Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T00:15:37.470Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Familial risk for major depression: differential white matter alterations in healthy and depressed participants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 September 2022

Alexandra Winter
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Katharina Thiel
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Susanne Meinert
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany Institute of Translational Neuroscience, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Hannah Lemke
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Lena Waltemate
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Fabian Breuer
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Regina Culemann
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Julia-Katharina Pfarr
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Frederike Stein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Katharina Brosch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Tina Meller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Kai Gustav Ringwald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Florian Thomas-Odenthal
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Andreas Jansen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Igor Nenadić
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Axel Krug
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
Jonathan Repple
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Nils Opel
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Katharina Dohm
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Elisabeth J. Leehr
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Dominik Grotegerd
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Harald Kugel
Affiliation:
University Clinic for Radiology, University of Muenster, Münster, Germany
Tim Hahn
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB), University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University Giessen, Marburg, Germany
Udo Dannlowski*
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Udo Dannlowski, E-mail: udo.dannlowski@uni-muenster.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Major depressive disorder (MDD) has been associated with alterations in brain white matter (WM) microstructure. However, diffusion tensor imaging studies in biological relatives have presented contradicting results on WM alterations and their potential as biomarkers for vulnerability or resilience. To shed more light on associations between WM microstructure and resilience to familial risk, analyses including both healthy and depressed relatives of MDD patients are needed.

Methods

In a 2 (MDD v. healthy controls, HC) × 2 (familial risk yes v. no) design, we investigated fractional anisotropy (FA) via tract-based spatial statistics in a large well-characterised adult sample (N = 528), with additional controls for childhood maltreatment, a potentially confounding proxy for environmental risk.

Results

Analyses revealed a significant main effect of diagnosis on FA in the forceps minor and the left superior longitudinal fasciculus (ptfce−FWE = 0.009). Furthermore, a significant interaction of diagnosis with familial risk emerged (ptfce−FWE = 0.036) Post-hoc pairwise comparisons showed significantly higher FA, mainly in the forceps minor and right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, in HC with as compared to HC without familial risk (ptfce−FWE < 0.001), whereas familial risk played no role in MDD patients (ptfce−FWE = 0.797). Adding childhood maltreatment as a covariate, the interaction effect remained stable.

Conclusions

We found widespread increased FA in HC with familial risk for MDD as compared to a HC low-risk sample. The significant effect of risk on FA was present only in HC, but not in the MDD sample. These alterations might reflect compensatory neural mechanisms in healthy adults at risk for MDD potentially associated with resilience.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a debilitating, life-quality diminishing, and often recurring mental disorder with a lifetime prevalence ranging from 10% to 29.9% (Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, Reference Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Wittchen2012; for a meta-analysis, see Lim et al. Reference Lim, Tam, Lu, Ho, Zhang and Ho2018). Besides examining the course and symptomatology of the disorder itself, it is of high clinical relevance to identify risk factors contributing to the development of depressive psychopathology. Family history of MDD is a consistently replicated risk factor: Genetic approaches involving twin studies estimate a heritability of 30–40% (Kendler, Gatz, Gardner, & Pedersen, Reference Kendler, Gatz, Gardner and Pedersen2006; Sullivan, Michael Neale, & Kendler, Reference Sullivan, Michael Neale and Kendler2000) and one of the largest genome-wide association studies has recently highlighted the complex genetic architecture of depression by identifying more than 200 genes associated with MDD (Howard et al. Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019). Moreover, parental depression increases the risk to develop MDD threefold, and was associated with a higher probability of recurring episodes, mortality and overall poorer functioning as compared to offspring of non-depressed parents (Hammen, Burge, Burney, & Adrian, Reference Hammen, Burge, Burney and Adrian1990; Weissman et al. Reference Weissman, Berry, Warner, Gameroff, Skipper, Talati and Wickramaratne2016a, Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad and Talati2016b). The period between 15 and 25 years of age is most critical to develop MDD for individuals with familial risk, but also without familial risk, as revealed in the 30-year follow-up study (Weissman et al. Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad and Talati2016b). Not only offspring, but first-degree relatives of MDD patients in general exhibit elevated rates of MDD as compared to individuals without a familial predisposition (Klein, Lewinsohn, Seeley, & Rohde, Reference Klein, Lewinsohn, Seeley and Rohde2001).

Importantly, this familial risk entails environmental stressors, as genetic risk and in some cases, the emotionally and socially strenuous environment among depressed relatives, jointly contribute to psychopathology in offspring (Goodman & Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999) by, for instance, emotionally neglectful upbringing and other forms of childhood maltreatment (Lovejoy, Graczyk, O'Hare, & Neuman, Reference Lovejoy, Graczyk, O'Hare and Neuman2000; Pawlby, Hay, Sharp, Waters, & Pariante, Reference Pawlby, Hay, Sharp, Waters and Pariante2011).

Yet, not all healthy individuals exposed to depression in their familial surrounding manifest clinically relevant depressive symptoms, but are resilient to this risk. Resilience is understood as the dynamic ability to maintain well-being and mental health by activating protective resources, even when facing adversity, i.e., suffering, discomfort or a potentially traumatic event in life (Sisto et al. Reference Sisto, Vicinanza, Campanozzi, Ricci, Tartaglini and Tambone2019; Windle, Reference Windle2011). However, little is known regarding neurobiological differences between resilient individuals exposed to familial risk, and exposed individuals who developed MDD themselves. On the one hand, it could be speculated that familial risk exposure shapes brain structure and function towards alterations found in patient samples, suggesting risk-associated markers of vulnerability. On the other hand, the opposite might be the case, suggesting (over-)compensatory mechanisms related to resilience in exposed persons.

While some neuroimaging studies rather point towards a structural and functional neurobiological similarity of at-risk individuals and MDD patients, e.g., regarding reduced hippocampal volume (Amico et al. Reference Amico, Meisenzahl, Koutsouleris, Reiser, Möller and Frodl2011; Peterson et al. Reference Peterson, Warner, Bansal, Zhu, Hao, Liu and Weissman2009); and alterations in emotion-processing circuits (e.g. Gotlib, Joormann, & Foland-Ross, Reference Gotlib, Joormann and Foland-Ross2014; Opel et al. Reference Opel, Redlich, Grotegerd, Dohm, Zaremba, Meinert and Dannlowski2017), others have provided evidence for correlates of resilience by increased grey matter volume in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Brosch et al. Reference Brosch, Stein, Meller, Schmitt, Yuksel, Ringwald and Krug2021). Despite the soaring awareness that differences in white matter (WM) fibre structure seem to be play a role in the pathophysiology of MDD (Murphy & Frodl, Reference Murphy and Frodl2011; van Velzen et al. Reference van Velzen, Kelly, Isaev, Aleman, Aftanas, Bauer and Schmaal2020), examining WM microstructure alterations in participants with familial risk has been neglected. The superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) is a major WM tract connecting the frontal lobe with posterior regions (Makris et al. Reference Makris, Kennedy, McInerney, Sorensen, Wang, Caviness and Pandya2005). While its function has not been fully established yet, it has been associated with depression (Murphy & Frodl, Reference Murphy and Frodl2011), emotion processing (Koshiyama et al. Reference Koshiyama, Fukunaga, Okada, Morita, Nemoto, Yamashita and Hashimoto2020), and working memory (Karlsgodt et al. Reference Karlsgodt, van Erp, Poldrack, Bearden, Nuechterlein and Cannon2008). Other tracts which have been associated with depression in a large meta-analysis are the corpus callosum, which connects the two hemispheres, and the corona radiata, a segment of the limbic-thalamo-cortical circuitry involved in emotion regulation (van Velzen et al. Reference van Velzen, Kelly, Isaev, Aleman, Aftanas, Bauer and Schmaal2020). However, the authors point towards a structural disconnectivity in depressed individuals as the revealed alterations in WM microstructure in adults with MDD are rather widespread. Alterations in these tracts can be observed through magnetic resonance diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), which presents a non-invasive technique investigating WM architecture in vivo based on the tissue water diffusion rate. Most frequently studied is fractional anisotropy (FA), which is regarded as a quantitative index of WM coherence. FA quantifies directional diffusion from zero ( = isotropic) to one ( = anisotropic/constrained along one axis). High FA values are interpreted as revealing a highly organised and normally myelinated axon structure. In contrast, decreased FA might represent reduced coherence in the main preferred diffusion direction and accordingly reflect WM dysfunction.

To the best of our knowledge, only few DTI studies, with small sample sizes, examined participants with familial risk. On the one hand, there is evidence of decreased FA in at-risk participants, pointing towards family history as a risk factor (Bracht, Linden, & Keedwell, Reference Bracht, Linden and Keedwell2015; Huang, Fan, Williamson, & Rao, Reference Huang, Fan, Williamson and Rao2011; Keedwell et al. Reference Keedwell, Chapman, Christiansen, Richardson, Evans and Jones2012), implying that healthy at-risk groups resemble diagnosed patients on a neurobiological level. On the other hand, Frodl et al. (Reference Frodl, Carballedo, Fagan, Lisiecka, Ferguson and Meaney2012) provided evidence for greater FA in healthy controls (HC) at-risk as compared to low-risk HC. This study did not include MDD patients, but the results suggest a compensation mechanism which might be further used to distinguish healthy at-risk participants from already-affected at-risk MDD patients.

In sum, there are contradictory reports on WM alterations associated with familial risk, and a lack of DTI studies which analyse (1) healthy as well as depressed individuals (2) with and without familial risk for MDD, (3) including adult, not only adolescent participants, (4) while controlling for the closely related, and therefore potentially confounding, factor environmental risk, as previous studies have found distinct WM correlates of childhood maltreatment (Frodl et al. Reference Frodl, Carballedo, Fagan, Lisiecka, Ferguson and Meaney2012; Huang, Gundapuneedi, & Rao, Reference Huang, Gundapuneedi and Rao2012; Meinert et al. Reference Meinert, Repple, Nenadic, Krug, Jansen, Grotegerd and Dannlowski2019). Therefore, the objective of the present study was to investigate the familial risk for MDD and its associations with WM microstructure in a large well-characterised sample of HC and MDD with additional consideration of childhood maltreatment. Based on multiple previous findings (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Gundapuneedi and Rao2012; Meinert et al. Reference Meinert, Repple, Nenadic, Krug, Jansen, Grotegerd and Dannlowski2019; Repple et al. Reference Repple, Mauritz, Meinert, de Lange, Grotegerd, Opel and van den Heuvel2020; van Velzen et al. Reference van Velzen, Kelly, Isaev, Aleman, Aftanas, Bauer and Schmaal2020), we expect a main effect of diagnosis, more specifically, reduced FA in MDD compared with HC, specifically in the left SLF. We further expect that healthy relatives of MDD patients might show adaptation on a neural level, i.e., increased FA, despite the familial risk as they have already entered adulthood and passed the critical age range in adolescence according to findings by Weissman et al. (Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad and Talati2016b). Lastly, we explore the effects of childhood maltreatment as an environmental component of familial risk by correcting our findings for the degree of self-reported maltreatment experiences.

Materials and methods

Participants

The present sample was drawn from the bicentric Marburg-Münster Affective Cohort Study (MACS/FOR2107-cohort) which has been previously described in more detail elsewhere (Kircher et al. Reference Kircher, Wöhr, Nenadic, Schwarting, Schratt, Alferink and Dannlowski2019; see Vogelbacher et al. Reference Vogelbacher, Möbius, Sommer, Schuster, Dannlowski, Kircher and Bopp2018, for the MRI quality assurance protocol). It consisted of N = 528 participants (N = 401 female, Mage = 31.26, s.d.age = 11.72), aged from 18 to 65 years with West-European ancestry; N = 262 HC compared with N = 266 patients with a lifetime diagnosis of MDD. Furthermore, we divided these two groups into two similarly-sized risk and low-risk groups, respectively: N = 129 HC (HCr) and N = 132 MDD with familial risk for MDD (MDDlr), and N = 133 HC (HClr) and N = 134 MDD without (MDDlr) (see online Supplement S1 for further information on sample selection). We operationalised familial risk as reporting at least one first-degree relative (biological mother, father, siblings and/or children) with a known history of past or current psychological treatment due to a diagnosis of MDD, while no other primary psychiatric diagnoses were allowed in these relatives. In contrast, we only included participants in the low-risk groups (HClr and MDDlr), if, reportedly, no first-degree relative had ever been diagnosed with a mental disorder.

Recruitment was implemented through psychiatric hospitals, newspapers and flyers. All groups were matched for age, site (Marburg/Münster), sex and years of education using ‘MatchIt’ in R® (2020, Version 4.0.1) and hence did not differ significantly with respect to these variables (Table 1). Participants were excluded if there was any history of neurological (e.g. concussion, stroke, tumour, neuro-inflammatory diseases) or medical (e.g. cancer, chronic inflammatory or heart diseases) conditions, or substance dependence.

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the total sample (N = 528)

Abbreviations: HC, healthy controls; MDD, major depressive disorder; HClr, low-risk HC; HCr, at-risk HC; MDDlr, low-risk MDD; MDDr, at-risk MDD; CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; SIGH-ADS, Structured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale with Atypical Depression online Supplement.

a χ2-test.

b One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) assuming equal variance.

c t test for independent samples, assuming unequal variance.

d t test for independent samples, assuming equal variance.

e Data not available for one MDD participant.

f Data not available for 14 participants.

Note. Numbers represent respective n, or Mean ± Standard Deviation.

*** p < 0.001, two-tailed.

The Structured Clinical Interview (SCID; Wittchen, Wunderlich, Grushwitz, & Zaudig, Reference Wittchen, Wunderlich, Grushwitz and Zaudig1997) was employed by trained personnel to assess whether participants fulfilled standardised criteria defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) for a lifetime diagnosis of MDD. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961) was administered additionally to assess the presence and severity of current (subclinical) depressive symptomatology, while all HCs with BDI score ⩾ 10, considering the cut-off for none or minimal depression, were excluded (Beck, Steer, & Carbin, Reference Beck, Steer and Carbin1988). HCr v. HClr as well as MDDr v. MDDlr did not differ significantly in BDI scores (Table 1).

For an overview of antidepressant medication intake, we calculated a Medication Load Index (MedIndex) for every MDD participant, i.e., the sum of absent medication ( = 0), equal or lower than the average dose ( = 1), or higher than the average dose ( = 2) for each psychopharmacological agent. This is an established method (Hassel et al. Reference Hassel, Almeida, Kerr, Nau, Ladouceur, Fissell and Phillips2008; Redlich et al. Reference Redlich, Almeida, Grotegerd, Opel, Kugel, Heindel and Dannlowski2014) considering the active ingredient and the daily dose intake recommended by the Physician's Desk Reference (Reynolds, Reference Reynolds2008). Since age of onset of MDD (van Velzen et al. Reference van Velzen, Kelly, Isaev, Aleman, Aftanas, Bauer and Schmaal2020) and lifetime cumulative duration of depressive episodes (De Diego-Adeliño et al. Reference De Diego-Adeliño, Pires, Gómez-Ansón, Serra-Blasco, Vives-Gilabert, Puigdemont and Portella2014) have been shown to be significantly associated with disruption of WM, it is important to note that the two MDD groups did not differ significantly with respect to these variables. Furthermore, MedIndex, remission status, and recurrency of episodes were evenly distributed across the two groups (Table 2).

Table 2. Clinical characteristics and medication in the MDD sample (n = 266)

Abbreviations: MDD, major depressive disorder; MDDlr, low-risk MDD; MDDr, at-risk MDD; MedIndex, Medication Load Index.

a χ2-test (two-tailed).

b Data available for n = 229.

c t test for independent samples (two-tailed), assuming equal variance.

d Data available for n = 255.

e Only available for patients in their acute stage, n = 158.

f Data missing for one participant g t test for independent samples (two-tailed), assuming unequal variance.

Note. Numbers represent respective n, or Mean ± Standard Deviation.

Concerning childhood maltreatment, HCr v. HClr as well as MDDr v. MDDlr, respectively, did not differ significantly (all ps > 0.05) in the total score of the German version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Wingenfeld et al. Reference Wingenfeld, Spitzer, Mensebach, Grabe, Hill, Gast and Driessen2010) (Table 1).

The study protocol for this cohort was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculties, University of Marburg (AZ: 07/14) and University of Münster (AZ: 2014-422-b-S). The procedure was performed in accordance with the ethical guidelines and regulations. All participants provided written informed consent and received financial compensation for participation.

DTI data acquisition

All participants were examined on a 3 T whole body MRI scanner (Marburg: Tim Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany; Münster: Prisma fit, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a GRAPPA acceleration factor of 2 and identical sequence parameters for both sites: Fifty-six axial slices, 2.5-mm thick with no gap, were measured with cubic voxels of 2.5 mm edge length (TE = 90 ms, TR = 7300 ms). Five non-diffusion-weighted (DW) images (b0 = 0) and 2 × 30 DW images with a b-value of 1000 s/mm2 for spatial directions were acquired. These scan parameters were consistent over the two sites. GRE field mapping: 2:01; DTI 1 & 2 each 4:10; DTI b0 3x 0:31; reverse phase encoding 0:53; in total, the DTI acquisition lasted 10 min and 46 s.

Image processing

Preprocessing and analyses were conducted with FSL6.0.1 [http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/, FMRIB, Oxford Center for Functional MRI of the Brain, University of Oxford, Department of Clinical Neurology, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, United Kingdom (Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich, & Smith, Reference Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich and Smith2012; Smith et al. Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Matthews2004; Woolrich et al. Reference Woolrich, Jbabdi, Patenaude, Chappell, Makni, Behrens and Smith2009)]. DTI image processing methods and quality controls were already described in Meinert et al. (Meinert et al. Reference Meinert, Repple, Nenadic, Krug, Jansen, Grotegerd and Dannlowski2019). Briefly, the DW images were corrected for motion artefacts and eddy currents with FSL's ‘eddy’ (Andersson & Sotiropoulos, Reference Andersson and Sotiropoulos2016), with a subsequent rotation of b-vectors. As reference for alignment, the first b0 was used after automated skull stripping. For this purpose, FMRIB's brain extraction tool (Smith, Reference Smith2002) was applied prior to fitting a diffusion tensor model at each voxel using ‘DTIFIT’ within FMRIB's Diffusion Toolbox (FDT) (Behrens et al. Reference Behrens, Woolrich, Jenkinson, Johansen-Berg, Nunes, Clare and Smith2003). For quality assurance of our data, the open-source software DTIPrep (Oguz et al. Reference Oguz, Farzinfar, Matsui, Budin, Liu, Gerig and Styner2014) was used with default options with an automatic pipeline. Gradients failing checks for intensity-related artefacts are deleted. Intensity artefacts are gradients with a large deviation from the mean of all gradients and were subsequently deleted. As a result, affected images up until a threshold of ⩽ 20% were deleted, in which case the participant was excluded from further analyses. This resulted in a number of 64.32 images on average for our sample [s.d. = 1.31, range: (54–65)].

FA is the most frequently reported measure of diffusion in DTI studies besides mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD). We focus on FA with the intention of making our results comparable to previous studies in this area. Our analyses on MD, RD, and AD are available in more detail in the online Supplements. These additional measures contribute to findings of FA providing information about tissue microstructure alterations. MD is a parameter of overall diffusion, averaged across the three eigenvalues of the diffusion tensor, frequently denoted as the apparent diffusion coefficient. RD reflects diffusion radial to the axons, whereas AD is associated with diffusion parallel to the axons (Alexander, Lee, Lazar, & Field, Reference Alexander, Lee, Lazar and Field2007; Feldman, Yeatman, Lee, Barde, & Gaman-Bean, Reference Feldman, Yeatman, Lee, Barde and Gaman-Bean2010).

Analyses

Demographic and clinical data were analysed with the Statistical Package for Social sciences (IBM SPSS Statistic 27; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Johansen-Berg, Rueckert, Nichols, Mackay and Behrens2006) were applied for the reduction of partial volume effects and registration misalignments. We corrected for multiple comparisons with Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) with 5000 permutations. Additionally, estimated cluster sizes were corrected for the family-wise error (FWE) at p < 0.05. Since data were acquired with two different MRI scanners, and due to a body coil exchange in Marburg, two dummy-coded variables (Marburg pre body-coil: yes/no, Marburg post body-coil: yes/no) with Münster as reference category were created (Vogelbacher et al. Reference Vogelbacher, Möbius, Sommer, Schuster, Dannlowski, Kircher and Bopp2018).

In a first step, analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were conducted in FSL with FA (as well as AD, RD and MD, respectively) as dependent variables, and diagnosis as well as state of familial risk as independent variables to estimate the main effect of diagnosis and familial risk as well as their interaction. Nuisance variables were age, sex, total intracranial volume (TIV), Marburg pre body-coil, and Marburg post body-coil. We investigated the main effect of diagnosis, and the main effect of risk as well as the interaction effect of diagnosis × risk (F tests). In case of significant results, post-hoc pairwise t tests were calculated to investigate individual group differences and the direction of the effects. For analyses in FSL, effect sizes were calculated based on the t value of the peak voxel provided by FSL and respective sample sizes according to Cooper, Hedges, & Valentine (Reference Cooper, Hedges and Valentine2009).

Furthermore, we conducted accessory analyses and robustness checks: In two sub-analyses, we excluded (a) participants under the age of 26, aiming to confirm results in a sample which has presumably passed the critical age range for developing MDD according to Weissman et al. (Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad and Talati2016b), and (b) participants of or above the age of 60 to rule out effects of age on FA (Salat et al. Reference Salat, Tuch, Greve, Van Der Kouwe, Hevelone, Zaleta and Dale2005; Sexton et al. Reference Sexton, Walhovd, Storsve, Tamnes, Westlye, Johansen-Berg and Fjell2014; Walhovd et al. Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Reinvang, Lundervold, Dale, Eilertsen and Fischl2005; Westlye et al. Reference Westlye, Walhovd, Dale, Bjørnerud, Due-Tønnessen, Engvig and Fjell2010). Next, we conducted control analyses for potential effects of outliers using SPSS by extracting individual mean FA values of the significant cluster from the main analysis. In the total sample, we examined outliers with Cook's distance > 3 s.d. and recalculated the identical general linear model (GLM) in SPSS. Furthermore, we investigated the influence of childhood maltreatment including CTQSum as a covariate. For analyses in SPSS, given effect sizes in η 2p were converted into Cohen's d for comparability. In addition, we conducted an accessory sensitivity analysis in which we only included participants in the risk groups whose parents had been treated for MDD (online Supplement S5).

Results

Main analysis: ANCOVA with main effect of diagnosis and risk, and their interaction

A significant main effect of diagnosis was found (p tfce-FWE = 0.021, total k = 1774 voxels in 3 clusters, peak voxel of a largest cluster: x = 27, y = −23, z = 16). A post-hoc t test revealed that individuals from both HC groups had higher FA compared with both MDD groups in the forceps minor (FM) and the left SLF with a moderate effect size (d = 0.40, p tfce-FWE = 0.009, total k = 19 792 voxels in 4 clusters, peak voxel of largest cluster: x = 24, y = −20, z = 6, Fig. 1). Significant effects were also found on RD (p tfce-FWE = 0.033) but not on MD (p tfce-FWE = 0.314), with increased RD in MDD patients (online Supplement S2). The main effect of familial risk was not significant neither for FA (p tfceFWE > 0.265), nor RD (p tfce-FWE > 0.129), nor MD (p tfce-FWE > 0.125). However, the interaction of diagnosis × familial risk yielded significant results (ptfce− FWE = 0.036, total k = 9297 voxels in 5 clusters, peak voxel of largest cluster: x = 26, y = −21, z = 28). Clusters predominantly in the left corticospinal tract (CST) and the bilateral SLF were affected by altered FA (Fig. 2a). The effect was also significant for MD (p tfce-FWE = 0.042) and RD (p tfce-FWE = 0.008) (online Supplement S2). No significant main or interaction effects were found for AD (all p tfce-FWE > 0.115).

Fig. 1. Affected white matter tracts by effect of diagnosis (HC > MDD).

Note. Increased FA in healthy controls as compared to patients with major depressive disorder, mainly in the forceps minor and superior longitudinal fasciculus, p tfce-FWE = 0.009. In order to illustrate the effects on the FMRIB58 template (visualised in green) for all three sectional views in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Atlas coordinate system, the mean FA value was obtained from FA values of all significant voxels (p tfce−FWE < 0.05). Red-yellow areas represent voxels in significant clusters, using the ‘fill’ command in FSL. The colour bar indicates the probability of a voxel being a member of the different labelled regions within the JHU-atlas, averaged over all the voxels in the significant mask (p tfce−FWE < 0.05). MNI coordinates of selected plane: x = 26, y = −19, z = 10. FM, forceps minor; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus

Figure 2. Interaction effect of diagnosis and familial risk, and effect of familial risk in HC.

Note. Affected white matter tracts by A: Interaction effect of diagnosis × familial risk, ptfce− FWE = 0.036. B: Familial risk in HC. Widespread increased FA in HCr as compared to HClr, ptfce− FWE < 0.001. Effects illustrated on the FMRIB58 template (visualised in green) in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Atlas coordinate system. Red-yellow areas represent voxels in significant clusters, using the ‘fill’ command in FSL. The colour bar indicates the probability of a voxel being a member of the different labelled regions within the JHU-atlas, averaged over all the voxels in the significant mask (p tfce−FWE < 0.05). MNI coordinates of selected plane for A and B: x = 37, y = −37, z = 7.C: Significant increase in FA in HCr as compared to all other groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. Asterisk represents statistical significance (ptfce−FWE < 0.05) in post-hoc-t-tests. The mean FA value was obtained from FA values of all significant voxels (p tfce−FWE < 0.05). FM, Forceps Minor; IFOF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus.

Post-hoc pairwise t tests revealed a significant difference between the two HC groups, with widespread increased FA values in the HCr group (ptfce− FWE < 0.001, total k = 41 494 voxels in one cluster, peak voxel: x = 37, y = −37, z = 12), mainly in the FM and right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), with a moderate effect size (d = 0.59) (Fig. 2b). Furthermore, HCr also had significantly higher FA values compared to the MDDr and MDDlr group (MDDr: ptfce− FWE < 0.001, total k = 42 419 voxels in one cluster, peak voxel: x = 28, y = −18, z = 21, d = 0.67; MDDlr: ptfce− FWE < 0.005, total k = 34 734 voxels in one cluster, peak voxel: x = −15, y = −9, z = −6, d = 0.61). For location and size of all significant clusters as well as affected tracts see online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3. All other effects were not significant (all p tfce−FWE > 0.332; online Supplementary Table S3).

Accessory analyses and robustness checks

Analysing only participants with ⩾ 26 years of age (N = 284) the post-hoc differences between healthy risk and low-risk participants found in the original sample could be replicated with a nominally higher effect size (p tfce−FWE = 0.009, d = 0.75) (online Supplement S3). Similarly, including participants with < 60 years of age yielded the same results (online Supplement S4). The results from our first analyses remained also unchanged when excluding participants whose children or siblings were affected (online Supplement S5).

Control analyses in SPSS confirmed our main results. Even after excluding 14 outliers from the original sample, the main effect of diagnosis and interaction effect remained significant (online Supplement S4). A weak but significantly negative correlation between CTQSum and FA scores (r(526) = −0.094, p = 0.031) did not abolish the effect either: when including CTQSum as a covariate in the general linear model in SPSS, the main effect (F (1,527) = 4.28, p = 0.039, d = 0.18) and interaction effect (F (1,527) = 36.18, p < 0.001, d = 0.53) were still significant with no significant impact of CTQ (F (1,527) = 0.325, p = 0.569).

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated cross-sectional correlates of familial risk for MDD and WM microstructure in a large sample of HC and patients with MDD. We were (a) able to replicate a previously reported significant main effect of diagnosis and (b) found a significant interaction effect of familial risk with diagnosis. HC had increased FA as compared to MDD participants in the FM and left SLF. The interaction effect was driven by widespread increased FA in healthy participants with a first-degree relative with MDD as compared to a low-risk healthy sample, mainly in the FM and right IFOF. The additional covariance of self-reported retrospective childhood maltreatment scores did not change the significant results, suggesting the reported WM alterations reflect rather genetic than environmental risk which has also been associated with distinct disruptions in WM microstructure (Frodl et al. Reference Frodl, Carballedo, Fagan, Lisiecka, Ferguson and Meaney2012; Huang et al. Reference Huang, Gundapuneedi and Rao2012; Meinert et al. Reference Meinert, Repple, Nenadic, Krug, Jansen, Grotegerd and Dannlowski2019), effects on cognition (Goltermann et al. Reference Goltermann, Redlich, Grotegerd, Dohm, Leehr, Böhnlein and Dannlowski2021) and grey matter anomalies (Opel et al. Reference Opel, Zwanzger, Redlich, Grotegerd, Dohm, Arolt and Dannlowski2016). No significant differences were found between MDDr and MDDlr concerning all four analysed diffusion indices while recurrence of episodes and state of remission did not differ between the MDD samples.

We were able to replicate results by Murphy & Frodl (Reference Murphy and Frodl2011) indicating decreased FA in MDD participants across different states of remission in the left SLF as compared to HC. Connecting the frontal cortex with the parietal, temporal and occipital cortex, the SLF is a major bidirectional association tract which is involved in specific behavioural and cognitive functions in healthy adults such as verbal memory, but also non-verbal cognition, working memory, and visuo-spatial functions (Koshiyama et al. Reference Koshiyama, Fukunaga, Okada, Morita, Nemoto, Yamashita and Hashimoto2020), as well as recognition of emotional faces (Ioannucci, George, Friedrich, Cerliani, & Thiebaut de Schotten, Reference Ioannucci, George, Friedrich, Cerliani and Thiebaut de Schotten2020). A meta-analysis across emotional disorders identified reduction in FA in the SLF, as compared to HC, as the most replicable finding and concludes that the functions associated with the SLF are in line with impaired perception of and attention to emotional information in these disorders (Jenkins et al. Reference Jenkins, Barba, Campbell, Lamar, Shankman, Leow and Langenecker2016). Consolidation of WM fibre microstructure in the SLF might be positively associated with emotional perception and cognitive control in HC as compared to MDD in general.

In contrast to previous studies, no significant reduction in FA in our healthy risk v. low-risk participants were found in the bilateral cingulum bundles (Bracht et al. Reference Bracht, Linden and Keedwell2015; Huang et al. Reference Huang, Fan, Williamson and Rao2011; Keedwell et al. Reference Keedwell, Chapman, Christiansen, Richardson, Evans and Jones2012). Instead, the present increases in FA in HCr as compared to HClr are partly in contrast to previous studies with a similar design, revealing decreases in FA. Nevertheless, our results are in line with Frodl et al. (Reference Frodl, Carballedo, Fagan, Lisiecka, Ferguson and Meaney2012) who also reported increased FA in the right IFOF, among other tracts, in healthy relatives of MDD patients as compared to HC without familial risk. These structural differences could result in differences in the neural processing of emotion. The IFOF is involved in emotional visual function, building an association between the vision-related ventro-medial occipital to the emotion-related infero- and dorsolateral regions of the frontal lobe (Catani, Howard, Pajevic, & Jones, Reference Catani, Howard, Pajevic and Jones2002). Altered emotional visual perception, i.e., altered transmission of valenced signals linked to reduced FA in IFOF has been observed in depression (Kieseppä et al. Reference Kieseppä, Eerola, Mäntylä, Neuvonen, Poutanen, Luoma and Isometsä2010) and female adolescent HCr (Joormann, Cooney, Henry, & Gotlib, Reference Joormann, Cooney, Henry and Gotlib2012). We also found increased FA in the FM in HCr v. HClr. The FM connects the two hemispheres, more specifically the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (DLPFC). The DLPFC is involved in attention, executive functions and internally guided behaviour during goal-oriented and working memory tasks (Kane & Engle, Reference Kane and Engle2002). Furthermore, it is activated during emotion regulation processes (Dixon, Thiruchselvam, Todd, & Christoff, Reference Dixon, Thiruchselvam, Todd and Christoff2017; Versace et al. Reference Versace, Ladouceur, Graur, Acuff, Bonar, Monk and Phillips2018), more specifically mindfulness-based negative emotion regulation strategies (Opialla et al. Reference Opialla, Lutz, Scherpiet, Hittmeyer, Jäncke, Rufer and Brühl2015). Enhanced WM fibres in the IFOF and FM might point towards enhanced negative emotion regulation and activation of cognitive strategies in HCr which prevent depressive symptomatology and promote mental health.

A reason why we found results contradicting previous studies might be a different age range of participants. Importantly, Huang et al. (Reference Huang, Fan, Williamson and Rao2011) included adolescents from age 12 to 20 only, whereas our sample consisted of individuals between 18 and 65, with the majority of participants in an age range from 25 to 30. Weissman et al. (Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad and Talati2016b) found that the risk for first onset of major depression in high-risk and low-risk individuals was highest between ages 15 and 25. In our reported sub-analysis excluding participants from our sample aged under 26 years, we were indeed able to replicate the findings of increased FA in HCr v. HClr with a higher effect size than in the original analysis. Our participants might therefore have adapted to the increased risk: Increased FA in HCr might reflect overcompensation, a neurological mechanism allocating more, yet protective, resources in tracts connecting areas which are involved in emotion-processing, emotion regulation and executional tasks. Extending the results of Frodl et al. (Reference Frodl, Carballedo, Fagan, Lisiecka, Ferguson and Meaney2012), we revealed an interaction between diagnosis and familial risk, suggesting that our HCr group is resilient in contrast to MDDr. However, this should be addressed in future studies with measurements of resilience, e.g., questionnaires.

Limitations

Our study's cross-sectional design does not allow disentangling between precursor and consequence. Longitudinal studies are needed to account for putative causal relationships between familial risk factors and alterations in WM microstructure.

Even though reported in the follow-up study by Weissman et al. (Reference Weissman, Berry, Warner, Gameroff, Skipper, Talati and Wickramaratne2016a, Reference Weissman, Wickramaratne, Gameroff, Warner, Pilowsky, Kohad and Talati2016b), studies have exposed contradictory findings on the age of onset for MDD (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005; Solmi et al. Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo and Fusar-Poli2021). However, we based our decision to conduct an accessory analysis with an older age group with this specific cut-off on data in high-risk and low-risk individuals which were investigated over a time period of 30 years.

Secondly, we relied on participants' self-report on whether a first-degree relative suffered from MDD. In order to increase the level of confidence regarding diagnosis, we only included participants who confirmed that the affected relative received treatment related to the diagnosis, but however, we cannot exclude that participants categorised as low-risk might have been unaware of affected relatives. Including only participants whose relatives were treated for depression might also indicate more severe cases of depression, and might also be influenced by the social and financial ability to seek and receive treatment. Moreover, we did not control for the number of affected relatives. Relatedly, family history and personal depression are not independent. On the one hand, the experience of having a first-degree relative with MDD might lead to more attention in the individual towards this disorder. The individual potentially reports symptoms earlier and seeks treatment faster, as they are aware of the increased risk. On the other hand, self-experienced stigma and shame might lead to not mentioning affected family members.

Furthermore, our operationalisation of familial risk does not only represent an estimation of genetic, but also of a stressful familial environment. Future studies should investigate this association between genetic markers and WM microstructure. However, childhood maltreatment scores as measured with the CTQSum did not differ significantly between HClr and HCr. Additionally, the interaction effect remained stable when including CTQSum as a covariate in our analyses, making strong influences of the environmental component of familial risk rather unlikely. However, disentangling the specific mechanisms of neglect and abuse as mediators on developing depression needs to be addressed in future studies with specific study designs including groups recruited for presence/absence of childhood maltreatment.

Notably, there were more females than males in our sample. Further studies on the interaction of sex and familial risk on FA would be of interest.

Conclusion

The main finding of our study shows that resilient individuals with familial risk for MDD exhibit widespread increased FA, with an emphasis on the forceps minor and right IFOF as compared to healthy individuals without such risk. This challenges previous results that HC at risk have decreased FA in the cingulum bundle. The effects remained significant after the exclusion of participants under the age of 26, and when correcting for a measure of environmental risk, i.e., self-reported childhood maltreatment. Even though we were able to replicate results indicating increased FA in HC in the left SLF as compared to MDD participants, the significant interaction of familial risk and diagnosis suggests adaptation processes on a neural level in a healthy, but not in an already affected at-risk sample, possibly reflecting resilience. Future longitudinal studies are needed to disentangle precursor and consequence of these WM alterations in resilient individuals and to which degree they can help understand the biological basis of depression, particularly taking the rather modest effect sizes into account.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172200188X

Acknowledgements

Principal investigators (PIs) with respective areas of responsibility in the FOR2107 consortium are: Work Package WP1, FOR2107/MACS cohort and brainimaging: Tilo Kircher (speaker FOR2107; DFG grant numbers KI 588/14-1, KI 588/14-2), Udo Dannlowski (co-speaker FOR2107; DA 1151/5-1, DA 1151/5-2), Axel Krug (KR 3822/5-1, KR 3822/7-2), Igor Nenadic (NE 2254/1-2), Carsten Konrad (KO 4291/3-1). Data access and responsibility: All PIs take responsibility for the integrity of the respective study data and their components. All authors and coauthors had full access to all study data. Acknowledgements and members of Work Package 1 (WP1): Henrike Bröhl, Katharina Brosch, Bruno Dietsche, Rozbeh Elahi, Jennifer Engelen, Sabine Fischer, Jessica Heinen, Svenja Klingel, Felicitas Meier, Tina Meller, Julia-Katharina Pfarr, Kai Ringwald, Torsten Sauder, Simon Schmitt, Frederike Stein, Annette Tittmar, Dilara Yüksel (Dept. of Psychiatry, Marburg University). Mechthild Wallnig, Rita Werner (Core-Facility Brainimaging, Marburg University). Carmen Schade-Brittinger, Maik Hahmann (Coordinating Centre for Clinical Trials, Marburg). Michael Putzke (Psychiatric Hospital, Friedberg). Rolf Speier, Lutz Lenhard (Psychiatric Hospital, Haina). Birgit Köhnlein (Psychiatric Practice, Marburg). Peter Wulf, Jürgen Kleebach, Achim Becker (Psychiatric Hospital Hephata, Schwalmstadt-Treysa). Ruth Bär (Care facility Bischoff, Neukirchen). Matthias Müller, Michael Franz, Siegfried Scharmann, Anja Haag, Kristina Spenner, Ulrich Ohlenschläger (Psychiatric Hospital Vitos, Marburg). Matthias Müller, Michael Franz, Bernd Kundermann (Psychiatric Hospital Vitos, Gießen). Christian Bürger, Katharina Dohm, Fanni Dzvonyar, Verena Enneking, Stella Fingas, Katharina Förster, Janik Goltermann, Dominik Grotegerd, Hannah Lemke, Susanne Meinert, Nils Opel, Ronny Redlich, Jonathan Repple, Katharina Thiel Kordula Vorspohl, Bettina Walden, Lena Waltemate, Alexandra Winter, Dario Zaremba (Dept. of Psychiatry, University of Münster). Harald Kugel, Jochen Bauer, Walter Heindel, Birgit Vahrenkamp (University Clinic for Radiology, University of Münster). Gereon Heuft, Gudrun Schneider (Dept. of Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Münster). Thomas Reker (LWL-Hospital Münster). Gisela Bartling (IPP Münster). Ulrike Buhlmann (Dept. of Clinical Psychology, University of Münster). The FOR2107 cohort project (WP1) was approved by the Ethics Committees of the Medical Faculties, University of Marburg (AZ: 07/14) and University of Münster (AZ: 2014-422-b-S). A list of all acknowledgements can be accessed here: https://for2107.de/acknowledgements/?lang=en

Author contributions

Substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work: A.W., KT., S.M., H.L., L.W., U.D.; Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data for the work: A.W., K.T., S.M., H.L., L.W., F.B., R.C., J.-K. P., F.S., K.B., T.M., K.G.R., F.T-O., A.J., I.N., A.K., J.R., N.O., K.D., E.J.L., D.G., H.K., T.H., T.K., and U.D.; Drafting the work or revising it critically for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved: All of the above-mentioned authors. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Financial support

This work is part of the German multicentre consortium ‘Neurobiology of Affective Disorders. A translational perspective on brain structure and function’, funded by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft DFG; Forschungsgruppe/Research Unit FOR2107). Tilo Kircher (speaker FOR2107; DFG grant numbers KI 588/14-1, KI 588/14-2), Udo Dannlowski (co-speaker FOR2107; DA 1151/5-1, DA 1151/5-2), Axel Krug (KR 3822/5-1, KR 3822/7-2), Igor Nenadic (NE 2254/1-2), Tim Hahn (HA 7070/2-2), Andreas Jansen (JA 1890/7-1, JA 1890/7-2).

Conflicts of interest

Tilo Kircher received unrestricted educational grants from Servier, Janssen, Recordati, Aristo, Otsuka, neuraxpharm. This funding is not associated with the current work. Markus Wöhr is the scientific advisor of Avisoft Bioacoustics. On behalf of all other authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest and nothing to disclose.

References

Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., & Field, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics, 4(3), 316329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Amico, F., Meisenzahl, E., Koutsouleris, N., Reiser, M., Möller, H. J., & Frodl, T. (2011). Structural MRI correlates for vulnerability and resilience to major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 36(1), 1522. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.090186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersson, J. L. R., & Sotiropoulos, S. N. (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage, 125, 10631078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck depression inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77100. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch Gen Psychiatry, 4(6), 561571.10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Nunes, R. G., Clare, S., … Smith, S. M. (2003). Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 50(5), 10771088. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.10609CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bracht, T., Linden, D., & Keedwell, P. (2015). A review of white matter microstructure alterations of pathways of the reward circuit in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 187, 4553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.06.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brosch, K., Stein, F., Meller, T., Schmitt, S., Yuksel, D., Ringwald, K. G., … Krug, A. (2021). DLPFC volume is a neural correlate of resilience in healthy high-risk individuals with both childhood maltreatment and familial risk for depression. Psychological Medicine, 17. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001094Google ScholarPubMed
Catani, M., Howard, R. J., Pajevic, S., & Jones, D. K. (2002). Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. NeuroImage, 17(1), 7794. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis 2nd edition. In The Hand. of Res. Synthesis and Meta-Analysis, 2nd Ed.Google Scholar
De Diego-Adeliño, J., Pires, P., Gómez-Ansón, B., Serra-Blasco, M., Vives-Gilabert, Y., Puigdemont, D., … Portella, M. J. (2014). Microstructural white-matter abnormalities associated with treatment resistance, severity and duration of illness in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 44(6), 11711182. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171300158XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, M. L., Thiruchselvam, R., Todd, R., & Christoff, K. (2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 10331081. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000096CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, H. M., Yeatman, J. D., Lee, E. S., Barde, L. H. F., & Gaman-Bean, S. (2010). Diffusion tensor imaging: A review for pediatric researchers and clinicians. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 346356. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181dcaa8bCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., Carballedo, A., Fagan, A. J., Lisiecka, D., Ferguson, Y., & Meaney, J. F. (2012). Effects of early-life adversity on white matter diffusivity changes in patients at risk for major depression. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 37(1), 3745. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.110028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goltermann, J., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Dohm, K., Leehr, E. J., Böhnlein, J., … Dannlowski, U. (2021). Childhood maltreatment and cognitive functioning: The role of depression, parental education, and polygenic predisposition. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(5), 891899. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00794-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review, 106(3), 458490. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., Joormann, J., & Foland-Ross, L. C. (2014). Understanding familial risk for depression: A 25-year perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 94108. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613513469CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C., Burge, D., Burney, E., & Adrian, C. (1990). Longitudinal study of diagnoses in children of women with unipolar and bipolar affective disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47(12), 11121117. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810240032006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassel, S., Almeida, J. R. C., Kerr, N., Nau, S., Ladouceur, C. D., Fissell, K., … Phillips, M. L. (2008). Elevated striatal and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity in response to emotional stimuli in euthymic bipolar disorder: No associations with psychotropic medication load. Bipolar Disorders, 10(8), 916927. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00641.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. M., Adams, M. J., Clarke, T. K., Hafferty, J. D., Gibson, J., Shirali, M., … McIntosh, A. M. (2019). Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 22(3), 343352. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0326-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, H., Fan, X., Williamson, D. E., & Rao, U. (2011). White matter changes in healthy adolescents at familial risk for unipolar depression: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(3), 684691. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, H., Gundapuneedi, T., & Rao, U. (2012). White matter disruptions in adolescents exposed to childhood maltreatment and vulnerability to psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(12), 26932701. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannucci, S., George, N., Friedrich, P., Cerliani, L., & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. (2020). White matter correlates of hemi-face dominance in happy and sad expression. Brain Structure and Function, 225(4), 13791388. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02040-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, L. M., Barba, A., Campbell, M., Lamar, M., Shankman, S. A., Leow, A. D., … Langenecker, S. A. (2016). Shared white matter alterations across emotional disorders: A voxel-based meta-analysis of fractional anisotropy. NeuroImage: Clinical, 12, 10221034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., & Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. NeuroImage, 62(2), 782790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J., Cooney, R. E., Henry, M. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Neural correlates of automatic mood regulation in girls at high risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 6172. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0025294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 637671. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196323CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsgodt, K. H., van Erp, T. G. M., Poldrack, R. A., Bearden, C. E., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Cannon, T. D. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging of the superior longitudinal Fasciculus and working memory in recent-onset schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 63(5), 512518. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2007.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keedwell, P. A., Chapman, R., Christiansen, K., Richardson, H., Evans, J., & Jones, D. K. (2012). Cingulum white matter in young women at risk of depression: The effect of family history and anhedonia. Biological Psychiatry, 72(4), 296302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., Gardner, C. O., & Pedersen, N. L. (2006). A Swedish national twin study of lifetime major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(1), 109114. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Wittchen, H. (2012). Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3), 169. https://doi.org/10.1002/MPR.1359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kieseppä, T., Eerola, M., Mäntylä, R., Neuvonen, T., Poutanen, V. P., Luoma, K., … Isometsä, E. (2010). Major depressive disorder and white matter abnormalities: A diffusion tensor imaging study with tract-based spatial statistics. Journal of Affective Disorders, 120(1–3), 240244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircher, T., Wöhr, M., Nenadic, I., Schwarting, R., Schratt, G., Alferink, J., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Neurobiology of the major psychoses: A translational perspective on brain structure and function—the FOR2107 consortium. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 269(8), 949962. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-018-0943-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Rohde, P. (2001). A family study of major depressive disorder in a community sample of adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(1), 1320. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.1.13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koshiyama, D., Fukunaga, M., Okada, N., Morita, K., Nemoto, K., Yamashita, F., … Hashimoto, R. (2020). Association between the superior longitudinal fasciculus and perceptual organization and working memory: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 738. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, G. Y., Tam, W. W., Lu, Y., Ho, C. S., Zhang, M. W., & Ho, R. C. (2018). Prevalence of depression in the community from 30 countries between 1994 and 2014 /692/699/476/1414 /692/499 article. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21243-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O'Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(5), 561592. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00100-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., McInerney, S., Sorensen, A. G., Wang, R., Caviness, V. S., & Pandya, D. N. (2005). Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: A quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 15(6), 854869. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHH186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinert, S., Repple, J., Nenadic, I., Krug, A., Jansen, A., Grotegerd, D., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Reduced fractional anisotropy in depressed patients due to childhood maltreatment rather than diagnosis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(12), 20652072. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-019-0472-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M. L., & Frodl, T. (2011). Meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies shows altered fractional anisotropy occurring in distinct brain areas in association with depression. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 1(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-1-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oguz, I., Farzinfar, M., Matsui, J., Budin, F., Liu, Z., Gerig, G., … Styner, M. (2014). DTIPrep: Quality control of diffusion-weighted images. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 8, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2014.00004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Dohm, K., Zaremba, D., Meinert, S., … Dannlowski, U. (2017). Prefrontal brain responsiveness to negative stimuli distinguishes familial risk for major depression from acute disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 42(5), 343352. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.160198CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Zwanzger, P., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Dohm, K., Arolt, V., … Dannlowski, U. (2016). Differing brain structural correlates of familial and environmental risk for major depressive disorder revealed by a combined VBM/pattern recognition approach. Psychological Medicine, 46(2), 277290. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715001683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opialla, S., Lutz, J., Scherpiet, S., Hittmeyer, A., Jäncke, L., Rufer, M., … Brühl, A. B. (2015). Neural circuits of emotion regulation: A comparison of mindfulness-based and cognitive reappraisal strategies. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265(1), 4555. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-014-0510-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pawlby, S., Hay, D., Sharp, D., Waters, C. S., & Pariante, C. M. (2011). Antenatal depression and offspring psychopathology: The influence of childhood maltreatment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199(2), 106112. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.110.087734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, B. S., Warner, V., Bansal, R., Zhu, H., Hao, X., Liu, J., … Weissman, M. M. (2009). Cortical thinning in persons at increased familial risk for major depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(15), 62736278. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0805311106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlich, R., Almeida, J. R., Grotegerd, D., Opel, N., Kugel, H., Heindel, W., … Dannlowski, U. (2014). Brain morphometric biomarkers distinguishing unipolar and bipolar depression: A voxel-based morphometry–pattern classification approach. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(11), 1222. https://doi.org/10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2014.1100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repple, J., Mauritz, M., Meinert, S., de Lange, S. C., Grotegerd, D., Opel, N., … van den Heuvel, M. P. (2020). Severity of current depression and remission status are associated with structural connectome alterations in major depressive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(7), 15501558. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0603-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, C. R. (2008). Physician's desk reference. In Encyclopedia of special education. Hoboken, NK, USA: John Wiley Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470373699.speced1606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salat, D. H., Tuch, D. S., Greve, D. N., Van Der Kouwe, A. J. W., Hevelone, N. D., Zaleta, A. K., … Dale, A. M. (2005). Age-related alterations in white matter microstructure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Neurobiology of Aging, 26(8), 12151227. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROBIOLAGING.2004.09.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sexton, C. E., Walhovd, K. B., Storsve, A. B., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., Johansen-Berg, H., & Fjell, A. M. (2014). Accelerated changes in white matter microstructure during aging: A longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(46), 1542515436. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0203-14.2014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sisto, A., Vicinanza, F., Campanozzi, L. L., Ricci, G., Tartaglini, D., & Tambone, V. (2019). Towards a transversal definition of psychological resilience: A literature review. Medicina, 55(11), 745. https://doi.org/10.3390/MEDICINA55110745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping, 17(3), 143155. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay, C. E., … Behrens, T. E. J. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage, 31(4), 14871505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., … Matthews, P. M. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23(Suppl. 1), S208S219. Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, M., Radua, J., Olivola, M., Croce, E., Soardo, L., Salazar de Pablo, G., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2021). Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: Large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 2021(17), 115. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7Google Scholar
Sullivan, P. F., Michael Neale, F. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Reviews and overviews genetic epidemiology of major depression: Review and meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(10), 15521562. http://views.vcu.edu/pub/mx/examples/mdreview 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Velzen, L. S., Kelly, S., Isaev, D., Aleman, A., Aftanas, L. I., Bauer, J., … Schmaal, L. (2020). White matter disturbances in major depressive disorder: A coordinated analysis across 20 international cohorts in the ENIGMA MDD working group. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(7), 15111525. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0477-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Versace, A., Ladouceur, C., Graur, S., Acuff, H., Bonar, L., Monk, K., … Phillips, M. (2018). Diffusion imaging markers of bipolar versus general psychopathology risk in youth at-risk. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(11), 22122220. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0083-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogelbacher, C., Möbius, T. W. D., Sommer, J., Schuster, V., Dannlowski, U., Kircher, T., … Bopp, M. H. A. (2018). The Marburg-Münster Affective Disorders Cohort Study (MACS): A quality assurance protocol for MR neuroimaging data. NeuroImage, 172, 450460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Reinvang, I., Lundervold, A., Dale, A. M., Eilertsen, D. E., … Fischl, B. (2005). Effects of age on volumes of cortex, white matter and subcortical structures. Neurobiology of Aging, 26(9), 12611270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, M. M., Berry, O. O., Warner, V., Gameroff, M. J., Skipper, J., Talati, A., … Wickramaratne, P. (2016a). A 30-year study of 3 generations at high risk and low risk for depression. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(9), 970977. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.1586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Gameroff, M. J., Warner, V., Pilowsky, D., Kohad, R. G., … Talati, A. (2016b). Offspring of depressed parents: 30 years later. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(10), 10241032. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15101327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westlye, L. T., Walhovd, K. B., Dale, A. M., Bjørnerud, A., Due-Tønnessen, P., Engvig, A., … Fjell, A. M. (2010). Life-span changes of the human brain white matter: Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cerebral Cortex, 20(9), 20552068. https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHP280CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windle, G. (2011). What is resilience? A review and concept analysis. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 21(2), 152169. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959259810000420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wingenfeld, K., Spitzer, C., Mensebach, C., Grabe, H., Hill, A., Gast, U., … Driessen, M. (2010). Die deutsche version des childhood trauma questionnaire (CTQ): Erste befunde zu den psychometrischen kennwerten. PPmP – Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 60(08), e13e13. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0030-1253494CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittchen, H. U., Wunderlich, U., Grushwitz, S., & Zaudig, M. (1997). SKID I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen. Interviewheft und Beurteilungsheft. Eine Deutschsprachige, Erweiterte Bearbeitung Der Amerikanischen Originalversion Des SCID I. Göttingen: HogrefeGoogle Scholar
Woolrich, M. W., Jbabdi, S., Patenaude, B., Chappell, M., Makni, S., Behrens, T., … Smith, S. M. (2009). Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. NeuroImage, 45(1 Suppl), S173S186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the total sample (N = 528)

Figure 1

Table 2. Clinical characteristics and medication in the MDD sample (n = 266)

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Affected white matter tracts by effect of diagnosis (HC > MDD).Note. Increased FA in healthy controls as compared to patients with major depressive disorder, mainly in the forceps minor and superior longitudinal fasciculus, ptfce-FWE = 0.009. In order to illustrate the effects on the FMRIB58 template (visualised in green) for all three sectional views in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Atlas coordinate system, the mean FA value was obtained from FA values of all significant voxels (ptfce−FWE < 0.05). Red-yellow areas represent voxels in significant clusters, using the ‘fill’ command in FSL. The colour bar indicates the probability of a voxel being a member of the different labelled regions within the JHU-atlas, averaged over all the voxels in the significant mask (ptfce−FWE < 0.05). MNI coordinates of selected plane: x = 26, y = −19, z = 10. FM, forceps minor; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus

Figure 3

Figure 2. Interaction effect of diagnosis and familial risk, and effect of familial risk in HC.Note. Affected white matter tracts by A: Interaction effect of diagnosis × familial risk, ptfce−FWE = 0.036. B: Familial risk in HC. Widespread increased FA in HCr as compared to HClr, ptfce−FWE < 0.001. Effects illustrated on the FMRIB58 template (visualised in green) in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Atlas coordinate system. Red-yellow areas represent voxels in significant clusters, using the ‘fill’ command in FSL. The colour bar indicates the probability of a voxel being a member of the different labelled regions within the JHU-atlas, averaged over all the voxels in the significant mask (ptfce−FWE < 0.05). MNI coordinates of selected plane for A and B: x = 37, y = −37, z = 7.C: Significant increase in FA in HCr as compared to all other groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. Asterisk represents statistical significance (ptfce−FWE < 0.05) in post-hoc-t-tests. The mean FA value was obtained from FA values of all significant voxels (ptfce−FWE < 0.05). FM, Forceps Minor; IFOF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus.

Supplementary material: File

Winter et al. supplementary material

Winter et al. supplementary material

Download Winter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 74.9 KB