Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T02:20:12.364Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Strategy selection during exploratory behavior: sex differences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Catherine Brandner*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne
*
*Address: Institute of Psychology, SSP Faculty, Anthropole, CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland. E-mail address: Catherine.Brandner@unil.ch
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study was designed to assess sex-related differences in the selection of an appropriate strategy when facing novelty. A simple visuo-spatial task was used to investigate exploratory behavior as a specific response to novelty. The exploration task was followed by a visual discrimination task, and the responses were analyzed using signal detection theory.

During exploration women selected a local searching strategy in which the metric distance between what is already known and what is unknown was reduced, whereas men adopted a global strategy based on an approximately uniform distribution of choices. Women’s exploratory behavior gives rise to a notion of a secure base warranting a sense of safety while men’s behavior does not appear to be influenced by risk. This sex-related difference was interpreted as a difference in beliefs concerning the likelihood of uncertain events influencing risk evaluation.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2007] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Males and females seem to differ in spatial abilities and styles (e.g., Jahoda, Reference Jahoda1980; Mann, et al., Reference Mann, Sasanuma, Sakuma and Masaki1990). Generally, studies involving navigational problems showed that female cognitive style relies more on detailed information, while male style relies more on global information (e.g., Meyer-Levy, Reference Meyers-Levy, Cafferata and Tybout1989; Moffat et al., Reference Moffat, Hampson and Hatzipantelis1998; Silverman et al., Reference Silverman, Choi, Mackewn, Fisher, Moro and Olshansky2000). Evolutionary mechanisms could potentially account for sex differences in spatial behavior. For example, these behavioral differences may be due to mating patterns that induced a selection of large-range navigation in males (Gaulin & Fitzgerald, Reference Gaulin and Fitzgerald1986, Reference Gaulin and Fitzgerald1989). Mating patterns or mating strategies are linked to the dynamics of reproduction and sexual selection (Darwin, Reference Darwin1871), and sexual selection is restricted to characteristics that influence mate choice and competition for mates. Typically, males have to compete through extensive ranging for access to mates while females have to choose mating partners according to reproductive success (for reviews: Andersson, Reference Andersson1994; Trivers, Reference Trivers and Campbell1972; Geary, Reference Geary2000).

Another proposition, but exclusively directed at humans, suggested that the division of labor (game hunting and plant gathering) would have put greater selection pressure on females’ spatial memory because females sustained gathering duties (Silverman & Eals, Reference Silverman, Eals, Barkow, Cosmides and Tooby1992).

However, as argued by Ecuyer-Dab and Robert (Reference Ecuyer-Dab and Robert2004a), “the selection of male characteristics depends on females’ choice for mates. In females, however, spatial cognition would have been primarily shaped by the natural selection of a strong concern for survival (both of self and of offspring). This concern would have compelled them to favor low-risk strategies, like concentrating on proximal spatial cues, when coping with space-related problems. Such focusing would have enabled secure navigation based on detailed landmark encoding, as well as, in certain species, regular feeding based on remembering the exact locations of potential resources” (p. 222). Thus, the hypothesis of labor division would be a by-product of sexual selection and not the cause of sex differences in spatial behavior.

Taken together, the literature seems to indicate that the key to understanding the evolution of behavioral sex differences relies on the relative costs and benefits of producing offspring (see Trivers, Reference Trivers and Campbell1972, but also Geary, Reference Geary2000). In that context spatial skills play a crucial role since they increase reproductive success and the accessibility to food resource but, at the same time, multiply the risks of getting lost, being killed or consumed by other animals (predation). Hence, the survival of mobile species depends on their ability to balance costs and benefits induced by locomotion and this balancing should differ according to sex.

Experimental investigations of sex differences in spatial abilities yield apparently disparate results (e.g., Jones et al., Reference Jones, Braithwaite and Healy2003; Ecuyer-Dab & Robert, Reference Ecuyer-Dab and Robert2004a). This might be partly due to the complexity of contemporary experimental designs, but also to a lack of investigations concerning decision-making processes involved in the selection of strategies.

The current study investigates sex differences in basic behaviors like exploration, detection and discrimination involving the selection of strategies when coping with uncertainty. Following the above quotation from Ecuyer-Dab and Robert’s, the hypothesis is that women, compared to men, should favor low-risk strategies when coping with space-related problems. To test this, I used a simple spontaneous two-dimensional exploratory task. This choice relies on the fact that exploration is a natural behavior and that it is fundamental in acquiring spatial knowledge. It seems to be based on driving factors such as curiosity, comfort or mastery over one’s environment. Moreover, it is commonly defined as serving to reduce uncertainty and thus allow coping with fear (for a review about exploration, see Hughes, Reference Hughes1997).

Exploration is mainly characterized by a succession of progressions and stops (Drai, Benjamini, & Golani, Reference Drai, Benjamini and Golani2000), and the selection of exploration could rely on its capacity to act as a regulator of uncertainty. Indeed, progressions are based on decisions taken during stops, and stops correspond to choice points allowing decisions. Voss (Reference Voss, Euler and Mandl1983) refers to the exploration process as the generation and testing of hypotheses concerning the object’s meaning and potential use.

In order to assess risk-taking, a classical visual discrimination task based on the stimuli observed during the exploration task was used. The results were analyzed with signal detection theory.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

The participants were 160 volunteers (80 males and 80 females) recruited from the campus of the University of Lausanne (Switzerland). The mean age of the males and females was 22.9 (SD 1.4) and 23.5 (SD 1.2) years, respectively.

2.2 Apparatus

A set of one-hundred cards differentiated by homogenous male and female characters was used.

For Experiment 1, a two-dimensional pattern (Figure 1) was created in placing 62 cards (3 × 5 cm) on a large board (130 cm × 75 cm). Two men and two women designed the shape of the pattern to avoid any sex bias. A card was randomly chosen and duplicated to serve as goal. The remaining set of 38 cards was used for Experiment 2.

Figure 1: Two-dimensional pattern used for Experiment 1, the card placed at the lower right corner corresponds to the goal.

2.3 Procedure of testing

2.3.1 Exploration task

The board lay on the floor of the experimental room with the 62 cards turned the wrong side up. The duplicate of the goal turned right side up was put on the lower right corner of the board (Figure 1). Each participant was greeted and placed in front of the board and was asked to find the hidden goal by turning up as few pictures as possible and leaving them exposed. After having found the goal, participants were asked to turn up the remaining pictures in order to look at all characters. Double blind shuffling of cards made different strategies (e.g., systematic or random) equivalent with regard to the probability to get the goal in one draw.

2.3.2 Discrimination task

A sample of 32 new pictures of characters was added to the original sample of 62 pictures used during the exploration phase and the hundred pictures (62 already shown in Experiment 1 and 38 new) were randomly presented one by one to the participants. The participant’s task was to discriminate between the pictures already shown (signal) and the one never shown during exploration. Subjects’ responses were coded as followed: 1/ hit (present, already shown, correct response), 2/ false alarm (present, never shown, incorrect response), 3/ correct rejection (absent, never shown, correct response), 4/ miss (absent, already shown, incorrect response).

3 Results and discussion

3.1 Exploration task

Exploration was assessed through axial translations units between explored locations during the first fifteen draws. As shown in Figure 2, men adopted a global strategy in which pictures were randomly chosen from an approximately uniform distribution over spatial locations. In contrast, women used a local searching strategy in which the probability of selecting a card was a function of its distance from a cluster of visited locations. Difference in searching strategy was confirmed by a one-way ANOVA on mean units translation (F(1, 158)=372.95; p=.001) indicating that the mean unit translation was larger in men than in women (Fisher’s PLSD men women, p=.001).

Figure 2: Mean (± sem) axial translations during the first fifteen draws (mean women: 2.56 ± 0.09; mean men: 7.73 ± 0.26)

This difference in strategy is quite intriguing and might be related to risk perception. Risk can be seen here as a chance of something negative happening. In the present task, finding the goal by turning up as few picture as possible has been put at stake. Thus, the probability of not being successful on the first trial was high and risk evaluation should be high too. Early psychologists (Angell, Reference Angell1907; Fernberger, Reference Fernberger1914; Boring, Reference Boring1920) argued that uncertainty biases responses because it alters confidence, moods and temperament (insecurity or boldness for example). Analogically, exploration of unfamiliar environmental stimuli by a free moving animal has been described as competing states of both fear and curiosity (Montgomery, Reference Montgomery1955). In this case, uncertainty and fear, with respect to the external environment, appears to be reduced when the present environment is compared with previously experienced stimuli (Russell, Reference Russell1983). Another component made apparent by the study of risk assessment behavior (new object reaction) in animals was how significant it is for these animals to keep continual contact with familiar markers (Blanchard et al., Reference Blanchard, Shelton and Blanchard1970; Blanchard & Blanchard, Reference Blanchard and Blanchard1989; Misslin & Ropartz, Reference Misslin and Ropartz1981). There is a parallel between these animal behaviors and the searching strategy adopted by women. The known part of the board could be used as a secure base and minimizing the distance between this base and what is unknown could permit to feel safer while exploring the environment. Indeed it allows balanced exploration and seeking the proximity of familiar markers. Under this experimental condition, the behavior of women might be interpreted as a risk-reducing strategy allowing coping with uncertainty. To support this hypothesis, it must be added that usually, but not in this task, nearby environments are likely to be more similar in the probability of what they contain. Similarly, distant environments are less known, more uncertain in the sense of having missing information.

However, an alternative hypothesis must be considered. It has been postulated that spatial skills depend on the amount of spatial information processed according to usual home range (Gaulin and Fitzgerald, Reference Gaulin and Fitzgerald1986, Reference Gaulin and Fitzgerald1989). Thus, women’s search pattern could be in relation with this range size navigation mode hypothesis (Ecuyer-Dab & Robert, Reference Ecuyer-Dab and Robert2004b) since they have a tendency to focus on a smaller-scale space than men. Accordingly, differences observed in searching strategies could depend on differences in the selection of environmental features to guide behavior. Indeed, it has been shown that men rely primarily on cardinal references and geometrical environmental features while women rely more on topological cues such as landmarks and their configurational relations (e.g., Montello et al., Reference Montello, Lovelace, Golledge and Self1999; James & Kimura, Reference James and Kimura1997; Choi & Silverman, Reference Choi and Silverman1996).

Taken together, these hypotheses suggest that at least two main cognitive processes control exploration and spatial behavior: 1/ Information processing that extracts, selects and encodes relevant information provided by internal and external worlds; 2/ decision or choice processes allowing selecting a strategy. These two operations are altered, respectively, by attentional mechanisms that change discrimination capacities, and by beliefs concerning the likelihood of uncertain events. Information processing is tuned by the attentional level that acts like a filter on perception, while decision-making processes are weighed by subjective probability of risk. Consequently, strategies selected during exploration should be mainly controlled by these two mechanisms.

In order to investigate sex differences in these two cognitive processes, a classical visual discrimination task based on the stimuli observed during the exploration task was used.

3.2 Discrimination task

Analysis of responses showed that men said more “yes” and less “no” responses than women (multivariate analysis sex x yes: F(1, 158)=52.16; p=.001, sex x no: F(1, 158)=53.3; p=.001), but the percentage of correct recognitions, assessed through a classical coding of answers as correct and incorrect responses, was significantly higher in women than in men (F(1, 158)=35.77; p=.001; Fisher’s PLSD women men, p=.001) as shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3: A. Mean (± sem) of “yes” and “no” responses in men and women during the discrimination task. B. Mean (± sem) percentage of correct recognition by men and women during discrimination.

The analysis of the percentage of correct recognition does not allow dissociating between perception and judgment. Signal detection theory (SDT) provides a tool for quantifying perceptual decisions in the presence of uncertainty (Green & Swets, Reference Green and Swets1966). This theory treats detection of a stimulus as a decision-making process determined by the nature of the stimulus and cognitive factors. The model generally used in SDT assumes that the theoretical distributions of signal and noise are normal and have equal variance. The probability of correct and incorrect signal detection can be calculated from the ratio of the subject’s acceptation and rejection responses (Table 1). This probability is then used to determine the probit transformations aimed at estimating d’ and c (Green & Swets, Reference Green and Swets1966).

Table 1: Calculation of the probabilities of correct and incorrect detection of a signal (S= signal; N= noise; A= absent; P= present).

The d’ index correspond to the distance between means of the distributions. It measures the strength of the stimulus relative to the sensitivity of the subject. The c index corresponds to the likelihood ratio. It reflects the strategy of the subject and can be changed with level of confidence. A low c matches with a rash temperament while a high c matches with a conservative temperament.

The SDT analysis showed that the sensitivity d’ did not significantly differ between men and women (F(1, 158)=1.33; ns), but the subjective criterion c was significantly different (F(1, 158)=75.74; p=.001). According to the theory, this last result indicates that women’s decisions were, on average, more circumspect and were based on conservatism while men’s decisions were more risky, in the sense of risking false positives (Figure 4).

Figure 4: d’ and c index values (mean ± sem) measuring sensitivity and temperament in women and men.

Finally, correlations as a whole, and within women and men between the exploration measure and the bias measure (c) were calculated. As shown in Figure 5, the overall analysis revealed a significant correlation between these two measures (r(160)=.498; p=0.01). However, separate analyses conducted within each sex were not significant (women: r(80)=-.012; ns; men: r(80)=.112; ns).

Figure 5: Relation between the exploration measure and the bias measure in women and men. Exploration measure corresponds to the mean translation unit observed during the exploration task, whereas the bias measure corresponds to the c index of the discrimination task.

4 General discussion

This study has first shown that the searching strategies used by women and men to solve an exploratory task that may be seek as involving trading off of risk and reward differed according to sex. Women adopted a local searching strategy in which the metric distance between what is already known and what is unknown was reduced. Men adopted a global strategy based on an approximately uniform distribution of choices. These findings appear to be compatible with a female frame of mind expressing careful consideration of all circumstances and possible consequences before making a decision.

The results of the discrimination task through a signal detection analysis showed that women and men sensitivity towards the stimuli (d’ index) was equivalent while the assessment of confidence in judgment under uncertainty (c index) differed. Indeed, men showed a subjective criterion indicating a more risky temperament than women. The absence of differences in sensitivity contrasted with what is generally expected from results showing that women outperform men when the task requires object identification (Silverman & Eals, Reference Silverman, Eals, Barkow, Cosmides and Tooby1992; James & Kimura, Reference James and Kimura1997) or item memory (Galea & Kimura, Reference Galea and Kimura1993, Maccoby & Jacklin, Reference Maccoby and Jacklin1974, Stumpf & Jackson, Reference Stumpf and Jackson1994). If correlation as a whole supported the hypothesis that strategies selected during exploration rely on temperament, it was not the case for correlations within each group.

It could be argued that these measures were not based on the same cognitive mechanism. Indeed, subjects were facing with the unknown in the exploration task while having to decide if they had already seen or not a picture in the discrimination task. Thus, these two tasks might not be equivalent according to uncertainty but also according to one’s confidence in skills. For example, LaGrone (Reference LaGrone1969) reported that women more often felt disoriented with regard to their “sense of orientation” than men did. Moreover, women expressed a feeling of “worry about becoming lost” that was negatively associated with confidence in the “sense of orientation” (Kozlowski & Bryant, Reference Kozlowski and Bryant1977). It has also been shown that the positive representation of an activity in term of payoffs and costs influenced women’s favorable judgments concerning consequences (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Jenkins and Glaser2006). Interestingly, variations in the relation between exploratory behavior and risk-taking have been also observed in birds (Parus major). In this species, correlation between exploratory behavior and risk-taking behavior depended on the presence and foraging behavior of a mate. Moreover, the presence of a mate resulted in a complex differential effect according to the fact that they were males or females and slow or fast explorers. Slow explorers of both sexes were more audacious in the presence of a mate whereas in fast explorers, females were less audacious in the presence of a mate (van Oers et al., Reference van Oers, Klunder and Drent2005).

The results of this experiment, together with those of an abundant literature, cannot lead to a clear understanding of why and how the mentioned abilities are associated with beliefs concerning the likelihood of information and degrees of confidence consistently with sex differences. One possibility to be examined is that sex-steroid hormones could modulate cognitive mechanisms involved in risk evaluation.

Finally, it is perhaps of some interest to note that the present work was drawn from Ittelson (Reference Ittelson1960) suggestion that “the problem of exploratory behavior... [is] central to the study of environmental perception.”

It differs from previous works since it offers a new perspective to assess sex-related differences in spatial behavior through its relation to perception. Perception and sensation should be regarded as particular forms of extended communicative actions and not as alternative end points for the use of environmental information (Green, Reference Green2001). All information about the world comes from our sensory systems, and the cognitive process begins with the ability to make some meaning out of this stream of sensory stimuli.

Footnotes

*

Thanks are due to Jason Borioli for careful reading.

References

Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angell, F. (1907). On judgment of “like” in discrimination experiments. American Journal of Psychology, 18, 253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, R. J., Shelton, V. T. and Blanchard, D. C. (1970). Historical effects of stimulus exposure: Readiness to eat and object exploration. Learning and Motivation, 1, 432-444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, R. J. and Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Attack and defense in rodents as ethoexperimental models for the study of emotion. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 13, 3-14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1920). The control of attitude in psychophysical experiments. Psychological Review, 27, 440-452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, J., and Silverman, I. (1996). Sexual dimorphism in spatial behaviors: applications to route learning. Evolution and Cognition, 2, 165-171.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Drai, I., Benjamini, Y., Golani, I. (2000). Statistical discrimination of natural modes of motion in rat exploratory behavior. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 96, 119-131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ecuyer-Dab, I. and Robert, M. (2004a). Have sex differences in spatial ability evolved from male competition for mating and female concern for survival? Cognition, 91, 221-257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ecuyer-Dab, I., and Robert, M. (2004b). Spatial ability and home range size: examining the relationship in Western men and women. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(2), 217-231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernberger, S. W. (1914). The effects of attitude of the subject upon the measure of sensitivity. American Journal of Psychology, 25, 538-543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galea, L. A. M. and Kimura, D. (1993). Sex differences in route-learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 53-65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaulin, S. J. C. and Fitzgerald, R. W. (1989). Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability. Animal Behaviour, 37, 322-331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaulin, S. J. C., and Fitzgerald, R. W. (1986). Sex differences in spatial ability: An evolutionary hypothesis and test. The American Naturalist, 127(1), 74-88.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55-77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, P. R. (2001). The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? Ecological Psychology, 13, 117-122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. M. and Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Harris, C. R., Jenkins, M. and Glaser, D. (2006). Gender differences in risk assessment: why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgement and Decision making, 1, 48-63.Google Scholar
Hughes, R. N. (1997). Intrinsic exploration in animals: motives and measurement. Behavioural Processes, 41, 213-226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ittelson, W. H. (1960). Visual space perception. Springer, New York, p13.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1980). Sex and ethnic differences on a spatial perceptual task: Some hypotheses tested. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 425-431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, T. W. and Kimura, D. (1997). Sex differences in remembering the location of objects in an array: location-shifts versus location-exchanges. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 155-163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, C. M., Braithwaite, V. A. and Healy, S. D. (2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 403-411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozlowski, L. T. and Bryant, K. J. (1977). Sense of direction, spatial orientation, and cognitive maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology (Human Perception), 3, 590-598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaGrone, G. W. (1969). Sex and personality differences in relation to feeling for direction. Journal of General Psychology, 81, 23-33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccoby, E. E. and Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA. Standford University Press.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., Sasanuma, S., Sakuma, N. and Masaki, S. (1990). Sex differences in cognitive abilities: a crosscultural perspective. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1063-1077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyers-Levy, J. (1989). Gender differences in information processing: a selectivity hypothesis. In Cafferata, P. C. and Tybout, A. M. (Eds). Cognitive and affective responses to advertising. Lexington Books, 219-260.Google Scholar
Misslin, R. and Ropartz, P. (1981). Effects of metamphetamine on novelty-seeking behavior by mice. Psychopharmacology, 75, 39-43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., and Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a “virtual” maze: sex differences and correlations with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 73-87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, K. C. (1955). The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 48, 254-260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montello, D. R., Lovelace, K. L., Golledge, R. G., and Self, C. M. (1999). Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 89, 515-534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Oers, K., Klunder, M., Drent, P. J. (2005). Context dependence of personalities: risk-taking behavior in a social and a nonsocial situation. Behavioral Ecology, 10.1093(ari054), 716-723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1983). Pancultural aspects of the human conceptual organization of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1281-1288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silverman, I., Choi, J., Mackewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J., and Olshansky, E. (2000). Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding: further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 201-213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, I. and Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (Eds), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology & generation of culture. NYC: Oxford Universities Press, 533-549.Google Scholar
Stumpf, H. and Jackson, D. N. (1994). Gender-related differences in cognitive abilities: evidence from a medical school admission testing program. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 335-344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Paternal investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871–1971 (pp. 136-179). Chicago, Aldine.Google Scholar
Voss, H.-G. (1983). Neugier und Exploration. In Euler, H. A., Mandl, H., (Hrsg.), Emotionspsychologie: Ein Handbuch in Schlüsselbegriffen, 220-226. Urban & Schwarzenberg, München.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1: Two-dimensional pattern used for Experiment 1, the card placed at the lower right corner corresponds to the goal.

Figure 1

Figure 2: Mean (± sem) axial translations during the first fifteen draws (mean women: 2.56 ± 0.09; mean men: 7.73 ± 0.26)

Figure 2

Figure 3: A. Mean (± sem) of “yes” and “no” responses in men and women during the discrimination task. B. Mean (± sem) percentage of correct recognition by men and women during discrimination.

Figure 3

Table 1: Calculation of the probabilities of correct and incorrect detection of a signal (S= signal; N= noise; A= absent; P= present).

Figure 4

Figure 4: d’ and c index values (mean ± sem) measuring sensitivity and temperament in women and men.

Figure 5

Figure 5: Relation between the exploration measure and the bias measure in women and men. Exploration measure corresponds to the mean translation unit observed during the exploration task, whereas the bias measure corresponds to the c index of the discrimination task.