Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T15:54:36.851Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do framing effects debunk moral beliefs?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2019

Kelsey McDonald
Affiliation:
Philosophy Department, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. kelsey.mcdonald@duke.edusiyuan.yin@duke.edutara.weese@duke.eduws66@duke.eduhttps://sites.duke.edu/wsa/
Siyuan Yin
Affiliation:
Philosophy Department, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. kelsey.mcdonald@duke.edusiyuan.yin@duke.edutara.weese@duke.eduws66@duke.eduhttps://sites.duke.edu/wsa/
Tara Weese
Affiliation:
Philosophy Department, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. kelsey.mcdonald@duke.edusiyuan.yin@duke.edutara.weese@duke.eduws66@duke.eduhttps://sites.duke.edu/wsa/
Walter Sinnott-Armstrong
Affiliation:
Philosophy Department, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708. kelsey.mcdonald@duke.edusiyuan.yin@duke.edutara.weese@duke.eduws66@duke.eduhttps://sites.duke.edu/wsa/

Abstract

May argues that framing effects do not undermine moral beliefs, because they affect only a minority of moral judgments in small ways. We criticize his estimates of the extent of framing effects on moral judgments, and then we argue that framing effects would cause trouble for moral judgments even if his estimates were correct.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Demaree-Cotton, J. (2016) Do framing effects make moral intuitions unreliable? Philosophical Psychology 29(1):122.Google Scholar
Helzer, E. G. & Pizarro, D. A. (2011) Dirty liberals! Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science 22(4):517–22.Google Scholar
Kelly, D., Stich, S., Haley, K. J., Eng, S. J. & Fessler, D. M. (2007) Harm, affect, and the moral/conventional distinction. Mind and Language 22(2):117–31.Google Scholar
Killgore, W. D. S., Kahn-Greene, E. T., Lipizzi, E. L., Newman, R. A., Kamimori, G. H. & Balkin, T. J. (2007) Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Medicine 9(5):517–26.Google Scholar
Kühberger, A. (1998) The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organizational behavior and human decision processes 75(1):2355.Google Scholar
May, J. (2018) Regard for reason in the moral mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nadelhoffer, T. & Feltz, A. (2008) The actor–observer bias and moral intuitions: Adding fuel to Sinnott-Armstrong's fire. Neuroethics 1(2):133–44.Google Scholar
Olsen, O. K., Pallesen, S. & Eid, J. (2010) The impact of partial sleep deprivation on moral reasoning in military officers. Sleep 33(8):1086–90.Google Scholar
Petrinovich, L. & O'Neill, P. (1996) Influence of wording and framing effects on moral intuitions. Ethology and Sociobiology 17(3):145–71.Google Scholar
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L. & Jordan, A. H. (2008) Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34(8):1096–09.Google Scholar
Tanner, C. & Medin, D. L. (2004) Protected values: No omission bias and no framing effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 11(1):185–91.Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P. & DeSteno, D. (2006) Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science 17(6):476–47.Google Scholar