosa, J and Mason, G 2018 Play and optimal welfare: Does play indicate the presence of positive affective states? Behavioural Processes 156: 315. https://doi.org/101016/jbeproc201711011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allard, JS, Tizabi, Y, Shaffery, JP, Trouth, CO and Manaye, K 2004 Stereological analysis of the hypothalamic hypocretin/orexin neurons in an animal model of depression. Neuropeptides 38: 311315; https://doi.org/101016/jnpep200406004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, KC 1994 Benefits of positive human interaction for socially housed chimpanzees. Animal Welfare 13: 239245. https://doi.org/PMC2875797Google Scholar
Barde, MP and Barde, PJ 2012 What to use to express the variability of data: Standard deviation or standard error of mean? Perspectives in Clinical Research 3: 113116. https://doi.org/104103/2229-3485100662CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, SA 1958 Exploratory behaviour. British Journal of Psychology 49: 289310. https://doi.org/101111/j2044-82951958tb00667xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bashaw, MJ, Kelling, AS, Bloomsmith, MA and Maple, TL 2007 Environmental effects on the behavior of zoo-housed lions and tigers, with a case study on the effects of a visual barrier on pacingJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 10: 95109. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888700701313116Google ScholarPubMed
Bekoff, M 1977 Mammalian dispersal and the ontogeny of individual behavioral phenotypes. The American Naturalist 111: 715732. https://doi.org/101086/283201Google Scholar
Benjamini, Y and Hochberg, Y 1995 Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B57: 289300; https://wwwjstororg/stable/2346101Google Scholar
Bertram, BC 1975 Social factors influencing reproduction in wild lions. Journal of Zoology 177: 463482. https://doi.org/101111/j1469-79981975tb02246xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bixler, EO, Vgontzas, AN, Lin, HM, Calhoun, SL, Vela-Bueno, A and Kales, A 2005 Excessive daytime sleepiness in a general population sample: the role of sleep apnea age obesity diabetes and depression. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism 90: 45104515. https://doi.org/101210/jc2005-0035Google Scholar
Boissy, A 1995 Fear and fearfulness in animals. Quarterly Review in Biology 70: 165191. https://doi.org/101086/418981Google ScholarPubMed
Bradshaw, J and Cameron-Beaumont, C 2000 The signalling repertoire of the domestic cat and its undomesticated relatives. In: Turner, DC and Bateson, P (Eds.) The Domestic Cat: The Biology of its Behaviour pp 6793. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Bracha, HS 2004 Freeze flight fight fright faint: Adaptationist perspectives on the acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spectrums 9: 679685. https://doi.org/101017/S1092852900001954CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, ME, Kasper, K, van Benthem, KJ, Magnusson, A, Berg, CW, Nielsen, A, Skaug, HJ, Machler, M and Bolker, BM 2017 glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated Generalized Linear Mixed Modeling. The R Journal 9: 378400. 754. https://doi.org/103929/ethz-b-000240890Google Scholar
Broom, DM 1991 Animal welfare: concepts and measurement. Journal of Animal Science 69: 41674175. https://doi.org/102527/199169104167xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burn, CC 2017 Bestial boredom: a biological perspective on animal boredom and suggestions for its scientific investigation. Animal Behaviour 130: 141151. https://doi.org/101016/janbehav201706006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Beaumont, C Lowe, SE and Bradshaw, JW 2002 Evidence suggesting preadaptation to domestication throughout the small Felidae. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 75: 361366. https://doi.org/101046/j1095-8312200200028xGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K 2009 A comparative approach to the study of keeper-animal relationships in the zoo. Zoo Biology 28: 589608. https://doi.org/101002/zoo20289Google Scholar
Carlstead, K and Shepherdson, D 2000 Alleviating stress in zoo animals with environmental enrichment. In: Moburg, G and Mench, J (Eds.) The Biology of Animal Stress: Basic Principles and Implications for Animal Welfare pp. 337354. CABI: Wallingford, UK.Google Scholar
Clubb, R and Mason, G 2003 Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivoresNature 425: 473474. https://doi.org/10.1038/425473aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, JJ and Mason, GJ 1998 The identification of abnormal behaviour and behavioural problems in stabled horses and their relationship to horse welfare: a comparative review Equine Veterinary Journal 30: 59. https://doi.org/101111/j2042-33061998tb05136xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dantzer, R and Mormede, P 1983 Stress in farm animals: a need for reevaluation. Journal of Animal Science 57: 618. https://doi.org/102527/jas19835716CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davey, G 2006 Relationships between exhibit naturalism animal visibility and visitor interest in a Chinese zoo. Applied Animal Behavioural Science 96: 93102. https://doi.org/101016/japplanim200504018Google Scholar
Davey, G 2007 Visitors’ effects on the welfare of animals in the zoo: a review. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 10: 169183. https://doi.org/101080/10888700701313595Google ScholarPubMed
de Passillé, AM and Rushen, J 1997 Motivational and physiological analysis of the causes and consequences of non-nutritive sucking by calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53: 1531. https://doi.org/101016/S0168-1591(96)01148-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Passillé, AM, Rushen, J and Janzen, M 1997 Some aspects of milk that elicit non-nutritive sucking in the calf. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53: 167173. https://doi.org/101016/S0168-1591(96)01161-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Passillé, AM 2001 Sucking motivation and related problems in calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 72: 175187. https://doi.org/101016/S0168-1591(01)00108-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunston, EJ, Abell, J, Doyle, RE, Evershed, M and Freire, R 2016 Exploring African lion (Panthera leo) behavioural phenotypes: individual differences and correlations between sociality boldness and behaviour. Journal of Ethology 34: 277290. https://doi.org/101007/s10164-016-0473-9Google Scholar
Dunston, EJ, Abell, J, Doyle, RE, Evershed, M and Freire, R 2017 Investigating the impacts of captive origin, time and vegetation on the daily activity of African lion pridesJournal of Ethology 35: 187195. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-017-0508-xGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, MS 1980 Animal Suffering: The Science of Animal Welfare. Chapman and Hall: New York, UK.Google Scholar
Dawkins, MS 1988 Behavioural deprivation: a central problem in animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 20: 209225. https://doi.org/101016/0168-1591(88)90047-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eilam, D 2005 Die hard: a blend of freezing and fleeing as a dynamic defense—implications for the control of defensive behavior. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29: 11811191. https://doi.org/101016/jneubiorev200503027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, RD 2012 The Behavior Guide to African Mammals: Including Hoofed Mammals, Carnivores, Primates. University of California Press: CA, USA.Google Scholar
Fernandez, EJ, Tamborski, MA, Pickens, SR and Timberlake, W 2009 Animal-visitor interactions in the modern zoo: conflicts and interventions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 120: 18. https://doi.org/101016/japplanim200906002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feusner, JD, Hembacher, E and Phillips, KA 2009 The mouse who couldn’t stop washing: pathologic grooming in animals and humans. CNS Spectrums 14: 503513. https://doi.org/101017/S1092852900023567Google ScholarPubMed
Fox, J and Weisberg, S 2019 An {R} Companion to Applied Regression, Third Edition. Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA.Google Scholar
Getaway 2020 Lion cub petting and why it’s harmful. https://www.getaway.co.za/magazine-issues/15460381/Google Scholar
Gonyou, HW 1994 Why the study of animal behaviour is associated with the animal welfare issue Journal of Animal Science 72: 21712177. https://doi.org/102527/19947282171xGoogle Scholar
Gosling, SD, Kwan, VSY and John, OP 2003 A dog’s have got personality: a cross-species comparative approach to personality judgements in dogs and humans. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology 85: 11611169. https://doi.org/101037/0022-35148561161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanby, JP, Bygott, JD and Packer, C 1995 Ecology demography and behavior of lions in two contrasting habitats: Ngorongoro Crater and the Serengeti Plains. In: ARE, Sinclair and Arcese, P (Eds.) Serengeti II: Dynamics Management and Conservation of an Ecosystem pp 315331. University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA.Google Scholar
Hartmann, DP 1982 Assessing the dependability of observational data. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Science 14: 5165Google Scholar
Hayward, MW and Hayward, GJ 2006 Activity patterns of reintroduced lion (Panthera leo) and spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta) in the Addo Elephant National Park, South Africa. African Journal of Ecology 45: 135141. https://doi.org/101111/j1365-2028200600686xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hediger, H 1964 Wild Animals in Captivity. Dover: New York, USA.Google Scholar
Held, SD and Špinka, M 2011 Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81: 891899. https://doi.org/101016/janbehav201101007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemsworth, PH 2003 Human-animal interactions in livestock production. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81: 185198. https://doi.org/101016/S0168-1591(02)00280-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, LA, Johnston, SD, Lisle, AT, Keeley, T, Wong, P, Nicolson, V, Horsup, AB, Janssen, T and Phillips, CJC 2011 Behavioural and physiological responses of captive wombats (Lasiorhinus latifrons) to regular handling by humans. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Elsevier, Amsterdam, the Netherlands, 134: 217228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.07.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, G 2008 A preliminary model of human-animal relationships in the zoo. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109: 105127. https://doi.org/101016/japplanim200704013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, G and Melfi, V 2015 Are we ignoring neutral and negative human-animal relationships in zoos? Zoo Biology 34: 18. https://doi.org/101002/zoo21182Google ScholarPubMed
Hyun, Y, Ellis, M, Riskowski, G and Johnson, RW 1998 Growth performance of pigs subjected to multiple concurrent environmental stressors. Journal of Animal Science 76: 721727. https://doi.org/102527/1998763721xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jago, JG, Krohn, CC and Matthews, LR 1999 The influence of feeding and handling on the development of the human animal interactions in young cattle. Applied Animal Behavioural Science 62: 137151. https://doi.org/101016/S0168-1591(98)00219-6Google Scholar
Kreger, MD and Mench, JA 1995 Visitor-animal Interactions at the zoo. Anthrozoös 8: 143158. https://doi.org/102752/089279395787156301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLellan, A, Fureix, C, Polanco, A and Mason, G 2021 Can animals develop depression? An overview and assessment of ‘depression-like’ states. Behaviour 158: 1415. https://doi.org/101163/1568539X-bja10132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margulis, SW, Hoyos, C and Anderson, M 2003 Effect of felid activity on zoo visitor interest. Zoo Biology 22: 587599. https://doi.org/101002/zoo10115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markowitz, TM, Dally, MR, Gursky, K and Price, EO 1988 Early handling increases lamb affinity for humans. Animal Behaviour 55: 573587. https://doi.org/101006/anbe19970640Google Scholar
Mason, G 2006 Stereotypic behaviour in captive animals: fundamentals and implications for welfare and beyond. In: Mason, G and Rushen, J (Eds.) Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare, Second Edition pp 325356.CABI: Wallingford, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, G, Clubb, R, Latham, N and Vickery, S 2007 Why and how should we use environmental enrichment to tackle stereotypic behaviour? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 163188. https://doi.org/101016/japplanim200605041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matoba, T, Kutsukake, N and Hasegawa, T 2013 Head rubbing and licking reinforce social bonds in a group of captive African lions (Panthera leo). PLoS One 8: e73044. https://doi.org/101371/journalpone0073044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride, G 1984 Feral animal studies in animal science: The uses and limitations of feral animal studies to contemporary animal science. Journal of Animal Science 58: 474481. https://doi.org/102527/jas1984582474xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, PT, Reale, D, Sol, D and Reader, SM 2006 Wildlife conservation and animal temperament: causes and consequences of evolutionary change for captive reintroduced and wild populations. Animal Conservation 9: 3948. https://doi.org/101111/j1469-1795200500004xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGlone, JJ 1986 Agonistic behavior in food animals: review of research and techniques. Journal of Animal Science 62: 11301139. https://doi.org/102527/jas19866241130xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellen, JD 1988 The effects of hand-raising on sexual behaviour of captive small felids using domestic cats as a modelAmerican Association of Zoo Parks and Aquariums (AAZPA) Annual Proceedings pp 253259Washington Park ZooPortland, USA.Google Scholar
Mellen, JD 1991 Factors influencing reproductive success in small captive exotic felids (Felis spp): a multiple regression analysis. Zoo Biology 10: 95110. https://doi.org/101002/zoo1430100202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M, Burman, OH and Paul, ES 2010 An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B277: 28952904. https://doi.org/101098/rspb20100303Google Scholar
Brooks, ME, Kasper, K, van Benthem, KJ, Magnusson, A, Berg, CW, Nielsen, A, Skaug, HJ, Machler, M and Bolker, BM 2017 glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated Generalized Linear Mixed Modeling. The R Journal 9: 378400. https://doi.org/103929/ethz-b-000240890Google Scholar
Moorhouse, T, D’Cruze, NC and Macdonald, DW 2017 Unethical use of wildlife in tourism: what’s the problem who is responsible and what can be done? Journal of Sustainable Tourism 25: 505516. https://doi.org/101080/0966958220161223087Google Scholar
Ncube, S and Ndagurwa, HGT 2010 Influence of social upbringing on the activity pattern of captive lion (Panthera leo) cubs: benefits of behavior enrichment. Current Zoology 56: 389394. https://doi.org/101093/czoolo/564389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, O and Terkel, J 2009 Effects of positive reinforcement training techniques on the psychological welfare of zoo-housed chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 71: 687695. https://doi.org/101002/ajp20703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, EO 1984 Behavioural aspects of animal domestication. Quarterly Review in Biology 59: 1. https://doi.org/101086/413673CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team 2013 R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna Austria. http://wwwR-projectorg/Google Scholar
Renner, MJ 1990 Neglected aspects of exploratory and investigatory behavior. Psychobiology 18: 1622. https://doi.org/103758/BF03327209Google Scholar
Richter, SH and Hintze, S 2019. From the individual to the population–and back again? Emphasising the role of the individual in animal welfare scienceApplied Animal Behaviour Science 212: 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.12.012Google Scholar
Richter, SH, Kästner, N, Kriwet, M, Kaiser, S and Sachser, N 2016 Play matters: the surprising relationship between juvenile playfulness and anxiety in later life. Animal Behaviour 114: 261271. https://doi.org/101016/janbehav201602003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roper, TJ 1984 Response of thirsty rats to absence of water: frustration disinhibition or compensation? Animal Behaviour 32: 12251235. https://doi.org/101016/S0003-3472(84)80240-7Google Scholar
Rushen, J and de Passillé, AM 1995 The motivation of non-nutritive sucking in calves Bos taurus Animal Behaviour 49: 15031510. https://doi.org/101016/0003-3472(95)90071-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, G 1972 The Serengeti Lion. University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA.Google Scholar
Shani, A 2009 Tourists’ attitudes toward the use of animals in tourist attractions: an empirical investigation. University of Central Florida Publications: Florida, USA.Google Scholar
Sherwen, SL and Hemsworth, PH 2019 The visitor effect on zoo animals: Implications and opportunities for zoo animal welfareAnimals 9: 366. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, PK 1982 Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5: 139155. https://doi.org/101017/S0140525X0001092XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolba, A and Wood-Gush, DGM 1984 The identification of behavioural key features and their incorporation into a housing design for pigs. Annales de Recherche Veterinaire 15: 287299.Google ScholarPubMed
Suárez, P, Recuerda, P and Arias-de-Reyna, L 2017 Behaviour and welfare: The visitor effect in captive felidsAnimal Welfare 26: 2534. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.1.025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tennessen, T and Hudson, RJ 1981 Traits relevant to the domestication of herbivores Applied Animal Ethology 7: 87102. https://doi.org/101016/0304-3762(81)90054-7Google Scholar
TripAdvisor 2016 TripAdvisor announces commitment to improve wildlife welfare standards in tourism with industry-leading education effort and booking policy changes. https://irtripadvisorcom/news-releases/news-release-details/tripadvisor-announces-commitment-improve-wildlife-welfareGoogle Scholar
Veasey, JS, Waran, NK and Young, RJ 1996 On comparing the behaviour of zoo housed animals with wild conspecifics as a welfare indicator. Animal Welfare 5: 1324.Google Scholar
Wechsler, B 1995 Coping and coping strategies: a behavioural review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 43: 123134. https://doi.org/101016/0168-1591(95)00557-9Google Scholar
Weiss, DS, Horowitz, MJ and Wilner, N 1984 The Stress Response Rating Scale: A clinician’s measure for rating the response to serious life‐events. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 23: 203215. https://doi.org/101111/j2044-82601984tb00647xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F 1985 Animal boredom: is a scientific study of the subjective experiences of animals possible? In: Fox, MW and Mickley, LD (Eds.) Advances in Animal Welfare Science, Volume One. Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands. https://doi.org/101007/978-94-009-4998-0_8Google Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F 1989 Animal boredom: do animals miss what they have never known? In: Close, BS, Dolins, F and Mason, G (Eds.) Animal Use in Education, Euroniche Conference Proceedings pp 6787Humane Education Centre: London, UK.Google Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F 1991 Animal boredom: do animals miss being alert and active. Applied Animal Behaviour: Past Present and Future pp 120123. UFAW: Wheathamstead, UK.Google Scholar
Wielebnowski, NC 1999 Behavioural differences as predictors of breeding status in captive cheetahs. Zoo Biology 18: 335349. https://doi.org/101002/(SICI)1098-2361(1999)18:4<335::AID-ZOO8>30CO;2-XGoogle Scholar
Wiepkema, PR 1987 Behavioural aspects of stress. In: Wiepkema, PR and van Adrichem, PWM (Eds.) Biology of Stress in Farm Animals: An Integrative Approach pp 113133. Martinus Nijhoff: Dordrecht, The Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, A and Phillips, CJC 2021 Identification and evaluation of African lion (Panthera leo) cub welfare in wildlife‐Interaction tourism. Animals 11: 2748. https://doi.org/103390/ani11092748fGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zulkifli, I 2013 Review of human-animal interactions and their impact on animal productivity and welfare. Journal of Animal Science and Biotechnology 4: 25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. The three lion cub facilities included in the study and the differences in the management of the lion cub interactions

Figure 1

Table 2. Behaviour categories and detailed descriptors used to record duration of daily behaviour of the cubs

Figure 2

Table 3. Activity budget as % of total time (sample mean; SD; SEM) of lion cubs (n = 12) used in three wildlife tourism interaction facilities

Figure 3

Figure 1. Sample mean (± SD) percentages of time spent in behaviour categories by the lion cubs across the three interaction facilities.

Figure 4

Table 4. Wilcoxon rank sum test statistics for each pair of Facilities (AB, AC and BC) to determine significant differences across behaviour categories

Figure 5

Figure 2. Relationship between sleeping behaviour frequency of cubs and the resting behaviour frequency of cubs within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Figure 6

Figure 3. Relationship between resting behaviour frequency of cubs and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facility C.

Figure 7

Figure 4. Relationship between conspecific play behaviour frequency of cubs and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Figure 8

Figure 5. Relationship between alert behaviour frequency of cubs and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.

Figure 9

Figure 6. Relationship between flight behaviour frequency and human interaction frequency within a recording day at Facilities pair AB.