Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-xdx58 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-19T21:24:09.143Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is too much insight bad for you?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2024

Javier-David Lopez-Morinigo*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain; and Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain
Anthony S. David
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, UCL Institute of Mental Health, London, UK
*
Correspondence: Javier-David Lopez-Morinigo. Email: javier.lopez@iisgm.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Insight in psychosis is associated with reduced psychotic symptom severity, less coercive treatment and better functioning. Controversially, it has been suggested that insight may lead to depression, higher suicide risk and worse self-perceived quality of life. Future clinical trials are warranted to address this ‘insight paradox’, particularly the direction of causality.

Type
Feature
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Positive and negative effects of insight in psychosis: the insight paradox

Since the latter half of the 20th century, it has generally been assumed within the psychiatric establishment that the majority of patients with schizophrenia and related psychoses lack insight. But what do we mean by clinical insight in psychosis? Although some have criticised the rather reductive view of insight as simply agreeing with the doctor, most have attempted to uncover positive aspects of insight and have endeavoured to explore its psychological and social underpinnings. In the early 1990s, multidimensional models of insight – illness awareness, symptom relabelling and treatment complianceReference Amador, Strauss, Yale and Gorman1,Reference David2 – were proposed, which revived academic interest in the concept and initiated a three-decade period of fruitful research.Reference David3 Based on similarities with anosognosia in neurological conditions, a neurocognitive deficit model of insight has been postulated.Reference Amador and David4 The relationship between insight and cognitive ability, although meta-analytically confirmed, is surprisingly weak.Reference Nair, Palmer, Aleman and David5 Later studies showed the greater contribution of metacognition to insight in psychosis.Reference Gan, Fu and Zhu6 As a result, insight in psychosis is widely viewed within the framework of metacognition and self-awareness rather than mere psychiatric labelling or as a simple deficit.

Although a version of the concept of metacognition has been studied since ancient times, the term was first introduced by Flavell in his 1979 seminal contribution,Reference Flavell7 in which metacognition was described as ‘knowledge and cognition about cognitive phenomena’ (p. 906). Twenty years later, Wells referred to metacognition as ‘the ability to think of one's own and others' thinking’.Reference Wells8 Hence, thinking about aspects of one's thinking such as beliefs, perceptions, and whether one is suffering from an illness – aspects of clinical insight – can also be viewed as a subspecies of metacognition. A deeper theoretical debate about the conceptualisation of metacognition and its role across psychology and behaviour is beyond the scope of this article. In short, metacognition in the cognitive science world refers to indices of confidence in decision-making and performance, whereas in social psychology it tends to be used to describe aspects of self and other knowledge and people's varying abilities in this regard. Some therapies have emerged in recent years which explicitly employ concepts derived from metacognitive theory (see ref.Reference Moritz and Lysaker9 for a review). We will return to this later.

Of relevance, the above concept of clinical insight (hereafter referred to as just insight) should be distinguished from the broader construct of cognitive insight, a metacognitive domain which was put forward by Beck and colleaguesReference Beck, Baruch, Balter, Steer and Warman10 and includes the ability to evaluate one's distorted beliefs and misinterpretations and receive external feedback (self-reflectiveness) and the tendency to have overconfidence in one's conclusions (self-certainty). Although cognitive insight was intended to shed light on insight as a thinking style while avoiding issues such as treatment adherence, researchers have been surprised by the relatively weak relationship between the two forms of insight.Reference Van Camp, Sabbe and Oldenburg11

Most importantly, insight in psychosis is generally linked to positive outcomes, namely reduced psychotic symptom severity,Reference Subotnik, Ventura, Hellemann, Zito, Agee and Nuechterlein12 less use of coercion within treatment servicesReference David3 and better psychosocial functioning.Reference Lysaker, Pattison, Leonhardt, Phelps and Vohs13 On the other hand, concerns have been raised about the potential link between insight gain and some degree of demoralisation, including lower mood,Reference Subotnik, Ventura, Hellemann, Zito, Agee and Nuechterlein12,Reference Belvederi Murri, Respino, Innamorati, Cervetti, Calcagno and Pompili14 higher suicide riskReference López-Moríñigo, Ramos-Ríos, David and Dutta15 and worse self-appraised quality of life (QoL).Reference Davis, Lysaker, Salyers and Minor16 This conforms to the belief that, as Birchwood and colleagues stated in their summary of studies of ‘post psychotic depression’, ‘psychosis is indeed what patients are depressed about.Reference Birchwood, Iqbal and Upthegrove17 This dual nature of the relationship between insight and outcomes has been called the ‘insight paradox'.Reference Lysaker, Roe and Yanos18 With this in mind, we will focus on the extent to which the aforementioned putative negative effects of insight are supported by evidence, particularly in terms of causation. We will also suggest some directions for future research.

Does insight (really) lead to depression and suicidal behaviour?

Back in 1965, the British epidemiologist Bradford Hill taught us the fundamental difference between statistical association and causation. Although the relationship between mood and insight – greater insight, lower mood – is well-established in psychosisReference Belvederi Murri, Respino, Innamorati, Cervetti, Calcagno and Pompili14 as well as in other conditions, the direction of causality remains far from clear. On the one hand, developing insight into having a serious mental (or physical) illness could be thought to lead directly to feelings of hopelessness and other depressive symptoms, that is, demoralisation. On the other hand, the well-known cognitive biases associated with low mood tend to result in a more pessimistic way of thinking (e.g. about illnesses), which is sometimes attributed to the depressive realism model, i.e. lower mood giving rise to greater insight at the assessment. A prospective longitudinal approach would appear to be one way of teasing these effects out. However, the careful attempts of Iqbal and colleagues to do so using measures across at least four time points found that insight moved up and down in lockstep with depression during the course of psychosisReference Iqbal, Birchwood, Chadwick and Trower19 – it appeared that they were two sides of the same coin.

Depression is a well-known major risk factor for suicide both in the general population and in schizophrenia and related disorders, particularly in early psychosis. In a systematic review, insight was weakly linked to suicide risk in individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders, but depression may mediate this association.Reference López-Moríñigo, Ramos-Ríos, David and Dutta15 Consistent with this, three large independent first-episode psychosis cohort studies from the UK GAP (n = 112) and AESOP (n = 181) projectsReference Lopez-Morinigo, Di Forti, Ajnakina, Wiffen, Morgan and Doody20 and from the PAFIP programme (n = 397) from Spain,Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Terán, Moríñigo, Rivero, Setién-Suero and Al-Halabi21 with follow-up periods ranging from 3 to 10 years, failed to confirm a direct relationship between insight and suicide risk. An apparent association might be explained by two confounders, namely previous suicide attempts and depression, both of which were associated with baseline (higher) insight levels and emerged as the strongest predictors of future suicidal events in individuals with first-episode psychosis.

Suicidal behaviour, particularly suicide (completion), is a complex biopsychosocial phenomenon, hence unlikely to be explained by any single factor, including depression. One common theme in predictors of suicide in those with mental health conditions is dropping out of and non-adherence with treatment,Reference Cassidy, Yang, Kapczinski and Passos22 both of which are inextricably linked to decline in the insight that one has a disorder that might be amenable to treatment. To better understand the interrelationship between contributing factors (including depression and insight) leading to such a tragic outcome as suicide, long-term intervention studies are warranted. Contrary to the commonly held view among clinicians about the demoralising impact of insight, one might hypothesise that enhancing insight may reduce the long-term risk of depressive symptoms and suicidal behaviour via improved adherence to (effective) treatment and reduced hopelessness, although this remains to be established.

Insight and Quality of Life: what do we know?

The World Health Organization defines Quality of Life (QoL) as ‘an individual's perception of their position in life in the context of the culture and value systems in which they live and in relation to their goals, expectations, standards and concerns’.23 In other words, QoL is a subjective measure of one's psychosocial functioning, which must include perception of illness. In keeping with this, the focus of psychiatric research, particularly intervention studies, has switched from objective outcome measures to so-called patient-reported outcome and experience measures, commonly known as PROMs and PREMs, respectively. QoL is thus likely to be the most important treatment goal for patients. The question arises: can (too much) insight negatively affect self-perceived QoL?

Among other determinants of QoL in schizophrenia and related psychoses, global insight has been found in meta-analyses to have a small negative effect – greater insight, worse QoL – which appears to be more relevant for those individuals who present with less severe symptoms.Reference Davis, Lysaker, Salyers and Minor16 One complication in interpreting this effect is the type of assessment of QoL (assessor-rated versus self-report), which influences the relationship between insight and QoL. In this regard, a re-analysis of data from the US multicentre CATIE study found that participants with poor insight more positively self-perceived their QoL compared with the corresponding QoL ratings from the treating physician, partially supporting the insight paradox notionReference Lysaker, Weiden, Sun, O'Sullivan and McEvoy24 but raising uncomfortable questions about objectivity. Indeed, more insightful (and possibly more depressed) patients are more likely to develop thoughts on the negative consequences of their illness, which would naturally affect self-appraisals of QoL negatively. But this is incompatible with the well-established association between insight in psychosis and better objectively assessed global functioningReference Canal-Rivero, Ayesa-Arriola, Ruiz-Veguilla, Ortiz-García de la Foz, Labad and Crespo-Facorro25 and highlights the different emphasis of the two constructs.

In order to improve patient QoL one might be tempted to counsel against making the patient aware of being ill. In clinical settings, it is not uncommon for family members to request that certain information, including a diagnosis of schizophrenia (or cancer, dementia, etc.), should be withheld from the patient, pitching legitimate concerns about welfare against autonomy and the patient's right to be informed. The assertion that insight and knowledge about one's illness are detrimental, even if found to be true to some extent, would not necessarily justify the withholding of information. For example, it may be that an immediate blow to self-esteem was more than compensated for by the benefits of knowledge, openness and cooperation. In other words, knowing the truth, including confirmation of a diagnosis of a disabling illness, need not automatically lead to poorer QoL, at least in the medium and long-term, although this is a topic for empirical research.

Above and beyond the insight paradox: other contributors

The potentially negative impact of insight on mood, suicide risk and QoL seems to be moderated by internalised stigma, which can be defined as the degree to which a person has endorsed societal negative stereotypes and stigmatising beliefs about having a mental illness. In particular, a cluster analysis of (n = 75) individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders revealed that within the good insight group, those who rejected stigmatising beliefs achieved better functional outcomes than those who endorsed them.Reference Lysaker, Roe and Yanos18

In addition, a cross-sectional study from Italy of (n = 89) patients with schizophrenia aimed to investigate putative moderators and mediators of the association between depression and insight. The results showed socioeconomic status, symptom severity and adherence to treatment to moderate this association, which was more pronounced in individuals with lower socioeconomic status, more severe symptoms and poorer engagement with services.Reference Belvederi Murri, Amore, Calcagno, Respino, Marozzi and Masotti26

What about causality? Looking forward to intervention studies

Although there is some evidence to support the so-called insight paradox, this is not to say that insight should not be improved in the clinical setting. Rather, we propose that efforts to enhance insight should be combined with measures to improve mastery over awareness-related psychological distress, to relieve low mood and decrease internalised stigma, including both psychological and pharmacological treatments. Moreover, early interventions regarding insight, along with measures to correct self-stigma, before the harmful secondary consequences of a psychotic illness have had time to develop, may result in better outcomes. In other words, interventions to improve insight should be delivered along with the development of coping strategies from a patient-centred approach. For example: ‘Yes, I have an illness, but I can see a path to recovery by working positively with my doctors/carers’. However, there is a lack of long-term studies examining the dynamic relationships among insight, depression, suicide risk and QoL, so that the key question we asked about causation remains unanswered.

To this end, longitudinal clinical trials, which may aid in untangling causality, are needed. In particular, it is worth noting that both depression and insight can evolve over time. Hence, the study of this complex interplay requires follow-up studies in which putative confounders and mediators are controlled for, such as in the context of randomised clinical trials. More specifically, if an insight improving intervention was shown to reduce the risk of depression and/or suicidality, let alone not worsen them, a conclusion might be drawn that insight can be good for you and the close association between insight and depression/suicidality could be picked apart.

So far, interventions for improving insight, including psychoeducation, psychoanalytically oriented therapies, cognitive–behavioural therapy, video-recorded self-observation and antipsychotics, have yielded modest results.Reference Phelan and Sigala27 Certainly, the management of poor insight in psychosis represents both a research gap and a major unmet clinical need. Thanks to meta-analyses,Reference Lopez-Morinigo, Ajnakina, Martínez, Escobedo-Aedo, Ruiz-Ruano and Sánchez-Alonso28 a newly developed metacognitive intervention, metacognitive training (but not other metacognitive-oriented therapies such as metacognitive therapy and metacognitive reflection and insight therapy), has emerged as an evidence-based treatment for changing insight in psychosis. These findings are in full agreement with the metacognitive model of insight, according to which lack of insight in psychotic disorders may be attributable to psychosis-related metacognitive deficits – hence, insight could be enhanced via improving metacognitive performance.Reference David3 Although the maximum follow-up period of the meta-analysed trials was 6 months, no negative effects on mood were observed.Reference Lopez-Morinigo, Ajnakina, Martínez, Escobedo-Aedo, Ruiz-Ruano and Sánchez-Alonso28 Longer-term, it remains unclear whether these benefits for insight are sustained and whether the insight improvement comes with demoralising effects.

Future large-scale long-term clinical trials should therefore include insight measures (as the primary outcome) and symptom severity, mood, suicidal behaviour, functioning and QoL (as secondary outcomes). From a statistical point of view, structural equation modelling techniques may offer a better understanding of the putative dynamic causal associations of insight with depression and suicide risk, as they can address latent variables as well as multiple observable variables (see ref. Reference MacCallum and Austin29 for a review). Previous literature has yielded inconclusive results on the efficacy of antidepressants for the management of depression in schizophrenia, although their combination with antipsychotics has been found to result in greater effects on negative symptoms.Reference Gregory, Mallikarjun and Upthegrove30,Reference Galling, Vernon, Pagsberg, Wadhwa, Grudnikoff and Seidman31 Regrettably, insight data from selected studies were not meta-analysed; hence, these studies of antidepressants for psychosis represent a missed opportunity to test the insight paradox. Nevertheless, depression in psychosis remains under-recognised in routine clinical practice and under-researched. This is of concern given its high prevalence among people with schizophrenia, which has been recently estimated at 32.6%,Reference Etchecopar-Etchart, Korchia, Loundou, Llorca, Auquier and Lançon32 and the strong association with increased suicide risk, particularly in early psychosis.Reference Lopez-Morinigo, Di Forti, Ajnakina, Wiffen, Morgan and Doody20,Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Terán, Moríñigo, Rivero, Setién-Suero and Al-Halabi21

Turning to high-risk studies, we can ask whether interventions aimed at reducing depression in people at risk of psychosis (or in the early stages of the disorder) lead to worsening of measures of insight (and probable worsening of positive psychotic symptoms). The latter does not seem to be the case,Reference Raballo, Poletti and Preti33 but we know of no studies that have measured insight specifically. Alternatively, patient-initiated treatment discontinuation, which is linked to impaired insight and might be regarded as a proxy measure, tends to be more commonly looked at in clinical trials, particularly in psychopharmacological research. Notably, the impact of antidepressant treatment on depressive symptoms in schizophrenia, although relatively small, did not affect treatment discontinuation rates.Reference Galling, Vernon, Pagsberg, Wadhwa, Grudnikoff and Seidman31 In other words, in contrast to what one may expect based on the insight paradox, improvement in depressive symptoms after antidepressant treatment does not seem to be associated with poorer insight in psychotic disorders, yet this remains a hypothesis worthy of direct testing.

Conclusion

The insight paradox predicts that insight and depression (and hence suicidality) are locked together like conjoined twins, but it is possible that insight and depression, while appearing to be correlated, are potentially dissociable. In particular, if – as appears to be the case thus far – any insight-improving interventions, while they may or may not be effective in their primary aim, do not have a negative impact on outcomes (including mood) over time, this would undermine the generalisability of the insight paradox assertion. Although pending future confirmation, the same seems true for depression-alleviating treatments – that is, they do not appear to worsen insight. So one may eventually reach the conclusion that ‘too much insight is not (necessarily) bad for you’.

Data availability

The data availability statement is not applicable to this article since no new data was created or analysed for the preparation and writing of this feature.

Acknowledgements

We thank our many collaborators in previous studies on insight in psychosis and depression/suicidal behaviour, including Rina Dutta, Rosa Ayesa-Arriola, Manuel Canal-Rivero, Benedicto Crespo-Facorro and Manuel J. Cuesta.

Author contributions

Both authors contributed equally to the paper.

Funding

This work was not supported by any specific funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit organisation.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Amador, XF, Strauss, DH, Yale, SA, Gorman, JM. Awareness of illness in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1991; 17: 113–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, AS. Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 156: 798808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, AS. Insight and psychosis: the next 30 years. Br J Psychiatry 2020; 217: 521–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amador, XF, David, AS. Insight and Psychosis: Awareness of Illness in Schizophrenia and Related Disorders (2nd edn). Oxford University Press, 2005.Google Scholar
Nair, A, Palmer, EC, Aleman, A, David, AS. Relationship between cognition, clinical and cognitive insight in psychotic disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2014; 152: 191200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gan, J, Fu, H, Zhu, X. Relationships between multiple dimensions of insight and neurocognition, metacognition, and social cognition: a meta-analysis. J Nerv Ment Dis 2022; 210: 577–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, JH. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. Am Psychol 1979; 34: 906–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, A. Metacognition and cognitive-behaviour therapy: a special issue. Clin Psychol Psychother 1999; 6: 71–2.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moritz, S, Lysaker, PH. Metacognition – what did James H. Flavell really say and the implications for the conceptualization and design of metacognitive interventions. Schizophr Res 2018; 201: 20–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, AT, Baruch, E, Balter, JM, Steer, RA, Warman, DM. A new instrument for measuring insight: the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale. Schizophr Res 2004; 68: 319–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Camp, LSC, Sabbe, BGC, Oldenburg, JFE. Cognitive insight: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2017; 55: 1224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subotnik, KL, Ventura, J, Hellemann, GS, Zito, MF, Agee, ER, Nuechterlein, KH. Relationship of poor insight to neurocognition, social cognition, and psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2020; 220: 164–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lysaker, PH, Pattison, ML, Leonhardt, BL, Phelps, S, Vohs, JL. Insight in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: relationship with behavior, mood and perceived quality of life, underlying causes and emerging treatments. World Psychiatry 2018; 17: 1223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belvederi Murri, M, Respino, M, Innamorati, M, Cervetti, A, Calcagno, P, Pompili, M, et al. Is good insight associated with depression among patients with schizophrenia? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2015; 162: 234–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Moríñigo, JD, Ramos-Ríos, R, David, AS, Dutta, R. Insight in schizophrenia and risk of suicide: a systematic update. Compr Psychiatry 2012; 53: 313–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, BJ, Lysaker, PH, Salyers, MP, Minor, KS. The insight paradox in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the relationship between clinical insight and quality of life. Schizophr Res 2020; 223: 917.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birchwood, M, Iqbal, Z, Upthegrove, R. Psychological pathways to depression in schizophrenia: studies in acute psychosis, post psychotic depression and auditory hallucinations. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005; 255: 202–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lysaker, PH, Roe, D, Yanos, PT. Toward understanding the insight paradox: internalized stigma moderates the association between insight and social functioning, hope, and self-esteem among people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33: 192–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iqbal, Z, Birchwood, M, Chadwick, P, Trower, P. Cognitive approach to depression and suicidal thinking in psychosis. 2. Testing the validity of a social ranking model. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 522–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez-Morinigo, J-D, Di Forti, M, Ajnakina, O, Wiffen, BD, Morgan, K, Doody, GA, et al. Insight and risk of suicidal behaviour in two first-episode psychosis cohorts: effects of previous suicide attempts and depression. Schizophr Res 2019; 204: 80–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayesa-Arriola, R, Terán, JMP, Moríñigo, JDL, Rivero, MC, Setién-Suero, E, Al-Halabi, S, et al. The dynamic relationship between insight and suicidal behavior in first episode psychosis patients over 3-year follow-up. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2018; 28: 1161–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, RM, Yang, F, Kapczinski, F, Passos, IC. Risk factors for suicidality in patients with schizophrenia: a systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression of 96 studies. Schizophr Bull 2018; 44: 787–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The World Health Organization (WHO). WHOQOL: Measuring Quality of Life. WHO, 2012 (https://www.who.int/tools/whoqol).Google Scholar
Lysaker, PH, Weiden, PJ, Sun, X, O'Sullivan, AK, McEvoy, JP. Impaired insight in schizophrenia: impact on patient-reported and physician-reported outcome measures in a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry 2022; 22: 574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canal-Rivero, M, Ayesa-Arriola, R, Ruiz-Veguilla, M, Ortiz-García de la Foz, V, Labad, J, Crespo-Facorro, B. Insight trajectories and their impact on psychosocial functioning: a 10-year follow-up study in first episode psychosis patients. J Psychopathol Clin Sci 2022; 131: 808–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belvederi Murri, M, Amore, M, Calcagno, P, Respino, M, Marozzi, V, Masotti, M, et al. The ‘insight paradox’ in schizophrenia: magnitude, moderators and mediators of the association between insight and depression. Schizophr Bull 2016; 42: 1225–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelan, S, Sigala, N. The effect of treatment on insight in psychotic disorders – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2022; 244: 126–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez-Morinigo, J-D, Ajnakina, O, Martínez, AS-E, Escobedo-Aedo, P-J, Ruiz-Ruano, VG, Sánchez-Alonso, S, et al. Can metacognitive interventions improve insight in schizophrenia spectrum disorders? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2020; 50: 2289–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCallum, RC, Austin, JT. Applications of structural equation modeling in psychological research. Annu Rev Psychol 2000; 51: 201–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, A, Mallikarjun, P, Upthegrove, R. Treatment of depression in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2017; 211: 198204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galling, B, Vernon, JA, Pagsberg, AK, Wadhwa, A, Grudnikoff, E, Seidman, AJ, et al. Efficacy and safety of antidepressant augmentation of continued antipsychotic treatment in patients with schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2018; 137: 187205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etchecopar-Etchart, D, Korchia, T, Loundou, A, Llorca, P-M, Auquier, P, Lançon, C, et al. Comorbid major depressive disorder in schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull 2021; 47: 298308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raballo, A, Poletti, M, Preti, A. Do antidepressants prevent transition to psychosis in individuals at clinical high-risk (CHR-P)? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2023; 53: 4550–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.