Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-11T11:22:30.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variability and its limits in bilingual language production

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 October 2016

JUBIN ABUTALEBI
Affiliation:
University Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Italy
HARALD CLAHSEN
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism, Germany
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Experimental and other empirical research on language is faced with the fact that language performance exhibits a high degree of variability at all linguistic levels. Variability is found across languages, across speech communities within one language, across individuals within one speech community and even within the same individual. Bilingual language use adds a further source of variability to this already complicated picture. On the other hand, there are aspects of language and language use that are constrained, stable, or robust and that are less (or not at all) subject to variability, for example, possible options that are not chosen in any language or kinds of error that are never produced. Several familiar ways of dealing with the variability of language use and its limits have turned out to be unsatisfactory. One approach has been to simply abstract away from variability with constructs such as the ‘ideal speaker–hearer’ (who – to our knowledge – nobody has met so far). Another strategy is to average across individuals, which sometimes results in arbitrary mean scores or mean activation patterns that are hard to replicate for individuals, even for those who took part in a given study. A third solution when confronted with variability in language use is to take it at face value, positing that every language, every speech community, and even every individual is different, an approach that essentially gives up on discovering any kind of generalizations. While none of these strategies appears to us to be particularly fruitful, the problem of how to deal with variability in language performance and its limits remains.

Type
Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Experimental and other empirical research on language is faced with the fact that language performance exhibits a high degree of variability at all linguistic levels. Variability is found across languages, across speech communities within one language, across individuals within one speech community and even within the same individual. Bilingual language use adds a further source of variability to this already complicated picture. On the other hand, there are aspects of language and language use that are constrained, stable, or robust and that are less (or not at all) subject to variability, for example, possible options that are not chosen in any language or kinds of error that are never produced. Several familiar ways of dealing with the variability of language use and its limits have turned out to be unsatisfactory. One approach has been to simply abstract away from variability with constructs such as the ‘ideal speaker–hearer’ (who – to our knowledge – nobody has met so far). Another strategy is to average across individuals, which sometimes results in arbitrary mean scores or mean activation patterns that are hard to replicate for individuals, even for those who took part in a given study. A third solution when confronted with variability in language use is to take it at face value, positing that every language, every speech community, and even every individual is different, an approach that essentially gives up on discovering any kind of generalizations. While none of these strategies appears to us to be particularly fruitful, the problem of how to deal with variability in language performance and its limits remains.

Our keynote article (Goldrick, Putnam & Schwarz, Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016a) offers a promising approach to this problem and applies it to a core phenomenon of bilingual language use: code mixing. Goldrick et al. (Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016a) promote a constraint-based approach to grammar (Gradient Symbolic Computation, Smolensky, Goldrick & Mathis, Reference Smolensky, Goldrick and Mathis2014) that provides a formally explicit way of bringing together grammatical constraints (or rules) and graded representations. Their specific test case is doubling constructions, blends, and other kinds of across-language code mixing phenomena in which an element of an intended utterance appears in both of a bilingual's languages within a single utterance. Goldrick et al. (Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016a) account for code mixing in terms of grammatical constraints and graded co-activation of representations from both languages of a bilingual speaker. They conclude that their approach not only provides an account of code mixing, but more generally offers a framework to integrate the ‘discrete and gradient properties of bilingual linguistic knowledge and processing’.

Twelve commentaries representing different perspectives on code mixing and bilingual language use accompany the keynote article. Most of the commentators praise Goldrick et al. (Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016a) as an attempt to develop a formally explicit account of bilingual code mixing. Several commentators have also pointed out limitations of the proposed account and of the data presented in its support. Bhatt (Reference Bhatt2016) wonders how a bilingual's so-called portmanteau sentences that have a hybrid structure relative to normal sentences in the two languages are to be dealt with in their account. Muysken (Reference Muysken2016) also points to more complex cases of code mixing that require an extension of the apparatus provided by the keynote article. López (Reference López2016) calls for more detailed grammatical analyses of the phenomena involved in code mixing. Deuchar and Biberauer (Reference Deuchar and Biberauer2016) question the significance of the phenomenon that Goldrick et al. (Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016a) present, arguing that doubling is extremely rare and is likely simply to be a performance error. Poplack and Torres Cacoullos (Reference Poplack and Torres Cacoullos2016) also note the scarcity of doublings and question the utility of this particular construction as a showcase for a particular model of code mixing. Similarly, Gullberg and Parafita Couto (Reference Gullberg and Parafita Couto2016) confront the keynote authors with data from other more frequent types of code mixing, which they argue are challenging for the GSC account. Hartsuiker (Reference Hartsuiker2016) observes that the GSC account is highly flexible, perhaps too flexible to be falsifiable, and that, to be relevant, the currently large number of degrees of freedom in how to account for code mixing and other phenomena needs to be reduced. Green and Wei (Reference Green and Wei2016) note that the proposed account is limited by capturing input output mappings but that it does not explain the mental machinery, the mechanisms of bilingual language control that underlie code mixing and other aspects of bilingual language use. A similar point is made by Sorace (Reference Sorace2016), namely that the GSC is a purely linguistic model that does not have much to say about the mechanisms of bilingual language processing. Bobb and Hoshino (Reference Bobb and Hoshino2016) call for an extension of Goldrick et al.’s (Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016a) account to include a developmental perspective. Other commentators apply the framework laid out in the keynote article to other phenomena of bilingual language performance. Van Hell, Cohen and Grey (Reference Van Hell, Cohen and Grey2016) demonstrate that the model is flexible enough to account for variable lexically-specific effects on code mixing, and Veríssimo (Reference Veríssimo2016) applies the model to gradient differences between native and non-native language processing. In their response, Goldrick et al. (Reference Goldrick, Putnam and Schwarz2016b) readily admit the current limitations of their proposal and welcome the extensions to other phenomena. What remains as their main contribution is a rather promising attempt at linking different traditions of bilingualism research, formal grammatical investigations and psycholinguistic studies.

We hope our readers will enjoy the keynote article together with the commentaries and the authors’ response as well as the interesting regular research articles and research notes presented in the current issue.

References

Bhatt, R. M. (2016). Coactivation: The portmanteau constructions in bilingual grammar. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 877878. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000146 Google Scholar
Bobb, S. C., & Hoshino, N. (2016). Fusing languages in the bilingual cognitive architecture. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 879880. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000109 Google Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Biberauer, T. (2016). Doubling: an error or an illusion? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 881882. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000043 Google Scholar
Goldrick, M., Putnam, M., & Schwarz, L. (2016a). Coactivation in bilingual grammars: A computational account of code mixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 857876. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000802 Google Scholar
Goldrick, M., Putnam, M., & Schwarz, L. (2016b). The future of code mixing research: Integrating psycholinguistic and formal grammatical theories. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 903906. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000390 Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Wei, L. (2016). Code-switching and language control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 883884. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000018 Google Scholar
Gullberg, M., & Parafita Couto, M. C. (2016). An integrated perspective on code-mixing patterns beyond doubling? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 885886. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000080 Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J. (2016). Constraints on variation, reduplication of semantics, and degrees of freedom: some notes on the computational account of code mixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 887888. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000092 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, L. (2016). Questions on data and the input to GEN. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 889890. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000079 Google Scholar
Muysken, P. (2016). Bilingual complexes: the perspective of the Gradient Symbolic Computation framework. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 891892. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000031 Google Scholar
Poplack, S., & Torres Cacoullos, R. (2016). Data before models. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 893894. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000067 Google Scholar
Smolensky, P., Goldrick, M., & Mathis, D. (2014). Optimization and Quantization in Gradient Symbol Systems: A Framework for Integrating the Continuous and the Discrete in Cognition. Cognitive Science, 38 (6), 11021138. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12047 Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2016). Language and cognition in bilingual production: the real work still lies ahead. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 895896. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000110 Google Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., Cohen, C., & Grey, S. (2016). Testing tolerance for lexically-specific factors in Gradient Symbolic Computation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 897899. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000122 Google Scholar
Veríssimo, J. (2016). Extending a Gradient Symbolic approach to the native versus non-native contrast: The case of plurals in compounds. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 900902. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000134 Google Scholar