Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:54:16.179Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Functional Connectome of the Five-Factor Model of Personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2018

Nicola Toschi
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedicine & Prevention, University “Tor Vergata”, Rome, Italy Department of Radiology, Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston & Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Roberta Riccelli
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
Iole Indovina
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy The Centre of Space BioMedicine, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy
Antonio Terracciano
Affiliation:
Department of Geriatrics, Florida State University College of Medicine, Tallahassee, FL, USA
Luca Passamonti*
Affiliation:
Institute of Bioimaging & Molecular Physiology, National Research Council, Catanzaro, Italy Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Luca Passamonti, E-mail: lp337@medschl.cam.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A key objective of the emerging field of personality neuroscience is to link the great variety of the enduring dispositions of human behaviour with reliable markers of brain function. This can be achieved by analysing big data-sets with methods that model whole-brain connectivity patterns. To meet these expectations, we exploited a large repository of personality and neuroimaging measures made publicly available via the Human Connectome Project. Using connectomic analyses based on graph theory, we computed global and local indices of functional connectivity (e.g., nodal strength, efficiency, clustering, betweenness centrality) and related these metrics to the five-factor model (FFM) personality traits (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness). The maximal information coefficient was used to assess for linear and nonlinear statistical dependencies across the graph “nodes”, which were defined as distinct large-scale brain circuits identified via independent component analysis. Multivariate regression models and “train/test” approaches were used to examine the associations between FFM traits and connectomic indices as well as to assess the generalizability of the main findings, while accounting for age and sex variability. Conscientiousness was the sole FFM trait linked to measures of higher functional connectivity in the fronto-parietal and default mode networks. This offers a mechanistic explanation of the behavioural observation that conscientious people are reliable and efficient in goal-setting or planning. Our study provides new inputs to understanding the neurological basis of personality and contributes to the development of more realistic models of the brain dynamics that mediate personality differences.

Type
Empirical Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018

Personality neuroscience is a rapidly growing research field that aims at understanding the neural underpinnings of variability in cognitive and emotional functions as well as the brain basis of individual differences in behaviour (Corr, Reference Corr2006; DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan, & Gray, Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010). Extensive research in personality has shown that the complexity of human behaviour can be described by an aggregate taxonomy termed the five-factor model (FFM) (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Digman, Reference Digman1990; McCrae & Terracciano, Reference McCrae and Terracciano2005), although other models of personality have also been developed to explain a wide range of behaviours, including clinical disorders, occupational/educational performance, and economic choices (Ashton et al., Reference Ashton, Lee, Perugini, Szarota, de Vries, Di Blas and … De Raad2004; Cloninger, Reference Cloninger1999; Cloninger, Przybeck, & Svrakic, Reference Cloninger, Przybeck and Svrakic1991; Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, Reference Cloninger, Svrakic and Przybeck1993; Corr, Reference Corr2006; Eysenck, Reference Eysenck1983, Reference Eysenck2012; Gray, Reference Gray1970; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2003). The FFM posits that neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness are universal descriptors of the human enduring behavioural dispositions (McCrae, Reference McCrae1991; McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa1987; McCrae & John, Reference McCrae and John1992; McCrae & Terracciano, Reference McCrae and Terracciano2005).

However, how individuals differ in these traits remain an important open question. Recently, sophisticated brain imaging techniques and new analytical methods have become available to formulate novel models regarding the neurological basis of human personality, although it must be acknowledged that neuroimaging is an indirect and correlational measure of brain anatomy and function. Past research has linked the FFM traits to different indices of brain structure and function, although the presence of mixed and often conflicting results in the literature limits the conclusions that can be drawn from these studies (Canli, Reference Canli2004; Canli, Sivers, Whitfield, Gotlib, & Gabrieli, Reference Canli, Sivers, Whitfield, Gotlib and Gabrieli2002; Cremers et al., Reference Cremers, Demenescu, Aleman, Renken, van Tol, van der Wee and … Roelofs2010, Reference Cremers, van Tol, Roelofs, Aleman, Zitman, van Buchem and … van der Wee2011; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010; Dima, Friston, Stephan, & Frangou, Reference Dima, Friston, Stephan and Frangou2015; Fischer, Wik, & Fredrikson, Reference Fischer, Wik and Fredrikson1997; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Erb, Ackermann, Martin, Grodd and Reiterer2011; Indovina, Riccelli, Staab, Lacquaniti, & Passamonti, Reference Indovina, Riccelli, Staab, Lacquaniti and Passamonti2014; Kapogiannis, Sutin, Davatzikos, Costa, & Resnick, Reference Kapogiannis, Sutin, Davatzikos, Costa and Resnick2012; Krebs, Schott, & Duzel, Reference Krebs, Schott and Duzel2009; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Weber, Reuter, Markett, Chu and Montag2013; Lu et al., Reference Lu, Huo, Li, Chen, Liu, Wang and … Chen2014; Passamonti et al., Reference Passamonti, Terracciano, Riccelli, Donzuso, Cerasa, Vaccaro and … Quattrone2015; Riccelli, Indovina, et al., Reference Riccelli, Indovina, Staab, Nigro, Augimeri, Lacquaniti and Passamonti2017; Rodrigo et al., Reference Rodrigo, Di Domenico, Graves, Lam, Ayaz, Bagby and Ruocco2016; Servaas et al., Reference Servaas, van der Velde, Costafreda, Horton, Ormel, Riese and Aleman2013; Wright, Feczko, Dickerson, & Williams, Reference Wright, Feczko, Dickerson and Williams2007; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Williams, Feczko, Barrett, Dickerson, Schwartz and Wedig2006). Several factors may explain the inconsistences across previous findings, including the use of different analytic approaches and the fact that most of the earlier studies, with some notable exceptions (Bjornebekk et al., Reference Bjornebekk, Fjell, Walhovd, Grydeland, Torgersen and Westlye2013; Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Lee, Hollinshead, Bakst, Roffman, Smoller and Buckner2012; Nostro, Muller, Reid, & Eickhoff, Reference Nostro, Muller, Reid and Eickhoff2016; Riccelli, Toschi, Nigro, Terracciano, & Passamonti, Reference Riccelli, Toschi, Nigro, Terracciano and Passamonti2017), have been conducted in small samples of participants.

Another important issue is the necessity to progress from accounts that describe personality differences in terms of anatomical and functional heterogeneity in isolated brain regions, to formal frameworks that model the complexity of the connectivity patterns at the whole-brain circuit level. Within this context, mathematical approaches based on graph theory have been developed to measure the architecture (“topology”) of the brain structural and functional connectivity (i.e., “connectomic” approaches) (Fornito & Bullmore, Reference Fornito and Bullmore2015). The graph theoretical approach provides a series of indices that quantify different aspects of the brain “connectome” (Fornito & Bullmore, Reference Fornito and Bullmore2015). For instance, the network’s capacity to “route” information across its elements (“nodes”) can be estimated by computing the efficiency of the paths (“edges”) linking these nodes (Boccaletti, Latora, Moreno, Chavez, & Hwang, Reference Boccaletti, Latora, Moreno, Chavez and Hwang2006). In other words, the network’s efficiency is a quantitative representation of “how easy” it is for an input to “travel” across the graph’s nodes. Consequently, increased efficiency reflects heightened capacity of a network to process and route relevant information across its nodes. Graph analyses also enable to quantify the degree of segregation of a network (modularity) and its capacity to integrate the information at a global or local level (i.e., global or local clustering coefficient) (Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010).

Studying how “communications” across large-scale brain circuits relate to each of the FFM traits has thus the potential to improve our understanding of the neurological roots of human personality. The rationale behind this study was to associate each of the FFM traits with functional connectivity patterns across large-scale brain networks. Although the relationship between the blood-oxygen-level-dependant activity in single regions and the whole-brain network measures is highly complex, there is evidence that “holistic” neuroimaging approaches are able to predict individual variability in multiple behavioural, demographic, and lifestyle measures (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and … Miller2015). However, it remains to be determined whether graph-based metrics can be associated to individual differences in the FFM personality traits. To take a step in this direction, we studied the brain functional connectome in relation to the FFM in a large sample of individuals drawn from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (n=818, age range: 22–37 years). The HCP is an international project that has granted open access to an unprecedented large set of demographics, personality, and neuroimaging data with high spatial and temporal resolution (McNab et al., Reference McNab, Edlow, Witzel, Huang, Bhat, Heberlein and … Wald2013).

By using robust and highly validated methods to analyse resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data, we tested how individual differences in neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness were associated to global and local indices of brain functional connectivity (e.g., nodal strength, efficiency, clustering). A validation approach based on a “training” and “testing” split of the total data set was also employed to assess for the replicability of the main findings. We hypothesized that the FFM traits linked to less favourable outcomes (e.g., risk of developing psychiatric disorders) like neuroticism were associated to reduced brain functional connectivity (e.g., low nodal strength, low clustering, and low efficiency). Conversely, FFM traits like openness, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (which have been linked to curiosity, social skills, and life success) were expected to relate to measures of heightened functional connectivity (e.g., high nodal strength, high clustering, and high efficiency).

These predictions were based upon a recent study which found that functional connectomic metrics relate to a “single-axis” covariation (ranging from “positive” to “negative” measures) in behavioural traits (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and … Miller2015). In other words, those individuals scoring high on the “positive” end of the behavioural axis linking lifestyle, demographic, and other psychometric measures (e.g., fluid intelligence) displayed stronger functional connectivity patterns than low-scoring participants (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and … Miller2015). Interestingly, the brain regions that most contributed to these increased functional connectivity patterns included those areas that belong to the default mode network (DMN) (e.g., the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate, and temporo-parietal junction). Although the precise role of each region within the DMN is still matter of debate (Leech, Kamourieh, Beckmann, & Sharp, Reference Leech, Kamourieh, Beckmann and Sharp2011), there is robust evidence that the DMN as a whole is involved in several aspects of human cognition and behaviour, including episodic and semantic memory, imagination, decision-making, and theory of mind (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Wiebels, Sumner, van Mulukom, Grady, Schacter and Addis2017; Schacter, Reference Schacter2012; Schacter et al., Reference Schacter, Addis, Hassabis, Martin, Spreng and Szpunar2012; Schacter, Benoit, De Brigard, & Szpunar, Reference Schacter, Benoit, De Brigard and Szpunar2015). It is thus reasonable to expect that enhanced functional connectivity patterns within and across the DMN is linked with FFM personality traits that predict “positive” and favourable behavioural outcomes, although caution is always warranted when making reverse inferences in interpreting neuroimaging findings (Poldrack, Reference Poldrack2006).

Participants and methods

Participants

The demographic and personality variables of the HCP sample are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and personality variables in the Human Connectome Project sample (n=818 volunteers)

Notes: NEO-FFI=NEO five-factors inventory questionnaire.

Age, education, and personality data are expressed as mean±standard deviation, whereas the range in parentheses is expressed as minimum–maximum.

Personality assessment

The FFM personality traits were assessed via the NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI) (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Terracciano, Reference Terracciano2003). The NEO-FFI is composed by 60 items, 12 for each of the five factors. For each item, participants reported their level of agreement on a 5-points Likert scale, from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The NEO instruments have been previously validated in the United States and several other countries (McCrae & Terracciano, Reference McCrae and Terracciano2005).

MRI scanning protocol and preprocessing

rs-fMRI data were acquired from a 3T scanner (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany) (Van Essen et al., Reference Van Essen, Ugurbil, Auerbach, Barch, Behrens, Bucholz and … Curtiss2012). Four runs of 15 min each were obtained. Participants lay within the scanner with open eyes while fixating a bright central cross-projected on a dark background. Oblique axial acquisitions were alternated between phase encoding in a right-to-left direction in one run and phase encoding in a left-to-right direction in the other run. Gradient-echo echo-planar imaging used the following parameters: repetition time (TR)=720 ms, echo time (TE)=33.1 ms, flip angle=52°, field of view (FOV)=208×180 mm, matrix 104×90, slice thickness=2.0 mm, 72 slices, 2.0 mm isotropic voxels, multiband factor=8, echo spacing=.58 ms, bandwidth (BW)=2,290 Hz/Px. There were 4,800 rs-fMRI volumes in total per participant, subdivided in four runs of 1,200 volumes each. Structural (T1-weighted) images and field maps were also acquired to aid data preprocessing.

Each 15-min (1,200 volumes) run of each participant’s rs-fMRI data were preprocessed using FMRIB Software Library (FSL; https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/) and it was minimally preprocessed according to the latest version (3.1) of the HCP pipeline (Glasser et al., Reference Glasser, Sotiropoulos, Wilson, Coalson, Fischl, Andersson and … Polimeni2013). Each data set was then temporally demeaned and had variance normalization applied according to Beckmann and Smith (Reference Beckmann and Smith2004). Group-principal component analysis (PCA) output was generated by MIGP (MELODIC’s Incremental Group-PCA), a technique that approximates full temporal concatenation of all participants’ data, from all 818 participants. This comprises the top 4,500 weighted spatial eigenvectors from a group-averaged PCA (Smith, Hyvärinen, Varoquaux, Miller, & Beckmann, Reference Smith, Hyvärinen, Varoquaux, Miller and Beckmann2014). The MIGP output was then fed into group-independent components analysis (ICA) using FSL’s MELODIC tool (Beckmann & Smith, Reference Beckmann and Smith2004), applying spatial-ICA at dimensionality of 15. Successively, the ICA maps were dual regressed into each participant’s four-dimensional data set to give a set of 15 time courses of 4,800 time points per participant. Further details regarding data acquisition and processing can be found in the HCP S900 Release reference manual available at https://www.humanconnectome.org/

Estimation of functional connectivity

To quantify the resting-state functional connectivity among the 15 circuits (“nodes”), the maximum information coefficient (MIC) between the time series of each pair of circuits was computed (Reshef et al., Reference Reshef, Reshef, Finucane, Grossman, McVean, Turnbaugh and … Sabeti2011). MIC is a powerful statistical measure that is sensitive to both linear and nonlinear associations of arbitrary shape between paired variables (Reshef et al., Reference Reshef, Reshef, Finucane, Grossman, McVean, Turnbaugh and … Sabeti2011). This method has been applied to investigate the functional connectivity patterns in patients with schizophrenia (Su, Wang, Shen, Feng, & Hu, Reference Su, Wang, Shen, Feng and Hu2013; Zhang, Sun, Yi, Wu, & Ding, Reference Zhang, Sun, Yi, Wu and Ding2015). The basic idea underlying MIC is that, when a relationship between two variables exists, it can be quantified via creating a grid on the scatterplot that creates a partition of the data. More formally, the MIC between two variables x and y is defined as

$$\eqalign { I\left( {x,y} \right) \,{\equals}\mathop \sum\limits_{i\,{\equals}\,1}^{n_{x} } p\left( {x_{i} } \right)\log _{2} {1 \over {p\left( {x_{i} } \right)}}{\plus}\mathop \sum\limits_{j\,{\equals}\,1}^{n_{y} } p\left( {y_{j} } \right)\log _{2} {1 \over {p\left( {y_{j} } \right)}} \cr \quad{\minus}\mathop \sum\limits_{i\,{\equals}1}^{n_{x} } \mathop \sum\limits_{j{\equals}1}^{n_{y} } p\left( {x_{i} y_{j} } \right)\log _{2} {1 \over {p\left( {x_{i} y_{j} } \right)}},$$

where n x and n y are the number of bins of the partition of the x- and y-axis. Therefore, the MIC of two variables x and y is calculated as

$${\rm MIC}\,{\equals}\,{\rm max}\left\{ {{{I\left( {x,\,y} \right)} \over {\log _{2} {\rm min}\{ n_{x} ,\,n_y{{{ \} }} }}}} \right\},$$

where the maximum is taken over all the possible n x by n y grids. The MIC between each pair of networks’ time series was calculated using the MINEPY toolbox (Albanese et al., Reference Albanese, Filosi, Visintainer, Riccadonna, Jurman and Furlanello2013) implemented in MATLAB (https://github.com/minepy/minepy). These analytical steps generated a 15×15 full and symmetric subject-specific matrix of functional connectivity data. The matrices were then treated as weighted networks to calculate the graph-related measures.

Local network analyses

All graph measures were computed via the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010) in MATLAB (https://sites.google.com/site/bctnet/). For each ICA and at the participant level, we calculated the graph measures that quantify the centrality of a node within a network (local strength and betweenness centrality) as well as its integration and segregation properties (clustering coefficient and local efficiency respectively). Local strength and betweenness centrality are two indices of centrality that measure the relative importance of a node within a network (Zuo et al., Reference Zuo, Ehmke, Mennes, Imperati, Castellanos, Sporns and Milham2012). Nodes with high levels of centrality are thought to facilitate information routing in the network with a key role in the overall communication efficiency of a network. The node’s strength is the simplest measure of centrality and is defined as the sum of all the edge weights between a node and all the other nodes in the network. Regions with high nodal strength have high connectivity with other nodes. Betweenness centrality of a node is defined as the fraction of all shortest paths in the network that contain a given node. If a node displays high betweenness centrality it participates in a large number of shortest paths and have an important role in the information transfer within a network. Along with centrality measures, the nodes of a network may display different levels of segregation and integration of information (Sporns, Reference Sporns2013). For example, the clustering coefficient is a commonly used metric to assess the segregation properties of a network. It reflects the ability of a node to communicate with other nodes with which it shares direct connections; in other words, it represents the fraction of triangles around an individual node. It is equivalent to the fraction of the node’s neighbours that are also neighbours of each other (Watts & Strogatz, Reference Watts and Strogatz1998) and in the case of weighted networks it is calculated as the geometric mean of all triangles associated with each node (Onnela, Saramäki, Kertész, & Kaski, Reference Onnela, Saramäki, Kertész and Kaski2005). Finally, an efficient information transfer across distributed nodes (i.e., nodes that are not directly connected) can be quantified via the local path length and local efficiency. In the case of a weighted network, high levels of correlations between the functional activity of two nodes are interpreted as short local path length. The local efficiency is the average of the inverse local path length. Local efficiency is calculated as the global efficiency of the subgraph formed by the node’s neighbours (Boccaletti et al., Reference Boccaletti, Latora, Moreno, Chavez and Hwang2006). It measures the ability of parallel information transfer at local level.

Global network analyses

Global graph metrics describe the topology of a network with a single number that represents the overall organization of a network. As global measures, we computed the global strength, the global clustering coefficient, and global efficiency (Boccaletti et al., Reference Boccaletti, Latora, Moreno, Chavez and Hwang2006; Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010). These measures were calculated as the average of the local strength, local clustering coefficient, and local efficiency of all nodes, respectively.

Group-level analyses

To estimate the replicability of our inference framework, the initial sample of n=818 participants was randomly split into two sub-samples: a “training” sample (70% of participants, n=573) and a “test” sample (30% of participants, n=245). The “training” sample was used to examine the association between each of the graph measures (i.e., global and local) and the FFM personality traits. Conversely, the “test” sample was only employed to assess whether the multivariate model based on the “training” sample was able to predict the outcome “connectomic” measures in the “test” sample (i.e., in a group of participants to which the model was completely “agnostic”). To test the associations between graph measures and personality differences, general linear models (GLMs), including each of the FFM traits as well as age and gender as nuisance covariates, were fitted using the “training” sample. The resulting p values were corrected for multiple comparisons using a false discovery rate (FDR) procedure. Associations surviving a stringent threshold of p<.01 FDR were considered statistically significant. The GLMs fitted in the former procedure were then used to estimate the graph measures resulting in the “test” sample using the demographic and personality scores of the “test” sample as inputs (in other words, the rs-fMRI data of the “train” sample were not employed in this procedure). The similarity between “real” graph measures (i.e., computed using rs-fMRI data from the “test” sample) and “estimated” graph indices (i.e., predicted using the GLMs fitted on “training” data only) was assessed using the relative root mean square error (RRMSE). This approach is typically referred as external validation and tests for generalizability of the findings beyond the study population. The image analysis workflow is summarized in Figure 1.

Figure 1 Image analysis workflow. After initial pre-processing, the resting-state functional magnetic imaging (fMRI) data were used to extract a set of 15 separate brain circuits via independent components analysis (ICA). Next, participant-specific time-series from each ICA brain circuit was obtained. The maximal information coefficient (MIC), an index that assesses for linear and nonlinear relationships in big data-sets, was used to measure statistical dependency between each pair of time-series. This led to a 15×15 functional connectivity matrix at the single-participant level. The participant-specific connectivity matrices were then used to compute local and global graph measures (i.e., strength, clustering, efficiency, and betweenness centrality). Each of these graph measures, which quantify different aspects of the brain topological organization, was finally correlated with the five-factor model personality traits at the group level. BOLD=blood-oxygen-level-dependant activity.

Results

ICA

The 15 brain networks identified via ICA were represented by a series of circuits that have been consistently reported in past rs-fMRI studies (e.g., the sensory-motor circuit, visual circuits, DMN, left and right fronto-parietal circuits, salience network, etc.) (Raichle, Reference Raichle2015; Toschi, Duggento, & Passamonti, Reference Toschi, Duggento and Passamonti2017) (see Figure 2 and Supplementary Table 1 for the list of the anatomical regions involved in each network node).

Figure 2 Results of independent component analysis (ICA). A total of 15 separate large-scale functional circuits were identified during the ICA step of the image analysis pipeline (see Figure 1 and methods section in the main text for further details). Each of these circuits was successively used as “node” in the graph analysis. The list of the brain areas belonging to each network is reported in Supplementary Table 1.

Correlations between global graph indices and FFM traits

No significant associations were found between any of the FFM personality traits and: (i) the global strength (R’s<.084, p’s>.14); (ii) global clustering coefficient (R’s<.081, p’s>.15,); and (iii) global efficiency (R’s<.083, p’s>.17).

Correlations between local graph indices and FFM traits

Neuroticism

No associations, either positive and negative, were found between neuroticism scores and: (i) the nodal strength (R’s<.07, p’s>.75); (ii) local clustering coefficient (R’s<.06, p’s>.88); (iii) local efficiency (R’s<.07, p’s>.82); and (iv) betweenness centrality (R’s<.09, p’s>.59)

Extraversion

As for neuroticism, no statistically significant association was found between extraversion scores and: (i) the nodal strength (R’s<.11, p’s>.09); (ii) local clustering coefficient (R’s<.12, p’s>.04); (iii) local efficiency (R’s<.12, p’s>.09); and (iv) betweenness centrality (R’s<.11, p’s>.09).

Openness

No positive or negative associations were detected between openness scores and: (i) the nodal strength (R’s<.07, p’s>.97); (ii) local clustering coefficient (R’s<.06, p’s>.96); (iii) local efficiency (R’s<.06, p’s>.99); and (iv) betweenness centrality (R’s<.09, p’s>.27).

Agreeableness

No positive or negative associations were detected between agreeableness scores and: (i) the nodal strength (R’s<.10, p’s>.13); (ii) local clustering coefficient (R’s<.10, p’s>.12); (iii) local efficiency (R’s<.10, p’s>.15); and (iv) betweenness centrality (R’s<.08, p’s>.25).

Conscientiousness

A schematic representation of the significant associations between conscientiousness scores and the local graph measures is illustrated in Figure 3, whereas the statistical details are reported in Table 2. In summary, significantly positive correlations were found between conscientiousness scores and the local strength, local clustering coefficient, and local efficiency in the left fronto-parietal network (FPN) (R’s>.14, p’s<.01, FDR). Increased local clustering and betweenness centrality in the DMN and right FPN were also associated with higher levels of conscientiousness (R’s>.14, p’s<.005, FDR). External validation showed good replicability, with RRMSE values of around .15 in the “test” sample.

Figure 3 Schematic representation of the main results. Depending on the graph metric (Table 2), the red circle represents either the left or right fronto-parietal network (FPN) or the default mode network (DMN), whereas the black circles represents the 14 remaining network nodes. Top row: The thicker lines in individuals with high levels of conscientiousness indicate the existence of higher strength in the “communications” between the left FPN and the other brain networks. Middle row: People scoring higher in conscientiousness show a higher degree of inter-connectedness between the left FPN and DMN and the local networks consisting of direct neighbours of the left FPN and DMN. Bottom row: The DMN and right FPN have higher betweenness centrality in individuals with higher levels of conscientiousness. This means that the DMN and right FPN are “hub” nodes in conscientious people.

Table 2 Positive correlations between local graph metrics and conscientiousness scores

Note: FDR=false discovery rate; RRMSE=relative root mean square error; FPN=fronto-parietal network; DMN=default mode network.

To further explore which specific aspects of conscientiousness were linked to local graph measures, we conducted post hoc analyses that included conscientiousness facets (i.e., Order, Dutifulness, Achievement striving, Self-Discipline) as main outcome measures. As in the previous analyses, age, sex, and the other FFM traits were included in the GLM as nuisance covariates. We found that betweenness centrality in the DMN was positively associated with Dutifulness (p=.01, FDR, RRMSE=.17) and Achievement (p=.01, FDR, RRMSE=.16). Finally, betweenness centrality in the right FPN was positively associated with Dutifulness (p=.01, FDR, RRMSE=.16).

Discussion

This study provides compelling new evidence that local graph metrics based on resting-state functional imaging are significantly associated with conscientiousness in a group of 818 young adults drawn from the HCP. More specifically, we found higher nodal strength, local clustering, and local efficiency in the left FPN in people scoring higher in conscientiousness. Likewise, higher local clustering and betweenness centrality in the right FPN and DMN were positively related to conscientiousness scores. A validation approach based on a “training” and “test” split of the total data set supported the robustness, replicability, and “cross-validity” of these findings.

Overall, our results demonstrated the value of applying connectomic approaches to study large-scale functional connectivity patterns in relation to the FFM of personality. The multivariate analyses also showed that the positive association between the FPN/DMN connectivity patterns and conscientiousness was not dependent on other FFM personality traits (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion, openness, and agreeableness) or potentially confounding factors like gender and age variability. Similarly, the non-significant correlations with global connectomic measures (e.g., global clustering and efficiency) suggests that individual differences in conscientiousness are mediated by specific functional dynamics across distinct large-scale neural nodes. In the following sections, we discuss the implication of our findings to improve the understanding of the brain underpinnings of conscientiousness as well as the main strengths and limitations of the study.

FPN and DMN connectivity patterns mediate conscientiousness

The higher nodal strength in the left FPN in people scoring high in conscientiousness reflects the fact that this specific circuit “node” has heightened “communications” with the other nodes. Highly conscientious people also show higher local clustering in the left FPN, which implies that the FPN is densely interconnected to its neighbours and formed an elevated number of local aggregates (“triangles”) with its most adjacent nodes. At the same time, the local efficiency in the left FPN and the betweenness centrality in the right FPN were higher in people scoring higher in conscientiousness.

The FPN includes citoarchitecturally complex and evolutionarily recent cortices that have been associated with inter-participants variance in several cognitive measures (Mueller et al., Reference Mueller, Wang, Fox, Yeo, Sepulcre, Sabuncu and … Liu2013; Zilles, Armstrong, Schleicher, & Kretschmann, Reference Zilles, Armstrong, Schleicher and Kretschmann1988). Furthermore, a study in n=126 people from the HCP database reported that the functional connectivity patterns involving the FPNs were the most distinguishing features (“fingerprints”) that predicted variability in cognitive functioning across individuals (Finn et al., Reference Finn, Shen, Scheinost, Rosenberg, Huang, Chun and … Constable2015). Although the FPNs are typically engaged during tasks that require high levels of attention and cognitive control, their connectivity patterns at rest also predict participant-specific cognitive performance with a high degree of precision (Finn et al., Reference Finn, Shen, Scheinost, Rosenberg, Huang, Chun and … Constable2015; Miranda-Dominguez et al., Reference Miranda-Dominguez, Mills, Carpenter, Grant, Kroenke, Nigg and Fair2014). This may depend on the fact the FPN nodes act as flexible “hubs” to coordinate the activity of several other brain networks (Finn et al., Reference Finn, Shen, Scheinost, Rosenberg, Huang, Chun and … Constable2015; Miranda-Dominguez et al., Reference Miranda-Dominguez, Mills, Carpenter, Grant, Kroenke, Nigg and Fair2014).

The enhanced connectivity patterns of FPNs in people scoring high in conscientiousness can therefore be interpreted as a “sign” of increased cognitive control in these individuals, bearing in mind the shortcomings of making reverse inferences (Poldrack, Reference Poldrack2006). This is in keeping with several observations showing that conscientious people are efficient in pursuing their objectives, which is itself a critical predictor of academic or occupational success, healthy life-styles, and longevity (Noftle & Robins, Reference Noftle and Robins2007; Ozer & Benet-Martinez, Reference Ozer and Benet-Martinez2006; Roberts, Lejuez, Krueger, Richards, & Hill, Reference Roberts, Lejuez, Krueger, Richards and Hill2014; Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Stephan, Luchetti, Artese, Oshio and Terracciano2016). Our data are also consistent with past neuroimaging studies that have implicated the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and other prefrontal cortex areas (e.g., the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which is also part of the FPN) in conscientiousness (Bunge & Zelazo, Reference Bunge and Zelazo2016; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Poore, Krueger, Barbey, Solomon and Grafman2014; Jackson, Balota, & Head, Reference Jackson, Balota and Head2011; Kapogiannis et al., Reference Kapogiannis, Sutin, Davatzikos, Costa and Resnick2012; Matsuo et al., Reference Matsuo, Nicoletti, Nemoto, Hatch, Peluso, Nery and Soares2009; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Allen, Fornito, Lubman, Simmons, Pantelis and Yucel2009). Nevertheless, our results show that it is the FPN connectivity patterns with the other “nodes” which is linked to conscientiousness rather than the activity in the DLPFC/ACC in isolation. This is a key issue, especially when considering the necessity to progress from models of personality that consider the function of single brain regions, to more naturalistic frameworks that describe individual differences in behavioural traits in terms of large-scale networks’ dynamics.

Finally, we found that the DMN showed higher local clustering and betweenness centrality in relation to high conscientiousness scores. This finding was predicted on the basis of previous data showing that connectivity patterns involving the DMN predict variability in a single “positive-to-negative” behavioural axis (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and … Miller2015). The DMN also contributes to working memory performances via the dynamic reconfiguration of its interactions with other networks, which suggests that the DMN is actively involved during the execution of cognitively demanding tasks (Vatansever, Menon, Manktelow, Sahakian, & Stamatakis, Reference Vatansever, Menon, Manktelow, Sahakian and Stamatakis2015). Overall, high-level cognitive functioning is critical in human evolution and is central in the life of conscientious people. Hence, we speculate that enhanced DMN “interplay” with other nodes explains, in mechanistic terms, why conscientious individuals are able to efficiently elaborate complex plans like imaging and planning future scenarios. This hypothesis is supported by our post hoc analyses showing that local measures in the DMN (i.e., local clustering and betweenness centrality) are respectively linked to the Dutifulness facet (i.e., reliable, dependable, careful, scrupulous, and strictly adherent to rules) and Achievement Striving facet (i.e., industrious, enterprising, ambitious, purposeful, and driven) of conscientiousness.

Strengths and limitations

The main strengths of our study are: (i) the large, homogeneous, and well-characterized sample of participants in terms of FFM personality traits, demographic variables, and neuroimaging data, which in itself offers greater statistical power compared with several previous studies, and (ii) the fact that we employed robust statistical approaches to show specificity and replicability of our findings. We note, however, that the effects sizes were small (T′s~3.5), although in the typical range of other studies using similar sample sizes (Mackey et al., Reference Mackey, Chaarani, Kan, Spechler, Orr, Banaschewski and … Consortium2016; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nichols, Vidaurre, Winkler, Behrens, Glasser and … Miller2015). There was also a relatively high number of statistical tests, although we strived to attenuate this potential problem with the use of stringent statistical procedures to correct for multiple comparisons (p<.01, FDR).

The fact that conscientiousness was the sole personality trait related to “connectomic” metrics does not necessarily imply that the other FFM traits do not have such brain correlates. Several reasons why the other FFM traits were not related to functional connectomic indices may be speculated—even if not resolved by our data set. These include: (i) type II errors; (ii) non-linear relationships between personality traits and brain connectomic metrics; (iii) the fact that our group-level statistical models were multivariate rather than univariate, which means that the shared variance explained by the other FFM traits was factored out while analysing the effect of each FFM trait; (iv) the possibility that correlations between brain functional connectomic measures and other personality traits do exist but can only be revealed by “meta-trait” measures (DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, Reference DeYoung, Peterson and Higgins2002).

Perhaps more importantly, our study suggests that different neuroimaging modalities and analytical techniques may be able to reveal the unique nature of how the brain mediates each of the FFM traits. Consistent with this idea, we have recently found in n=507 individuals from the same HCP data set that measures of cortical anatomy (i.e., cortical thickness, folding, and surface area) were differently associated with each of the FFM traits (Riccelli, Toschi, Nigro, Terracciano, & Passamonti, Reference Toschi, Duggento and Passamonti2017). Hence, brain structural heterogeneity is likely to underlie variability in all FFM traits, whereas the same may not be true for functional measures that assess more transient “communication” patterns. Different functional connectivity approaches (e.g., time-variant connectivity methods) are also warranted to further explore the complexity of the neural dynamics mediating individual differences in personality (Riccelli, Passamonti, Duggento, Guerrisi, Indovina, Terracciano, et al., Reference Riccelli, Passamonti, Duggento, Guerrisi, Indovina, Terracciano and Toschi2017a; Riccelli, Passamonti, Duggento, Guerrisi, Indovina, & Toschi, Reference Riccelli, Passamonti, Duggento, Guerrisi, Indovina and Toschi2017b).

Summary and conclusions

To summarize, we found robust and specific associations between conscientiousness and graph measures of local connectivity in the FPN and DMN. These highly integrated circuits include different parts of the prefrontal and parietal cortices, a set of brain regions that have significantly evolved in human beings and have been consistently implicated in goal-setting and planning, two high-order cognitive functions in which conscientious people excel.

Acknowledgements:

The authors would like to thank Dr. Gaetano Valenza (University of Pisa) for insightful discussion of connectivity estimation using MIC.

Financial Support: Roberta Riccelli is funded by the University “Tor Vergata” of Rome, Italy, whereas Luca Passamonti is funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC) (MR/P01271X/1) at the University of Cambridge, UK. Antonio Terracciano is supported by the National Institute On Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01AG053297 and R03AG051960. Iole Indovina is funded by the Italian Ministry of Health (PE-2013-02355372). Data collection and sharing for this project was provided by the MGH-USC Human Connectome Project (HCP; Principal Investigators: Bruce Rosen, MD, PhD, Arthur W. Toga, PhD, Van J. Weeden, MD). The HCP project is supported by the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (NIDCR), the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (Principal Investigators: Bruce Rosen, MD, PhD, Martinos Center at Massachusetts General Hospital; Arthur W. Toga, PhD, University of Southern California, Van J. Weeden, MD, Martinos Center at Massachusetts General Hospital).

Conflicts of Interest: The authors have nothing to disclose.

Supplementary Material: To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2017.2

References

Albanese, D., Filosi, M., Visintainer, R., Riccadonna, S., Jurman, G. Furlanello, C. (2013). Minerva and minepy: A C engine for the MINE suite and its R, Python and MATLAB wrappers. Bioinformatics, 29(3), 407408. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bts707.Google Scholar
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di Blas, L., … De Raad, B. (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 356366. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.356.Google Scholar
Beckmann, C. F. Smith, S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 23(2), 137152. https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2003.822821.Google Scholar
Bjornebekk, A., Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Grydeland, H., Torgersen, S. Westlye, L. T. (2013). Neuronal correlates of the five factor model (FFM) of human personality: Multimodal imaging in a large healthy sample. Neuroimage, 65, 194208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.009.Google Scholar
Boccaletti, S., Latora, V., Moreno, Y., Chavez, M. Hwang, D.-U. (2006). Complex networks: Structure and dynamics. Physics Reports, 424(4), 175308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physrep.2005.10.009.Google Scholar
Bunge, S. A. Zelazo, P. D. (2016). A brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 118121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00419.x.Google Scholar
Canli, T. (2004). Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neuroticism: Learning from individual differences in emotion processing. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 11051132. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00292.x.Google Scholar
Canli, T., Sivers, H., Whitfield, S. L., Gotlib, I. H. Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science, 296(5576), 2191. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1068749.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R. (1999). Personality and psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R. Svrakic, D. M. (1991). The tridimensional personality questionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychological Reports, 69(3 Pt 1), 10471057. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1991.69.3.1047.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(12), 975990. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008.Google Scholar
Corr, P. (2006). Understanding biological psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T. McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Cremers, H., van Tol, M. J., Roelofs, K., Aleman, A., Zitman, F. G., van Buchem, M. A., … van der Wee, N. J. (2011). Extraversion is linked to volume of the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. PloS One, 6(12), e28421. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028421.Google Scholar
Cremers, H. R., Demenescu, L. R., Aleman, A., Renken, R., van Tol, M. J., van der Wee, N. J., … Roelofs, K. (2010). Neuroticism modulates amygdala-prefrontal connectivity in response to negative emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage, 49(1), 963970. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.023.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Personality neuroscience and the biology of traits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(12), 11651180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N. Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience: Brain structure and the Big Five. Psychological Science, 21(6), 820828. https://doi.org/0956797610370159 [pii]10.1177/0956797610370159.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B. Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences, 33(4), 533552. https://doi.org/Pii S0191-8869(01)00171-4 Doi 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00171-4.Google Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41(1), 417440. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221.Google Scholar
Dima, D., Friston, K. J., Stephan, K. E. Frangou, S. (2015). Neuroticism and conscientiousness respectively constrain and facilitate short-term plasticity within the working memory neural network. Human Brain Mapping, 36(10), 41584163. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22906.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1983). Psychophysiology and personality: Extraversion, neuroticism and psychoticism. In A. Gale, & J. A. Edwards (Eds.), Individual differences and psychopathology: Physiological correlates of human behaviour (Vol. 3, pp. 1330). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (2012). A model for personality. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Finn, E. S., Shen, X., Scheinost, D., Rosenberg, M. D., Huang, J., Chun, M. M., … Constable, R. T. (2015). Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(11), 16641671. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4135.Google Scholar
Fischer, H., Wik, G. Fredrikson, M. (1997). Extraversion, neuroticism and brain function: A PET study of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(2), 345352. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00027-5.Google Scholar
Forbes, C. E., Poore, J. C., Krueger, F., Barbey, A. K., Solomon, J. Grafman, J. (2014). The role of executive function and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the expression of neuroticism and conscientiousness. Social Neuroscience, 9(2), 139151. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.871333.Google Scholar
Fornito, A. Bullmore, E. T. (2015). Connectomics: A new paradigm for understanding brain disease. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(5), 733748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.02.011.Google Scholar
Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., … Polimeni, J. R. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage, 80, 105124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8(3), 249266. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(70)90069-0.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. McNaughton, N. (2003). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, A. J., Lee, P. H., Hollinshead, M. O., Bakst, L., Roffman, J. L., Smoller, J. W., & Buckner, R. L. (2012). Individual differences in amygdala-medial prefrontal anatomy link negative affect, impaired social functioning, and polygenic depression risk. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(50), 1808718100. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2531-12.2012.Google Scholar
Hu, X., Erb, M., Ackermann, H., Martin, J. A., Grodd, W. Reiterer, S. M. (2011). Voxel-based morphometry studies of personality: Issue of statistical model specification – effect of nuisance covariates. Neuroimage, 54(3), 19942005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.024.Google Scholar
Indovina, I., Riccelli, R., Staab, J. P., Lacquaniti, F. Passamonti, L. (2014). Personality traits modulate subcortical and cortical vestibular and anxiety responses to sound-evoked otolithic receptor stimulation. Journal Psychosomatic Research, 77(5), 391400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.09.005.Google Scholar
Jackson, J., Balota, D. A. Head, D. (2011). Exploring the relationship between personality and regional brain volume in healthy aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(12), 21622171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.12.009.Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Sutin, A., Davatzikos, C., Costa, P. Jr. Resnick, S. (2012). The five factors of personality and regional cortical variability in the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Human Brain Mapping, 34(11), 28292840. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22108.Google Scholar
Krebs, R. M., Schott, B. H. Duzel, E. (2009). Personality traits are differentially associated with patterns of reward and novelty processing in the human substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area. Biological Psychiatry, 65(2), 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.019.Google Scholar
Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C. F. Sharp, D. J. (2011). Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(9), 32173224. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011.Google Scholar
Liu, W. Y., Weber, B., Reuter, M., Markett, S., Chu, W. C. Montag, C. (2013). The Big Five of personality and structural imaging revisited: A VBM – DARTEL study. Neuroreport, 24(7), 375380. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e328360dad7.Google Scholar
Lu, F., Huo, Y., Li, M., Chen, H., Liu, F., Wang, Y., … Chen, H. (2014). Relationship between personality and gray matter volume in healthy young adults: A voxel-based morphometric study. PloS One, 9(2), e88763. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088763.Google Scholar
Mackey, S., Chaarani, B., Kan, K. J., Spechler, P. A., Orr, C., Banaschewski, T., … Consortium, I. (2016). Brain regions related to impulsivity mediate the effects of early adversity on antisocial behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 82(4), 275282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.027.Google Scholar
Matsuo, K., Nicoletti, M., Nemoto, K., Hatch, J. P., Peluso, M. A., Nery, F. G., & Soares, J. C. (2009). A voxel-based morphometry study of frontal gray matter correlates of impulsivity. Human Brain Mapping, 30(4), 11881195. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20588.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (1991). The five-factor model and its assessment in clinical settings. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57(3), 399414. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5703_2.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. Costa, P. T. Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(1), 8190. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175215. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. Terracciano, A. (2005). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 547561. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547.Google Scholar
McNab, J. A., Edlow, B. L., Witzel, T., Huang, S. Y., Bhat, H., Heberlein, K., … Wald, L. L. (2013). The Human Connectome Project and beyond: Initial applications of 300 mT/m gradients. Neuroimage, 80, 234245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.074.Google Scholar
Miranda-Dominguez, O., Mills, B. D., Carpenter, S. D., Grant, K. A., Kroenke, C. D., Nigg, J. T., & Fair, D. A. (2014). Connectotyping: Model based fingerprinting of the functional connectome. PloS One, 9(11), e111048. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111048.Google Scholar
Mueller, S., Wang, D., Fox, M. D., Yeo, B. T., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., … Liu, H. (2013). Individual variability in functional connectivity architecture of the human brain. Neuron, 77(3), 586595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.028.Google Scholar
Noftle, E. E. Robins, R. W. (2007). Personality predictors of academic outcomes: Big Five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 116130. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.116.Google Scholar
Nostro, A. D., Muller, V. I., Reid, A. T. Eickhoff, S. B. (2016). Correlations between personality and brain structure: A crucial role of gender. Cerebral Cortex, 27(7), 36983712. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw191.Google Scholar
Onnela, J.-P., Saramäki, J., Kertész, J. Kaski, K. (2005). Intensity and coherence of motifs in weighted complex networks. Physical Review E, 71(6), 065103. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevE.71.065103.Google Scholar
Ozer, D. J. Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401421. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127.Google Scholar
Passamonti, L., Terracciano, A., Riccelli, R., Donzuso, G., Cerasa, A., Vaccaro, M., … Quattrone, A. (2015). Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. Neuroimage, 104, 301309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.017.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 5963. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E. (2015). The restless brain: How intrinsic activity organizes brain function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 370(1668), 20140172. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0172.Google Scholar
Reshef, D. N., Reshef, Y. A., Finucane, H. K., Grossman, S. R., McVean, G., Turnbaugh, P. J., … Sabeti, P. C. (2011). Detecting novel associations in large data sets. Science, 334(6062), 15181524. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1205438.Google Scholar
Riccelli, R., Indovina, I., Staab, J. P., Nigro, S., Augimeri, A., Lacquaniti, F., & Passamonti, L. (2017). Neuroticism modulates brain visuo-vestibular and anxiety systems during a virtual rollercoaster task. Human Brain Mapping, 38(2), 715726. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23411.Google Scholar
Riccelli, R., Passamonti, L., Duggento, A., Guerrisi, M., Indovina, I., Terracciano, A., & Toschi, N. (2017a). Dynamical brain connectivity estimation using GARCH models: An application to personality neuroscience. 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Jeiju Island, Korea, 11–15 July 2017, pp. 3305–3308. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2017.8037563.Google Scholar
Riccelli, R., Passamonti, L., Duggento, A., Guerrisi, M., Indovina, I. Toschi, N. (2017b). Dynamic inter-network connectivity in the human brain. 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Jeiju Island, Korea, 11–15 July 2017, pp. 3313–3316. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2017.8037565.Google Scholar
Riccelli, R., Toschi, N., Nigro, S., Terracciano, A. Passamonti, L. (2017). Surface-based morphometry reveals the neuroanatomical basis of the five-factor model of personality. Social Cognitive Affective Neurosciences, 24(4), 671684. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw175.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Lejuez, C., Krueger, R. F., Richards, J. M. Hill, P. L. (2014). What is conscientiousness and how can it be assessed? Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 13151330. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031109.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. P., Wiebels, K., Sumner, R. L., van Mulukom, V., Grady, C. L., Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2017). An fMRI investigation of the relationship between future imagination and cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 95, 156172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.019.Google Scholar
Rodrigo, A. H., Di Domenico, S. I., Graves, B., Lam, J., Ayaz, H., Bagby, R. M., & Ruocco, A. C. (2016). Linking trait-based phenotypes to prefrontal cortex activation during inhibitory control. Social Cognitive Affective Neurosciences, 11(1), 5565. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv091.Google Scholar
Rubinov, M. Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. Neuroimage, 52(3), 10591069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (2012). Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. American Psychologist, 67(8), 603613. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029869.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. N. Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677694. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., De Brigard, F. Szpunar, K. K. (2015). Episodic future thinking and episodic counterfactual thinking: Intersections between memory and decisions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 117, 1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.12.008.Google Scholar
Servaas, M. N., van der Velde, J., Costafreda, S. G., Horton, P., Ormel, J., Riese, H., & Aleman, A. (2013). Neuroticism and the brain: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies investigating emotion processing. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(8), 15181529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.005.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., Hyvärinen, A., Varoquaux, G., Miller, K. L. Beckmann, C. F. (2014). Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. Neuroimage, 101, 738749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., Nichols, T. E., Vidaurre, D., Winkler, A. M., Behrens, T. E., Glasser, M. F., … Miller, K. L. (2015). A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nature Neurosciences, 18(11), 15651567. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4125.Google Scholar
Sporns, O. (2013). Network attributes for segregation and integration in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(2), 162171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.015.Google Scholar
Su, L., Wang, L., Shen, H., Feng, G. Hu, D. (2013). Discriminative analysis of non-linear brain connectivity in schizophrenia: An fMRI Study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 702. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00702.Google Scholar
Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., Artese, A., Oshio, A. Terracciano, A. (2016). The five-factor model of personality and physical inactivity: A meta-analysis of 16 samples. Journal of Research in Personality, 63, 2228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.05.001.Google Scholar
Terracciano, A. (2003). The Italian version of the NEO PI-R: Conceptual and empirical support for the use of targeted rotation. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(8), 18591872. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00035-7.Google Scholar
Toschi, N., Duggento, A. Passamonti, L. (2017). Functional connectivity in amygdalar-sensory/(pre)motor networks at rest: New evidence from the Human Connectome Project. European Journal of Neurosciences, 45(9), 12241229. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13544.Google Scholar
Van Essen, D. C., Ugurbil, K., Auerbach, E., Barch, D., Behrens, T., Bucholz, R., … Curtiss, S. W. (2012). The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage, 62(4), 22222231. https://10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018.Google Scholar
Vatansever, D., Menon, D. K., Manktelow, A. E., Sahakian, B. J. Stamatakis, E. A. (2015). Default mode dynamics for global functional integration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(46), 1525415262. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2135-15.2015.Google Scholar
Watts, D. J. Strogatz, S. H. (1998). Collective dynamics of “small-world” networks. Nature, 393(6684), 440442. https://doi.org/10.1038/30918.Google Scholar
Whittle, S., Allen, N. B., Fornito, A., Lubman, D. I., Simmons, J. G., Pantelis, C., & Yucel, M. (2009). Variations in cortical folding patterns are related to individual differences in temperament. Psychiatry Research, 172(1), 6874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.06.005.Google Scholar
Wright, C. I., Feczko, E., Dickerson, B. Williams, D. (2007). Neuroanatomical correlates of personality in the elderly. Neuroimage, 35(1), 263272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.039.Google Scholar
Wright, C. I., Williams, D., Feczko, E., Barrett, L. F., Dickerson, B. C., Schwartz, C. E., & Wedig, M. M. (2006). Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism. Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 18091819. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj118.Google Scholar
Zhang, Z., Sun, S., Yi, M., Wu, X. Ding, Y. (2015). MIC as an appropriate method to construct the brain functional network. BioMedical Research International, 2015, 110. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/825136.Google Scholar
Zilles, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A. Kretschmann, H. J. (1988). The human pattern of gyrification in the cerebral cortex. Anatomy and Embryology, 179(2), 173179. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304699.Google Scholar
Zuo, X.-N., Ehmke, R., Mennes, M., Imperati, D., Castellanos, F. X., Sporns, O., & Milham, M. P. (2012). Network centrality in the human functional connectome. Cerebral Cortex, 22(8), 18621875. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr269.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic and personality variables in the Human Connectome Project sample (n=818 volunteers)

Figure 1

Figure 1 Image analysis workflow. After initial pre-processing, the resting-state functional magnetic imaging (fMRI) data were used to extract a set of 15 separate brain circuits via independent components analysis (ICA). Next, participant-specific time-series from each ICA brain circuit was obtained. The maximal information coefficient (MIC), an index that assesses for linear and nonlinear relationships in big data-sets, was used to measure statistical dependency between each pair of time-series. This led to a 15×15 functional connectivity matrix at the single-participant level. The participant-specific connectivity matrices were then used to compute local and global graph measures (i.e., strength, clustering, efficiency, and betweenness centrality). Each of these graph measures, which quantify different aspects of the brain topological organization, was finally correlated with the five-factor model personality traits at the group level. BOLD=blood-oxygen-level-dependant activity.

Figure 2

Figure 2 Results of independent component analysis (ICA). A total of 15 separate large-scale functional circuits were identified during the ICA step of the image analysis pipeline (see Figure 1 and methods section in the main text for further details). Each of these circuits was successively used as “node” in the graph analysis. The list of the brain areas belonging to each network is reported in Supplementary Table 1.

Figure 3

Figure 3 Schematic representation of the main results. Depending on the graph metric (Table 2), the red circle represents either the left or right fronto-parietal network (FPN) or the default mode network (DMN), whereas the black circles represents the 14 remaining network nodes. Top row: The thicker lines in individuals with high levels of conscientiousness indicate the existence of higher strength in the “communications” between the left FPN and the other brain networks. Middle row: People scoring higher in conscientiousness show a higher degree of inter-connectedness between the left FPN and DMN and the local networks consisting of direct neighbours of the left FPN and DMN. Bottom row: The DMN and right FPN have higher betweenness centrality in individuals with higher levels of conscientiousness. This means that the DMN and right FPN are “hub” nodes in conscientious people.

Figure 4

Table 2 Positive correlations between local graph metrics and conscientiousness scores

Supplementary material: PDF

Toschi et al. supplementary material

Toschi et al. supplementary material 1

Download Toschi et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 391.8 KB