Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:24:38.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal associations between specific types of emotional reactivity and psychological, physical health, and school adjustment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 January 2022

Jessica M. Dollar*
Affiliation:
Department of Kinesiology and Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
Nicole B. Perry
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Susan D. Calkins
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
Lilly Shanahan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Jacobs Center for Productive Youth Development, University of Zurich, Zürich, Switzerland
Susan P. Keane
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
Lenka Shriver
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
Laurie Wideman
Affiliation:
Department of Kinesiology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
*
Corresponding author. Jessica M. Dollar, email: jmdollar@uncg.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Using a multimethod, multiinformant longitudinal design, we examined associations between specific forms of positive and negative emotional reactivity at age 5, children’s effortful control (EC), emotion regulation, and social skills at age 7, and adolescent functioning across psychological, academic, and physical health domains at ages 15/16 (N = 383). We examined how distinct components of childhood emotional reactivity directly and indirectly predict domain-specific forms of adolescent adjustment, thereby identifying developmental pathways between specific types of emotional reactivity and adjustment above and beyond the propensity to express other forms of emotional reactivity. Age 5 high-intensity positivity was associated with lower age 7 EC and more adolescent risk-taking; age 5 low-intensity positivity was associated with better age 7 EC and adolescent cardiovascular health, providing evidence for the heterogeneity of positive emotional reactivity. Indirect effects indicated that children’s age 7 social skills partially explain several associations between age 5 fear and anger reactivity and adolescent adjustment. Moreover, age 5 anger reactivity, low-, and high-intensity positivity were associated with adolescent adjustment via age 7 EC. The findings from this interdisciplinary, long-term longitudinal study have significant implications for prevention and intervention work aiming to understand the role of emotional reactivity in the etiology of adjustment and psychopathology.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

There is little doubt that children’s early experience and expression of emotions are associated with later well-being or maladjustment (e.g., Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012; Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Eisenberg, Damon and Richard2006). For example, early emotional expressions predict later mental (e.g., Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Degnan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009) and physical health (e.g., Anzman-Frasca et al., Reference Anzman-Frasca, Stifter and Birch2012), behavior problems (e.g., Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Putnam and Rothbart2012), and social (e.g., Dougherty, Reference Dougherty2006) and school functioning (e.g., Dollar et al., Reference Dollar, Perry, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2018). Although these direct links have been demonstrated, specific forms of positive and negative emotions are rarely considered; this is especially true when examined simultaneously, which is necessary to identify the most salient forms of emotional reactivity in predicting adjustment. Moreover, most studies do not examine outcomes across multiple domains which limits the impact on preventive interventions. Thus, a greater understanding of how distinct components of emotional reactivity predict later domain-specific adjustment above and beyond the propensity to express other forms of emotional reactivity is needed.

In addition, development is the result of a cascade-based acquisition of developmentally-salient skills that influence subsequent adaptation to the challenges of the next developmental period (e.g., Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). Therefore, a developmentally informed theory of risk or well-being must move beyond direct effects and aim to understand the mechanisms that explain how and why these associations transpire. One area of interest to developmental scientists has been the associations between children’s emotional reactivity and self-regulation in developmental trajectories toward optimal functioning. Behavioral and emotional self-regulatory skills are more difficult to acquire and employ for emotionally reactive children (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, McKay, Iddins, Robertson and Rittmueller2009; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland, Spinrad, Fabes, Shepard, Reiser and Guthrie2001; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum-Wilkens2010; Stifter & Spinrad, Reference Stifter and Spinrad2002); yet these self-regulatory skills are essential for subsequent adjustment (see Calkins & Perry, Reference Calkins, Perry and Cicchetti2016; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016 for reviews).

Social functioning (i.e., social skills) also may explain variation in adjustment trajectories because it is both predicted by early emotional reactivity and predictive of well-being in adolescence and beyond (e.g., Parker et al., Reference Parker, Rubin, Erath, Wojslawowicz, Buskirk, Cicchetti and Cohen2006). Thus, the regulation of emotion and behavior, as well as the development of strong social skills, by middle childhood are important developmental mechanisms to consider when investigating the link between early emotional reactivity and subsequent outcomes. The current study examines the longitudinal associations among specific types of positive (i.e., low-intensity positivity, high-intensity positivity) and negative (i.e., anger, fear) emotional reactivity in early childhood, regulatory (effortful control [EC], emotion regulation [ER]) and social skills in middle childhood, and adolescent adjustment across multiple developmental domains (risk-taking, psychological and physical health, school problems). This work is essential to better understand the role of distinct components of early emotional reactivity in the etiology of individual differences underlying pathways toward domain-specific adolescent outcomes.

Childhood emotional reactivity and adjustment

Although positive and negative emotions are experienced and expressed by everyone, there are notable individual differences in the intensity and frequency of emotions. These individual differences are often discussed as reflecting, at least in part, a child’s temperament, defined as early emerging, relatively stable individual differences in affect, activity level, attention, and self-regulation (Rothbart, Reference Rothbart2011; Shiner et al., Reference Shiner, Buss, McClowry, Putnam, Saudino and Zentner2012). All emotions serve adaptive purposes (Saarni et al., Reference Saarni, Campos, Camras, Witherington, Eisenberg, Damon and Lerner2006), but for some children the experience of intense and/or frequent forms of certain emotions may have maladaptive consequences (Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016).

Below, we provide a selective overview of empirical work that links anger, fear, and positive emotional reactivity to psychological, school, and physical health outcomes. In line with a growing literature that considers fine-grained aspects of temperament within one study (Buss et al., Reference Buss, Kiel, Morales and Robinson2014; Dollar & Buss, Reference Dollar and Buss2014; Morales et al., Reference Morales, Tang, Bowers, Miller, Buzzell, Smith and Fox2021), we emphasize the importance of accounting for the propensity to experience other forms of emotional reactivity when investigating the associations between distinct types of emotional reactivity and adjustment. Moreover, we argue that it is necessary to consider how these associations are explained through intermediate skills in middle childhood in order to best inform intervention efforts targeting regulatory or social skills.

We focus on the adolescent outcomes of risk-taking, cardiovascular risk, emotional symptomatology, and school problems, each of which has been linked to negative and/or positive emotional reactivity. Given that the adolescent period is characterized by rapid biological changes, heightened stress, conflicting role demands, and increased impulsivity and sensation seeking (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2007), examining the social, emotional, and behavioral predictors of maladaptive adolescent psychological and physical health is of considerable importance and may have lifelong consequences (e.g., Haller et al., Reference Haller, Handley, Chassin and Bountress2010; Hartiala et al., Reference Hartiala, Magnussen, Kajander, Knuuti, Ukkonen, Saraste and Raitakari2012; Monroe et al., Reference Monroe, Slavich, Georgiades, Gotlib and Hammen2014).

Emotional reactivity and psychopathology

Negative emotional reactivity

There is little dispute that early proclivities to express negative emotional reactivity is predictive of behavior problems and poorer mental health in the internalizing and externalizing realms (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Bayles, Bennett, Ridge, Brown, Pepler and Rubin1991; Bohlin & Hagekull, Reference Bohlin and Hagekull2009; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland, Spinrad, Fabes, Shepard, Reiser and Guthrie2001; McClowry et al., Reference McClowry, Giangrande, Tommasini, Clinton, Foreman, Lynch and Ferketich1994). However, it is important to move beyond a general measure of negative emotional reactivity to consider specific negative emotions (i.e., anger, fear), given that each form of reactivity differentially predicts children’s trajectories (e.g., Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016).

Anger reactivity, which is often a response to blocked goals (Saarni et al., Reference Saarni, Campos, Camras, Witherington, Eisenberg, Damon and Lerner2006), is positively associated with childhood and adolescent behavior problems (Frick & Morris, Reference Frick and Morris2004; Perry et al., Reference Perry, Calkins, Dollar, Keane, O’Brien and Shanahan2017; Rydell et al., Reference Rydell, Berlin and Bohlin2003). In a sample of children followed from toddlerhood through adolescence, high anger at age 5 was associated with a greater probability of a stable pattern of externalizing behaviors from age 2 to 15 (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Calkins, Dollar, Keane, O’Brien and Shanahan2017). Furthermore, children with high anger reactivity sometimes develop cooccurring externalizing and internalizing symptoms (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Eisenberg, Valiente and Spinrad2016).

Fear reactivity, defined as negative affect related to anticipated novelty, threat, distress, and/or pain (Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Eisenberg, Damon and Richard2006), is most often linked with high internalizing problems (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Hirshfeld-Becker, Rosenbaum, Hérot, Friedman, Snidman and Faraone2001; Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Degnan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Rydell et al., Reference Rydell, Berlin and Bohlin2003). For example, behaviorally inhibited children, a temperamental group of children characterized by fearful and avoidant responses to unfamiliar people, objects, and situations (Kagan, Reference Kagan2018), are at greater risk for anxiety, often specifically social anxiety (e.g., Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Degnan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Snidman and Kagan1999), in adolescence.

Positive emotional reactivity

Positive emotional reactivity is linked with both well-being and maladjustment (Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016). Although positive emotional reactivity is often considered as one construct on a spectrum from low-intensity positivity to high-intensity positivity, the seemingly contradictory literature may be a function of its heterogeneous nature (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007). That is, the mixed findings may be explained by whether researchers emphasize the high approach or low-intensity elements of positive affect in their measurement (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007; Polak-Toste & Gunnar, Reference Polak-Toste, Gunnar, Marshall and Fox2006).

Low-intensity positivity, including pleasure (smiling, laughing) experienced in nonstimulating activities, may be an adaptive quality by suggesting the ability to experience pleasure in calm, low-risk situations. Indeed, childhood positive emotional reactivity is protective against developing internalizing difficulties, such that low childhood positive reactivity is linked with electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetries that characterize depressed individuals, and a greater likelihood of having mothers with a history of depressive disorders (Durbin et al., Reference Durbin, Klein, Hayden, Buckley and Moerk2005; Shankman et al., Reference Shankman, Tenke, Bruder, Durbin, Hayden and Klein2005). Further, low positive emotional reactivity at age 3 predicted depressive symptoms at age 10, after controlling for negative emotional reactivity and depressive behaviors (Dougherty et al., Reference Dougherty, Klein, Durbin, Hayden and Olino2010).

In contrast, children who experience high levels of high-intensity positivity (i.e., excitement) may have difficulty controlling their behavior and finding pleasure in calm situations; in turn, these children may miss opportunities that support learning social, behavioral, and academic skills (Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016). Increased positive emotional reactivity, for example, is linked with more externalizing behaviors (e.g., Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Ahadi and Hershey1994), although this effect is sometimes moderated by regulatory skills (Rydell et al., Reference Rydell, Berlin and Bohlin2003). Further, intense positive emotional reactivity is a defining characteristic of exuberance, a temperamental style that describes children who show positive affect, approach, activity level, and sometimes impulsivity (Degnan et al., Reference Degnan, Hane, Henderson, Moas, Reeb-Sutherland and Fox2011; Dollar et al., Reference Dollar, Stifter and Buss2017, Putnam & Rothbart, Reference Putnam and Rothbart2006; Putnam & Stifter, Reference Putnam and Stifter2005), which is linked to greater externalizing symptomology (Putnam & Stifter, Reference Putnam and Stifter2005; Stifter et al., Reference Stifter, Putnam and Jahromi2008). This work underscores the need to consider low- and high-intensity positivity in one study to identify if the heterogeneity in positive reactivity explains the mixed findings. If so, intervention efforts can be better informed as to the intensities of positivity that are protective or maladaptive for specific forms of adjustment.

Emotional reactivity and risk-taking behaviors

Negative emotional reactivity

An important aspect of adolescent maladjustment involves the propensity to engage in risk-taking (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2007), which is highly related to externalizing behaviors. Anger reactivity is often an important predictor of risk-taking behaviors (e.g., Gambetti & Giusberti, Reference Gambetti and Giusberti2009; Lerner & Keltner, Reference Lerner and Keltner2001; Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Derryberry, Hershey, Molfese and Molfese2000; Sigfusdottir et al., Reference Sigfusdottir, Farkas and Silver2004). Indeed, anger is more strongly related to alcohol and drug use than other negative emotions in adolescence and adulthood (McCreary & Sadava, Reference McCreary and Sadava2000; Pardini et al., Reference Pardini, Lochman and Wells2004), suggesting that anger-prone individuals may engage in substance use to deal with their intense negative emotions and social difficulties. More complex associations have emerged for children high in fear reactivity, such that these associations depend on gender and related behaviors (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Tremblay, Pagani and Vitaro1997; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Fox, Lejuez, Reynolds, Henderson, Perez-Edgar and Pine2010). For example, high behavioral inhibition increased risk for substance-related problems for boys but protected against these problems among girls (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Fox, Lejuez, Reynolds, Henderson, Perez-Edgar and Pine2010).

Positive emotional reactivity

Heightened positive emotional reactivity in adolescence also predicts risk-taking behaviors, including smoking (Romer & Jamieson, Reference Romer, Jamieson and Slovic2001), drug use (Romer & Hennessy, Reference Romer and Hennessy2007), and lifetime marijuana use (Creemers et al., Reference Creemers, Dijkstra, Vollebergh, Ormel, Verhulst and Huizink2010). In childhood, research has focused on exuberant children. Lahat et al. (Reference Lahat, Degnan, White, McDermott, Henderson, Lejuez and Fox2012) found that children high in exuberance and low in attention-shifting had a high propensity for childhood risk-taking. This suggests that individuals who experience high-intensity positivity may be more likely to engage in impulsive, risky behaviors possibly because their initial assessment of the situation’s risk is outweighed by a strong experience of positive affect. Moreover, childhood high-intensity positivity is likely to evolve into later extraversion, impulsivity, sensation-seeking, and lowered self-regulation (Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012; Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Eisenberg, Damon and Richard2006). Together, these motivations and tendencies could contribute to adolescents’ increased engagement in risky behavior. Longitudinal developmental studies are needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Emotional reactivity and physical health

Negative emotional reactivity

Given that mental and physical health are intertwined, a growing number of studies have examined how emotional reactivity and physical health are linked. For example, heightened anger is linked to cardiovascular risk (CVR; e.g., Harburg et al., Reference Harburg, Julius, Kaciroti, Gleiberman and Schork2003), cancer (e.g., Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Groer, Davis, Droppleman, Mozingo and Pierce2000), and elevated blood pressure and heart rate (Hauber et al., Reference Hauber, Rice, Howell and Carmon1998). Evidence of these associations begins as early as adolescence and continues throughout adulthood. However, no known work has examined if high levels of childhood anger is predictive of lowered later physical health. Moreover, increased stress responses that come with frequent fear could increase children’s risk for CVR (Dhabhar, Reference Dhabhar2014; McEwen, Reference McEwen1998). This is particularly salient for intervention efforts because identification of children more prone to develop early CVR could significantly lower the public health burden associated with cardiovascular disease (CVD).

Positive emotional reactivity

Adults who experience more positive emotion are generally healthier (e.g., Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Alper, Doyle and Treanor2006) and live longer (e.g., Cohen & Pressman, Reference Cohen and Pressman2006). However, existing studies are difficult to synthesize because measures of positivity vary greatly by study and often examine general constructs (e.g., life satisfaction), as opposed to experienced or expressed positive emotion (Cohen & Pressman, Reference Cohen and Pressman2006; Pressman & Cohen, Reference Pressman and Cohen2005). Thus, the role of specific forms of childhood positive emotional reactivity as predictors of physical health needs to be identified using well-established measures. Evidence that early low- and/or high-intensity positivity is associated with lowered CVR could inform multifocal prevention intervention efforts aimed at lowering CVD.

Emotional reactivity and school problems

Negative emotional reactivity

Children’s emotional reactivity influences their reactions to the school environment, ability to adhere to classroom rules, and relationships with teachers and peers (Blair, Reference Blair2002; Keogh, Reference Keogh2003; Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson and Reiser2008). In turn, these things influence their school engagement. Increased anger reactivity is associated with lowered school engagement (Valiente et al., Reference Valiente, Swanson and Lemery-Chalfant2012), more school problems (mediated by social skills; Dollar et al., Reference Dollar, Perry, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2018), and frequent anger may undermine the student-teacher interactions (Raver, Reference Raver2002). High fear reactivity is associated with positive (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Chen, Li and Wang2009) and negative school adjustment (Weeks et al., Reference Weeks, Coplan and Kingsbury2009). Identification of which types of negative emotional reactivity most strongly predict school difficulties could significantly help to specify academic adjustment intervention efforts.

Positive emotional reactivity

Positive emotions impact school adjustment in various ways, including enhancing motivation, directing attention toward task performance, and facilitating information processing (Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, Reference Pekrun, Linnenbrink-Garcia, Christenson, Reschly and Wylie2012), as well as helping children develop stronger teacher relationships (Denham, Reference Denham1998). However, some studies fail to find this association (Diaz et al., Reference Diaz, Eisenberg, Valiente, VanSchyndel, Spinrad, Berger and Southworth2017; Dollar et al., Reference Dollar, Perry, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2018; Herndon et al., Reference Herndon, Bailey, Shewark, Denham and Bassett2013). Thus, consideration of specific forms of positive emotional reactivity may elucidate these pathways and inform academic intervention efforts.

Taken together, specific negative and positive emotional expressions are associated with subsequent adjustment across domains in unique ways. However, with the exception of a few studies that include children (i.e., Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Putnam and Rothbart2012) or adolescents (i.e., Creemers et al., Reference Creemers, Korhonen, Kaprio, Vollebergh, Ormel, Verhulst and Huizink2009, Reference Creemers, Dijkstra, Vollebergh, Ormel, Verhulst and Huizink2010), it is largely unknown how distinct aspects of emotional reactivity (i.e., low-intensity positivity and high-intensity positivity as opposed to “positive reactivity”) might uniquely predict functioning. This question can only be assessed by simultaneously accounting for other forms of emotional reactivity. For example, is high anger reactivity in early childhood uniquely associated with adolescent outcomes, even after accounting for its shared variance with positive and fear reactivity, reflecting the child’s general tendency to be emotionally reactive? The first aim of the current study was to elucidate answers to questions like this. Specifically, we tested whether specific types of positive and negative emotional reactivity in early childhood were associated with psychological, physical health, and school adjustment in adolescence.

EC, ER, and social skills as potential mechanisms

It is clear that numerous direct links exist between early emotional reactivity and adjustment. However, failure or success in key tasks in one developmental period significantly influences functioning in subsequent developmental periods (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). Thus, successful skill acquisition in middle childhood is essential to consider when predicting adolescent functioning from early childhood traits. The transition to middle childhood involves entering a more formal school environment, becoming active members of peer groups, and establishing and maintaining complex social relationships. It is also a period of increased autonomy and school demands that require the regulation of emotion and behavior. A core focus of the early childhood period is refinement of these necessary social and regulatory behaviors (Kopp, Reference Kopp1989). Thus, the extent to which children have mastered these skills by the beginning of middle childhood is likely to have lasting impacts on adjustment. Specifically, if children have not developed strong social and regulatory skills by middle childhood, they are at a higher risk of maladjustment in adolescence and beyond (e.g., Crockett et al., Reference Crockett, Raffaelli and Shen2006; Shortt et al., Reference Shortt, Capaldi, Dishion, Bank and Owen2003).

One important skill that is refined into middle childhood is EC. EC includes children’s ability to regulate their attention and behavior and is predicted by positive and negative emotional reactivity. Children who expressed less anger (Tan et al., Reference Tan, Armstrong and Cole2013) and high low-intensity positivity (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007) had greater EC skills, whereas children with high-intensity positivity displayed lower EC (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007). Given the known link between EC and adolescent adjustment (King et al., Reference King, Fleming, Monahan and Catalano2011, Reference King, Lengua and Monahan2013; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Nigg, Wong, Fitzgerald, Jester, Puttler and Zucker2007; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Chassin, Eisenberg and Spinrad2015), these findings could have important long-term implications. For instance, if children’s high-intensity positivity negatively impacts their ability to develop EC by middle childhood, then they will likely engage in more adolescent impulsive, risk-taking behaviors because their intense experience of positive affect may overpower their ability to manage their behavior and assess the situation’s risk.

ER skills, including processes that serve to maintain, inhibit, or enhance the intensity and valence of these emotional experiences to accomplish one’s goals (Calkins & Hill, Reference Calkins, Hill and Gross2007; Fox & Calkins, Reference Fox and Calkins2003) are developed and refined into middle childhood (Cole, Reference Cole2014). Children prone to experience intense emotions, including anger and fear reactivity, are less likely to develop adaptive ER skills (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, McKay, Iddins, Robertson and Rittmueller2009; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland, Spinrad, Fabes, Shepard, Reiser and Guthrie2001, Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum-Wilkens2010; Stifter & Spinrad, Reference Stifter and Spinrad2002), likely because it is harder to regulate emotions as they become more intense. However, it is unknown how proclivities toward experiencing low- and high-intensity positivity are associated with the development of ER skills. Hundreds of studies have confirmed that children’s poor ER is associated with externalizing and internalizing behavior problems (see Calkins & Perry, Reference Calkins, Perry and Cicchetti2016 for review; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum-Wilkens2010), poorer school functioning (Hinshaw, Reference Hinshaw1992; Miles & Stipek, Reference Miles and Stipek2006), increased substance use (Cougle et al., Reference Cougle, Zvolensky and Hawkins2013; Kober, Reference Kober and Gross2014), and poorer physical health (e.g., Kitayama et al., Reference Kitayama, Park, Boylan, Miyamoto, Levine, Markus and Ryff2015; Shriver et al., Reference Shriver, Dollar, Lawless, Wideman, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2019). It is necessary to understand how specific forms of negative and positive reactivity predict ER skills in order to identify children who need increased support in developing these necessary abilities.

Finally, the development of strong social skills (i.e., the ability to respond in an appropriate manner in social situations, sharing and cooperating; Gresham & Elliott, Reference Gresham and Elliott1990; Rose-Krasnor & Denham, Reference Rose-Krasnor, Denham, Rubin, Bukowski and Laursen2009) is important for adjustment given its association with lowered internalizing (Hymel et al., Reference Hymel, Wagner, Butler, Asher and Coie1990), externalizing (Blandon et al., Reference Blandon, Calkins, Grimm, Keane and O'Brien2010) and school problems (Pianta & Stuhlman, Reference Pianta and Stuhlman2004; Spilt et al., Reference Spilt, Hughes, Wu and Kwok2012). Moreover, high anger reactivity is predictive of children’s inability to engage in appropriate social skills (Rydell et al., Reference Rydell, Berlin and Bohlin2003; Rydell et al., Reference Rydell, Thorell and Bohlin2007), whereas children with low anger reactivity have an easier time acquiring socially skilled behaviors (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Bukowski, Parker, Eisenberg, Damon and Lerner2006). In addition, children high in fear reactivity are less likely to develop strong social skills and often display reticent behaviors (Coplan et al., Reference Coplan, Prakash, O’Neil and Armer2004), withdrawing from social interactions, and/or not initiating social interactions with peers (Coplan & Prakash, Reference Coplan and Prakash2003; Coplan et al., Reference Coplan, Prakash, O’Neil and Armer2004). Therefore, children’s social skills development by middle childhood likely serves as a central mechanism by which early emotional reactivity is linked with adolescent adjustment.

Less is understood about the link between positive emotional reactivity and social skills. Although few studies have considered low- and high-intensity positivity specifically, studies examining general positivity and exuberance report mixed findings. Greater childhood positive emotional reactivity is associated with declines in social skills (Sallquist et al., Reference Sallquist, Eisenberg, Spinrad, Reiser, Hofer, Zhou and Eggum2009). However, other work suggests that positive emotional reactivity may facilitate the initiation and regulation of social exchanges (Denham et al., Reference Denham, McKinley, Couchoud and Holt1990; Dougherty, Reference Dougherty2006), providing a greater opportunity to learn appropriate social behaviors. Similarly, some report that exuberant children exhibit poorer social functioning (Dollar & Stifter, Reference Dollar and Stifter2012), whereas others suggest this association is dependent on left frontal EEG asymmetry (Degnan et al., Reference Degnan, Hane, Henderson, Moas, Reeb-Sutherland and Fox2011) or inhibitory control (Dollar et al., Reference Dollar, Stifter and Buss2017).

Although some preliminary support establishes the links between early emotional reactivity, middle childhood skills, and adolescent outcomes, either through short-term longitudinal studies and/or by showing long-term direct links, more work is needed to elucidate the mechanisms that underlie variation in developmental pathways toward adolescent adjustment. Thus, the second aim was to examine whether EC, ER, and social skills by middle childhood serve as mechanisms through which emotional reactivity during early childhood was associated with psychological, academic, and physical health adjustment in adolescence.

A multimethod, multiinformant, longitudinal design was employed to understand how specific forms of emotional reactivity in early childhood are associated with subsequent risk or well-being. We hypothesized that high levels of anger, fear, and high-intensity positivity would be associated with maladaptive functioning in middle childhood and adolescence, whereas high levels of low-intensity positivity would be associated with positive adjustment. Second, we hypothesized that the developed skills by middle childhood would serve as mechanisms that explain, at least in part, why negative and positive emotional reactivity in early childhood were associated with adolescent adjustment. That is, we hypothesized that the extent to which these skills have developed by age 7 is essential in explaining the associations between early emotional reactivity and adolescent adjustment. We chose to examine these skills at age 7 because regulatory and social processes are fairly well-developed in most children by this age (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Altamirano, Corley, Young, Rhea and Hewitt2016; Raffaelli et al., Reference Raffaelli, Crockett and Shen2005) and children have transitioned to a more formal school environment. Thus, children who have not developed these necessary skills, especially those who are emotionally reactive, during this time that requires significant regulatory and social skills, are less likely to acquire them and are at a heightened risk of maladaptive outcomes.

Methods

Participants

This study utilized data from three cohorts of children who are part of an ongoing longitudinal study of social and emotional development. The goal for recruitment was to obtain a sample of children who were at risk for developing future externalizing behavior problems and who were representative of the surrounding community in terms of race and socioeconomic status (SES). All cohorts were recruited through child daycare centers, the County Health Department, and the local Women, Infants, and Children program. Potential participants for cohorts 1 and 2 were recruited at 2-years-old and screened using mother report of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1992) to oversample for externalizing behavior problems. Efforts were made to obtain approximately equal numbers of boys and girls. This recruitment effort resulted in a total of 307 families who agreed to participate. Cohort 3 was initially recruited when infants were 6-months-old for their level of frustration, based on laboratory observation and parent report, and were followed through the toddler period (see Calkins, Dedmon, Gill, Lomax, & Johnson, Reference Calkins, Dedmon, Gill, Lomax and Johnson2002 for more information). Children from cohort 3 whose mothers completed the CBCL at age 2 (N = 140) were then included in the larger study. Of the entire sample (N = 447), 37% of children were identified as being at risk for future externalizing problems at age 2. There were no significant demographic differences between cohorts regarding gender, χ2(2, N = 447) = .63, p = .73, race, χ2(2, N = 447) = 1.13, p = .57, or age 2 SES, F (2, 444) = .53, p = .59. Although this sample was initially oversampled for externalizing behaviors, by early childhood the sample as a whole was no longer high in clinically relevant levels of externalizing behavior problems.

Of the 447 originally selected participants, six were dropped because they did not participate in any data collection at age 2 after the screening process. Four participants were dropped from the original sample due to developmental delays. Three hundred and sixty-five families participated at age 5, 350 families participated at age 7, and 327 families participated at age 15. There were no significant differences between families who did and did not participate in terms of race, χ2 (1, N = 447) = .14–3.96, p = .71, 27 or age 2 externalizing T score, t (445) = .24–1.39, p = .81, .17 at ages 5, 7, and 15. Families with lower age 2 SES, t (432) = −2.61, p < .01, were less likely to participate in the age 7 assessment. These differences were nonsignificant at ages 5 and 15, t (432) = −.56 to −1.93, p = .58, .17. Boys were less likely to participate in the age 15 assessment χ2 (1, N = 447) = 9.31, p = .002. These gender differences were nonsignificant at ages 5 and 7, χ2 (1, N = 447) = .76–2.12, p = .38, .15. There were no differences between cohorts in any of the study variables (p’s > .10) except age 15 risk-taking behaviors, such that cohort 1 was more likely to engage in risk-taking than cohort 3, F (2, 300) = 2.92, p = .03. For information regarding this sample at the age 16 health visit please see Wideman et al. (Reference Wideman, Calkins, Janssen, Lovelady, Dollar, Keane and Shanahan2016) and Dollar et al. (Reference Dollar, Calkins, Shriver, Keane, Shanahan and Wideman2021).

The sample for the current study included 383 children (58% girls) who had available data at the age 5 or age 15/16 assessments and then Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) was used to handle missing data within the sample. The sample was 66% European American, 29% African American, 3% biracial, and 2% identified as other. Families were economically diverse based on Hollingshead (Reference Hollingshead1975) scores at the age 5 assessment, with a range from 14 to 66 (M = 39.86, SD = 11.08), thus representing families from each level of social strata.

Procedures

Children participated in a longitudinal study beginning at age 2. The current analyses include data collected when children were ages 5, 7, 15, and 16. Mothers reported on children’s low- and high-intensity positivity, anger, and fear when the children were age 5. When the children were age 7, mothers reported on children’s EC and ER, and teachers reported on children’s social skills. At age 15, adolescents self-reported their engagement in risky behaviors, school problems, and emotional symptoms. At age 16, adolescents completed a blood draw, anthropometrics (waist circumference [WC]), and blood pressure measurements.

Measures

Low- and high-intensity positivity, anger, and fear

When children were 5 years old, mothers completed the Child Behavior Questionnaire-short form (CBQ-SF; Putnam & Rothbart, Reference Putnam and Rothbart2006) to report on children’s low- and high-intensity positivity, anger, and fear. Mothers were asked to report their child’s reaction to a variety of situations and decide to what extent each item is true, using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = extremely untrue to 7 = extremely true). Of interest in the current study were the low-intensity pleasure (8 items; α = .73), high-intensity pleasure (6 items; α = .68), anger (6 items; α = .82), and fear (6 items; α = .68) subscales. Low-intensity pleasure measures the amount of pleasure involved in situations with low stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity (e.g., “Enjoys just being talked to”). High-intensity pleasure measures the amount of pleasure related to situations involving high stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity (e.g., “Enjoys activities such as being chased, spun around by the arms, etc.”). Anger measures the amount of negative affect related to interruption of ongoing tasks or goal blocking (e.g., “Has temper tantrums when s/he doesn’t get what s/he wants.”). Fear measures the amount of negative affect related to anticipated pain, distress, or situations that might be frightening (e.g., “Is afraid of loud noises.”).

The impulsivity (6 items; α = .61), approach (6 items; α = .60), and activity level (7 items; α = .73) subscales on the CBQ were used as covariates. Impulsivity measures the speed of response initiation (e.g., “Tends to say the first thing that comes to mind, without stopping to think about it”). Approach measures the amount of excitement and positive anticipation for expected pleasurable activities (e.g., “Gets so worked up before an exciting event that s/he has trouble sitting still”). Activity level measures the level of gross motor activity including rate and extent of locomotion (e.g., “Tends to run rather than walk from room to room”).

EC

Mothers reported on their 7-year-old children’s EC using the CBQ-SF (Putnam & Rothbart, Reference Putnam and Rothbart2006). The EC variable (α = .70) was an average of the attentional focusing, perceptual sensitivity, and inhibitory control subscales. Attentional focusing measures the tendency to maintain focus on a particular task (6 items; α = .70; e.g., “When building or putting something together, becomes very involved in what s/he is doing, and works for long periods”. Perceptual sensitivity measures the extent to which slight, low-intensity stimuli from the external environment can be detected (7 items; α = .68; e.g., “Seems to listen, even to quiet sounds”. Inhibitory control measures the capacity to plan and to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions or in novel or uncertain situations (6 items; α = .68; e.g., “Can easily stop an activity when s/he is told ‘no’”).

ER

Mothers completed the Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields & Cicchetti, Reference Shields and Cicchetti1997) when children were 7 years old. This measure assesses mothers’ perception of the child’s ER through 24 items rated on a 4-point Likert scale indicating how frequently the behaviors occur (1 = almost always to 4 = never). The regulation subscale (8 items; α = .67; e.g., “S/he displays appropriate negative affect in response to hostile, aggressive or intrusive play by peers”) assesses appropriate ER, emotive expression, and comprehension of one’s own emotions and empathy.

Social skills

Teachers completed the elementary version of the Social Skills Rating System (Gresham & Elliott, Reference Gresham and Elliott1990) in first grade (age 7), which assesses teachers’ perceptions of children’s behavioral social skills based on how often certain behaviors occur (0 = never to 2 = very often). The social skills scale (40 items; α = .73), which is a mean composite of the assertion (10 items; α = .79; e.g., “Appropriately questions rules that may be unfair”), cooperation (10 items; α = .91; e.g., “Follows your directions”), and self-control (10 items; α = .77; e.g., “Controls temper in conflict situations with peers”) subscales, was used.

Risk-taking

At age 15, adolescents reported their risky behavior on a measure adapted from multiple risk-taking self-report questionnaires (i.e., Conger & Elder, Reference Conger and Elder1994; Halpern-Felsher et al., Reference Halpern-Felsher, Cornell, Kropp and Tschann2005; 27 items). Adolescents reported on how frequently they have engaged in minor risky behaviors (α = .79; e.g., “drunk a bottle or glass of beer or other alcohol”) and major risky behaviors (α = .68; e.g., “purposely set a fire in a building or in any other place”; 0 = not at all to 2 = more than twice). A risk-taking score was created using a sum of the items (α = .84).

CVR

Metabolic syndrome (MetS), defined as systematic elevations of CVR including obesity, elevated lipids, altered glucose metabolism, and hypertension, was used as the CVR indicator. At age 16, blood was analyzed for metabolic and immune markers using a multiplex system. Commercially available assay kits were used to assess glucose (Cayman Chemical, Ann Arbor, MI, USA) and lipid profiles (Wako Chemical, Richmond, VA, USA). A continuous MetS score was created by summing the standardized measures of glucose, triglycerides, high-density lipoprotein inverse, WC, and mean arterial pressure (MAP). MAP was calculated as MAP = DBP + 1/3(SBP-DBP). Greater MetS values indicate greater CVR. Three percent of our sample met the criteria for clinical elevations of MetS and 19% had subclinical elevations of MetS (Eisenmann et al., Reference Eisenmann, Laurson, DuBose, Smith and Donnelly2010).

Emotional symptoms

At age 15, adolescents completed the Self-Report of Personality adolescent version (SRP-A) of the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus, Reference Reynolds and Kamphaus2004). This 176-item questionnaire assesses how the adolescent perceives their behavior, emotions, and personality characteristics. There are true/false questions and scale questions (0 = never to 3 = almost always). The emotional symptoms composite includes six subscales (61 items; α = .86), including social stress (10 items; α = .88; e.g., worry or distress related to social situations sense of inadequacy), anxiety (13 items; α = .88; e.g., child’s frequency and level of perfectionism, nervousness, and feelings of worry and fear), depression (12 items; α = .83; e.g., depressive symptoms such as crying easily, loneliness, feeling sad and pessimistic, and having the desire to harm or kill oneself), sense of inadequacy (10 items; α = .81; e.g., adolescent’s perception that they are unsuccessful in school, unable to achieve goals, and generally inadequate), self-esteem (10 items; α = .83; e.g., feelings of self-esteem, self-respect, and self-acceptance), and self-reliance (8 items; α = .71; e.g., adolescent’s confidence in his/her ability to solve problems and belief in his/her dependability and decisiveness). Normative t-scores that are representative of the US population were used.

School problems

At age 15, adolescents reported on their school problems using the SRP-A BASC-2 (Reynolds & Kamphaus, Reference Reynolds and Kamphaus2004). The school problems composite (25 items; α = .87) includes three subscales: attitude to school (7 items; α = .85; e.g., feelings of alienation, disaffection, and hostility toward school), attitude to teachers (9 items; α = .84; e.g., feelings of resentment and dislike of teachers as well as beliefs that teachers are unfair, uncaring or overly demanding), and sensation seeking (9 items; α = .76; e.g., tendency to take risks and seek excitement). Normative t-scores that are representative of the US population were used.

Sex

Child sex was coded as 1 = male and 2 = female.

Age 5 externalizing behaviors

The CBCLs (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1992) externalizing subscale, which includes items measuring aggressive, destructive, and oppositional behaviors, was used as an index of parent report of externalizing behavior problems at age 5 (33 items; α = .90; e.g., “Cruelty, bullying, or meanness to others”). The mother indicated how true the statement was of her child on a scale of 0 = not true to 2 = often true. Standardized t-scores of externalizing behaviors were used.

Results

Analytic plan

Mplus (Verson 8; Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017) was used to conduct a path analysis to examine the study aims. FIML was used to handle missing data. Model fit was assessed by examining the comparative fit index (CFI; Marsh & Hau, Reference Marsh and Hau2007), the standardized root mean square residual, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Cole & Maxwell, Reference Cole and Maxwell2003). A bias-corrected bootstrapping procedure (10,000 draws) was used to test the indirect effects. This approach has been shown to generate the most accurate confidence intervals for indirect effects, reducing Type 1 error rates, and increasing power (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood and Williams2004).

Model results

Descriptive statistics and correlations are presented in Table 1. Significant differences in age 5 low- and high-intensity positivity and age 7 EC and social skills were found by sex. Therefore, these variables were regressed onto sex. To account for the significant associations and conceptual overlap, age 5 approach, impulsivity, and activity level were regressed onto age 5 high-intensity positivity. Age 5 externalizing was regressed onto the age 5 emotional reactivity variables because this sample was originally oversampled for externalizing behaviors.

Table 1. Correlations and descriptive statistics

Note. AR = adolescent report; B = biological measure; CVR = cardiovascular risk; ER = emotion regulation; HIP = high-intensity positivity; LIP = low-intensity positivity; PR = parent report; TR = teacher report; Sex (1 = male, 2 = female). ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

The hypothesized model was a good fit to the data, χ 2 (46, N = 383) = 103.20, p = .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .05 [CI = .04, .07] (see Figure 1). The first aim was to assess whether childhood expressions of positive and negative emotional reactivity at age 5 were associated with psychological and physical health and school adjustment at ages 15/16. High-intensity positivity was positively associated with adolescent-reported risk-taking (β = .16, p < .01). Early childhood low-intensity positivity was negatively associated with adolescent CVR (β = −.28, p < .05).

Figure 1. Standardized estimates and model fit. Model Fit: χ2(46, N = 383) = 103.20, p = .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .05 [CI = .04, .07]. All model paths shown; Significant paths bolded and include coefficients; A = adolescent report; B = biological measure; P = parent report; T = teacher report.

The second aim of the study was to examine whether EC, ER, and social skills at age 7 serve as mechanisms through which emotional reactivity at age 5 was associated with adjustment at ages 15/16. High-intensity positivity was negatively associated with EC (β = −.11, p < .05). Low-intensity positivity was associated positively with EC (β = .40, p < .001) and ER (β = .21, p < .001). Anger reactivity was negatively associated with EC (β = −.19, p < .001), ER (β = −.22, p < .001), and social skills (β = −.28, p < .001). Fear reactivity was positively associated with social skills (β = .14, p < .05). EC was negatively associated with adolescent-reported engagement in risk-taking (β = −.19, p < .05), emotional symptoms (β = −.16, p < .05), and school problems (β = −.29, p < .001). Social skills was negatively associated with adolescent-reported engagement in risk-taking (β = −.27, p < .001), emotional symptoms (β = −.16, p < .05), and school problems (β = −.13, p < .05). ER was negatively associated with adolescent-reported emotional symptoms (β = −.13, p < .05)

All significant indirect effects are provided in Table 2. Several mediational pathways were significant, indicating that EC and social skills play unique roles in the association between specific forms of childhood emotional reactivity and adolescent outcomes. Specifically, high anger reactivity was significantly associated with increased adolescent risk-taking and emotional symptoms via lower social skills. In addition, greater anger reactivity was significantly associated with increased risk-taking, adolescent emotional symptoms, and school problems via lower EC. High levels of childhood low-intensity positivity was associated with lower risk-taking and fewer adolescent emotional symptoms and school problems via stronger EC at age 7. High childhood high-intensity positivity was associated with more adolescent school problems through lower EC skills. Greater childhood fear reactivity was associated with lower adolescent risk-taking through stronger childhood social skills. The indirect effects from age 5 emotional reactivity to emotional symptoms through ER were not statistically significant.

Table 2. Standardized estimates of significant indirect effects, standard errors, and 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals

Note. ANG = anger; AR = adolescent report; B = biological measure; EC = effortful control; HIP = high-intensity positivity; LIP = low-intensity positivity; PR = parent report; SS = social skills; TR = teacher report. Only significant indirect effects reported.

Discussion

This study contributes to the current literature in three important ways. First, we considered multiple aspects of emotional reactivity in order to identify unique effects beyond the expression of other forms of emotional reactivity and to elucidate the heterogeneity within positive emotional reactivity. Second, we considered multiple aspects of adolescent adjustment simultaneously, which allows for a better understanding of which domains are most strongly associated with specific forms of emotional reactivity. Finally, this study is one of the first to investigate potential mechanisms that inform the role of cascading effects from early specific emotional proclivities to adolescent functioning more than a decade later.

Direct and indirect associations emerged between early childhood low- and high-intensity positivity and various forms of adolescent adjustment. In terms of direct effects, we found that children who displayed greater high-intensity positivity were more likely to engage in adolescent risky behavior, whereas those characterized by high low-intensity positivity were more likely to be physically healthy, with lower CVR. These findings support the heterogeneous nature of positive reactivity and align with the hypothesis that high-intensity positivity may be associated with some forms of maladjustment, while low-intensity positivity is more likely to be associated with well-being (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007).

One reason for these discrepant associations may be the underlying motivations and behaviors that differing levels of intensity elicit (Polak-Toste & Gunnar, Reference Polak-Toste, Gunnar, Marshall and Fox2006; Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012). High-intensity positivity activates approach motivation and may be specific to intense positive emotions such as joy and excitement. Moreover, childhood high-intensity positivity is likely to evolve into later extraversion, impulsivity, sensation-seeking, and, overall, lowered self-regulation (Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012; Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Eisenberg, Damon and Richard2006). Together, these motivations and tendencies could contribute to adolescents’ increased likelihood of engaging in risky behavior. Importantly, our study expands previous work (e.g., Creemers et al., Reference Creemers, Dijkstra, Vollebergh, Ormel, Verhulst and Huizink2010; Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007) by being the first study to document these associations across a decade from childhood into adolescence.

Of note, we accounted for the shared variance between age 5 high-intensity positivity and age 5 impulsivity, approach, and activity level to provide a methodologically cleaner measure of high-intensity positivity rather than what is typically associated with the broader construct of surgency. By removing the variance in age 5 high-intensity positivity that is associated with impulsivity, approach, and activity level, our findings highlight that high-intensity positivity can put children at heightened risk of maladjustment and lends confidence to the importance of this construct in predicting later adjustment. This finding has significant implications for prevention work by suggesting that targeting high-intensity positivity early may offset risk prior to the development of later occurring behaviors that may be more difficult to alter (i.e., high sensation seeking, high impulsivity). Although these findings are an important first step, additional work is needed. For instance, there is a need for studies to break down the exuberance profile (i.e., high-intensity positivity, approach, activity level) in order to identify which components are the strongest predictors of later outcomes (Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016).

Although little work has focused on the developmental role of childhood low-intensity positivity, including affiliation, contentment, and happiness in calm situations, a significant literature shows that adults who experience more positive emotion generally are healthier (e.g., Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Alper, Doyle and Treanor2006) and live longer (e.g., Cohen & Pressman, Reference Cohen and Pressman2006). Youth obesity has reached an all-time high, with more than one-third of U.S. adolescents meeting criteria for obesity (Ogden et al., Reference Ogden, Carroll, Kit and Flegal2014), and CVR is significantly increasing among adolescents (Gaydosh et al., Reference Gaydosh, Schorpp, Chen, Miller and Harris2018). This study provides the first known developmental work to extend the link between positive emotion and physical health to childhood and adolescence, which has significant implications for preventive intervention work. Although the mechanisms by which this association exists (i.e., biological, behavioral) are unknown, an important target for obesity/CVR intervention efforts may be to promote children’s feelings of affiliation, contentment, and happiness in calm situations. Importantly, existing adult studies are difficult to synthesize because measures of positivity vary greatly by study and often examine general constructs (e.g., life satisfaction), as opposed to experienced or expressed positivity (Cohen & Pressman, Reference Cohen and Pressman2006; Pressman & Cohen, Reference Pressman and Cohen2005). This study uses a well-established measure of an individual’s propensity to experience and express low- and high-intensity positivity, and highlights that low-intensity positivity, not high-intensity positivity, is associated with better physical health.

Indirect effects also emerged to suggest that skills developed by middle childhood explain at least, in part, the association between low- and high-intensity positivity and adolescent adjustment. As expected, children with greater high-intensity positivity had lower EC abilities at age 7, whereas children characterized by low-intensity positivity had greater EC skills at age 7. Moreover, children’s EC skills at age 7 served as a significant mechanism by which low- and high-intensity positivity at age 5 were linked to school problems at age 15, although different pathways emerged by the type of positivity. Children showing greater high-intensity positivity were more likely to have adolescent school problems through lowered EC skills in middle childhood, whereas children high in low-intensity positivity reported having fewer adolescent school problems explained through stronger EC skills in middle childhood. These results provide important insight into mechanisms that explain divergent pathways for children who express different intensities of positive reactivity in childhood and builds upon the work of Kochanska et al. (Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007), who first highlighted that low- and high-intensity positivity were differentially associated with the development of EC.

It is interesting that the regulatory and social skills in middle childhood did not explain more associations between low- and high-intensity positivity and adolescent adjustment. Although these are key abilities that lower developmental risk, the results suggest that there are additional processes to explain trajectories for children high in low- and high-intensity positivity that should be examined in future work. For instance, it is likely that other social processes, such as affiliation with deviant peers, should be considered to understand developmental pathways for children prone to experience high levels of high-intensity positivity. In addition, physiological processes (i.e., heart rate, inflammatory markers) may explain, at least in part, trajectories for children high in low-intensity positivity.

EC was also revealed as an important mechanism by which children high in low-intensity positivity were less likely to engage in risk-taking and develop emotional symptoms in adolescence. Specifically, children who find pleasure in low-intensity situations were less likely to report engaging in risk-taking behaviors and reported experiencing lower emotional symptoms through their ability to regulate their attention and behavior in middle childhood. Emotional symptoms in this study was a measure of adolescent social stress, anxiety, depression, sense of inadequacy, self-esteem, and self-reliance, and thus very similar to internalizing symptomatology. A growing literature has shown that childhood positive emotional reactivity is protective against developing later depression (Dougherty et al., Reference Dougherty, Klein, Durbin, Hayden and Olino2010; Durbin et al., Reference Durbin, Klein, Hayden, Buckley and Moerk2005; Shankman et al., Reference Shankman, Tenke, Bruder, Durbin, Hayden and Klein2005). These findings extend this literature by generalizing to a broader array of symptomatology, as well as highlighting EC as a mechanism by which this association exists.

It is logical that children high in low-intensity positivity would engage in fewer risky behaviors and that this association would be explained through EC. There is growing evidence that children who find pleasure in low-intensity situations often develop strong attentional and behavioral regulation skills (e.g., Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Aksan, Penney and Doobay2007), possibly because they are inherently calmer and thus acquire strong regulatory skills with greater ease. In turn, these regulatory skills prevent adolescents from engaging in risky behaviors (e.g., King et al., Reference King, Fleming, Monahan and Catalano2011, Reference King, Lengua and Monahan2013). Importantly, these associations were found across a 10-year period, highlighting an important focus for preventive interventions by providing early target behaviors and entry points to offset maladaptive behavior early in development. Because high-intensity positivity is likely harder to regulate than low-intensity positivity, identifying youth who are prone to experience high-intensity positivity in early childhood and engaging with them in ways that bolster their regulatory skills when experiencing intense positive arousal may have profound implications for altering developmental trajectories toward maladjustment.

There were also a number of important pathways that emerged for children prone to experience anger and fear reactivity. Interestingly, there were no significant direct effects. These null effects, in conjunction with the number of significant indirect effects that emerged, point to the significance of middle childhood social and regulatory skills for children high in fear, and especially anger, reactivity. This builds on the premise that although children high in negative reactivity have greater difficulties developing social and regulatory skills, those who are able to acquire these skills are less likely to engage in or develop maladaptive behaviors by adolescence.

Specifically, consistent with existing research (for reviews see Calkins & Perry, Reference Calkins, Perry and Cicchetti2016; Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Bukowski, Parker, Eisenberg, Damon and Lerner2006) we found that childhood anger reactivity predicted lowered social skills, ER, and EC in middle childhood. Moreover, many indirect paths from childhood anger reactivity to adolescent outcomes via poorer social and behavioral skills emerged, suggesting that the tendency to experience intense/frequent anger may impede their development and lead to less optimal adolescent outcomes. For example, because anger-prone children are more likely to miss out on building social skills (Trentacosta & Shaw, Reference Trentacosta and Shaw2009), they may be rejected by their peers, and, in response, spend time with deviant peers. In turn, affiliation with these peers likely enhances children’s previous anger-prone tendencies and poor social skills (e.g., Cairns & Cairns, Reference Cairns and Cairns1994). In addition, children who experience intense anger have a more challenging task of developing strong ER and EC skills, albeit also more important, than children who have low to moderate experiences of anger. Cumulatively, this tendency to experience anger and difficulty developing important skills appears to contribute to a wide range of maladjustment, including risk-taking, school problems, and emotional symptoms, in adolescence.

For children high in anger reactivity, there were indirect paths toward all adolescent outcomes, other than CVR, via middle childhood low EC and social skills. For instance, EC and social skills served as mechanisms to explain why children who express intense anger are more likely to report high levels of emotional symptoms 10 years later. Thus, these findings suggest that anger-prone children who have difficulties controlling their behavior and developing social skills experience lowered self-esteem/self-reliance and greater anxiety and depressive symptoms in adolescence. Moreover, lowered social skills emerged as the most significant mechanism to explain why anger-prone children engage in more adolescent risk-taking. Many hypotheses have been posed to explain this link, including using substances to deal with intense emotions, being rejected by one’s peers, and not developing important self-regulation skills to resist the impulse to engage in risk-taking. These results provide evidence for the important role of social skills above and beyond regulatory abilities.

Similar to children high in high-intensity positivity, anger-prone children were also more likely to report more adolescent school problems, which was at least partially explained through their lowered EC skills. Given that the school problems measure included sensation seeking as one of the subscales, these findings provide additional evidence that children who experience intense approach-based emotions, such as high-intensity positivity and anger, in early childhood need to develop strong behavioral regulation skills to appropriately cope with the impulse to take risks and seek excitement (Putnam, Reference Putnam, Zentner and Shiner2012; Polak-Toste & Gunnar, Reference Polak-Toste, Gunnar, Marshall and Fox2006). Although our measure of school problems is widely employed (Reynolds & Kamphaus, Reference Reynolds and Kamphaus2004), the aspects of school functioning assessed can differ across studies. In particular, the inclusion of sensation seeking in our school problems construct may differ from school-based measures in other studies, making it important for future work to confirm these findings when employing other measures.

Children who were high in fear reactivity were rated to have higher social skills than children low in fear reactivity. Moreover, there was a significant indirect effect by which children high in fear reactivity were less likely to engage in risk-taking behaviors through the development of strong social skills by age 7. These are significant for multiple reasons. First, a sizeable literature has shown that children high in fear reactivity/behavioral inhibition are more prone to have social difficulties, including lowered social competence and heightened social reticence (e.g., Coplan et al., Reference Coplan, Prakash, O’Neil and Armer2004; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Bukowski, Parker, Eisenberg, Damon and Lerner2006). Our results, however, indicate that fear-prone children, as perceived by their mother, were rated by their teachers as showing strong social skills at age 7. This suggests that fearful children do not have social difficulties due to a lack of social skills; instead, other processes are at play, likely including how to modulate their social anxiety and feelings of being overwhelmed in social situations.

These findings are also notable when considering the well-established link between early fear reactivity/behavioral inhibition and later social anxiety (Chronis-Tuscano et al., Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Degnan, Pine, Perez-Edgar, Henderson, Diaz and Fox2009; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Snidman and Kagan1999). Children prone to heightened fear may be inclined to engage in substance use, especially in peer contexts, to cope with their social anxiety. Thus, although not tested directly by this study, these findings suggest that the development of social skills may be especially critical for fear-prone children in lowering the likelihood that they engage in substance use in order to dampen the effects of social anxiety. This should be considered by future work.

Finally, although there were significant direct effects between anger/low-intensity positivity and ER, and ER and adolescent emotional symptoms, there were no significant indirect effects where ER served as the mediator. This is surprising given the literature highlighting ER as a necessary developmental skill for children prone to experience intense emotional displays. However, most studies do not also consider other forms of self-regulation, as was done in the current study. Our results repeatedly revealed EC as a significant mechanism to explain the processes by which children high in anger or low in low-intensity positivity reported adolescent maladjustment (i.e., emotional symptoms). Thus, by middle childhood, EC skills may supersede the importance of ER as a significant mechanism to assuage risk. Across development distinct forms of self-regulation become intertwined and continuously build upon one another (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Perry, Dollar, Balter and Tamis-LeMonda2016); however, EC is a measure of children’s behavioral and attentional regulation, whereas ER encompasses children’s ability to regulate their emotional arousal. Thus, by age 7, the ability to regulate one’s attention and behavior may be more essential for those who have heightened anger or low levels of low-intensity positivity. It is also important to consider that by middle childhood children increasingly internalize their emotional expressions and regulatory strategies making it more difficult for parents to accurately report. Thus, additional research on these links is warranted given that the lack of findings could be attributable to our methodology.

Limitations and future directions

Despite the many strengths of this study, it is not without limitations. First, only maternal reports of emotional reactivity, ER, and EC were considered. A strength of maternal reports is that mothers observe children’s emotions/behavior across a range of contexts. However, maternal reports can be biased by mothers’ personality or mental health (Kagan & Fox, Reference Kagan, Fox, Damon, Lerner and Eisenberg2006). Future studies should employ observed reactivity and regulation measures to confirm this study’s findings. Moreover, although there were two years between measurements, mothers who view their children as more emotionally reactive at age 5 could also be viewing them as lower in EC at age 7. Importantly, however, social skills was assessed via teacher-report and was also predicted by children’s emotional reactivity. Another point to consider is that although these cohorts were combined at age 2, recruitment procedures varied slightly and there are a few significant demographic differences among them in early ages.

In addition, we did not include measures of parental factors that likely influence developmental pathways. Recent research has shown that child effects on caregiving behaviors may be a mechanism by which children’s later regulatory skills are, in part, undermined by children’s anger reactivity (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Ganiban, Neiderhiser, Natsuaki, Shaw, Reiss and Leve2018). Thus, additional work is needed to build upon the current study’s findings to include parental genetic and socialization factors. Moreover, parental monitoring becomes a significant predictor of risk (e.g., Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Lessard, Colder, Livingston, Casey and Leonard2016) and may impact these pathways. Future work considering these associations is needed. Additionally, we employed a measure of MetS that encompassed multiple aspects of CVR. However, given that some aspects of MetS may develop earlier than others, additional work should examine discrete CVR risks to identify if there are specific pathways that could inform preventive interventions.

Finally, we did not include genetic measures that may explain the associations between emotional reactivity and adolescent adjustment. Children’s early emotional reactivity has biological underpinnings (Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Eisenberg, Damon and Richard2006), which could be similar to those underlying CVR, risky behaviors, or psychopathology (e.g., Kreek et al., Reference Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman and LaForge2005; Turcot et al., Reference Turcot, Lu, Highland, Schurmann, Justice, Fine and Loos2018). Work is needed to consider the role of genetic factors in these associations.

Conclusion

This study makes significant theoretical, empirical, and translational contributions. First, current theoretical models of biobehavioral risk rarely acknowledge that development is the result of a cascade-based acquisition of important skills that influence subsequent adaptation to the challenges of the next developmental period. Here we provided evidence for how early experiences of different emotions and early regulatory and social skill acquisition are important to consider in lowering biobehavioral risk in adolescence. From an empirical standpoint, this study makes an important contribution to the developmental literature by examining multiple forms of emotional reactivity concurrently to predict a wide range of psychological, physical health, social, and school adjustment outcomes across an 11-year time frame. Moreover, this is the first known study to highlight that specific forms of early childhood positive emotional reactivity are differentially associated with risk or well-being in adolescence, thus highlighting the necessity for future research to consider specific forms of emotional reactivity as predictors of developmental trajectories. Finally, this study makes a significant translational contribution given that it can inform multifocal health behavior interventions that target cooccurring risks and provide an early entry point and important target mechanisms for preventive interventions.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the families who generously gave their time to participate in the study.

Funding statement

This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (SDC and SPK, MH 58144) and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (LW, LS, SDC, and SPK, HD 078346).

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the child behavior checklist/2-3 and 1992 profile. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT.Google Scholar
Anzman-Frasca, S., Stifter, C. A., & Birch, L. L. (2012). Temperament and childhood obesity risk: A review of the literature. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(9), 732745. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e31826a119f CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, J. E., Bayles, K., Bennett, D. S., Ridge, B., & Brown, M. M. (1991). Origins of externalizing behavior problems at eight years of age. In Pepler, D. J., & Rubin, K. H. (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Hillsdale, NJ: 93120.Google Scholar
Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Hérot, C., Friedman, D., Snidman, N.Faraone, S. V. (2001). Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(10), 16731679. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1673 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57(2), 111127. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Grimm, K. J., Keane, S. P., & O'Brien, M. (2010). Testing a developmental cascade model of emotional and social competence and early peer acceptance. Development and Psychopathology, 22(4), 737748. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000428 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohlin, G., & Hagekull, B. (2009). Socio-emotional development: From infancy to young adulthood. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(6), 592601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00787.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Ganiban, J. M., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., Reiss, D.Leve, L. D. (2018). Contributions of mothers' and fathers' parenting to children’s self-regulation: Evidence from an adoption study. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12692. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12692 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., McKay, T., Iddins, E., Robertson, C.Rittmueller, A. (2009). Maternal and contextual influences and the effect of temperament development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 103116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, K. A., Kiel, E. J., Morales, S., & Robinson, E. (2014). Toddler inhibitory control, bold response to novelty, and positive affect predict externalizing symptoms in kindergarten: Inhibitory control, positive affect, and externalizing. Social Development, 23(2), 232249. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12058 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of youth in our time. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, https://doi.org/10.1086/230945 Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Dedmon, S. E., Gill, K. L., Lomax, L. W., & Johnson, L. M. (2002). Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy, 3(2), 175197. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000415 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation: Biological and environmental transactions in early development. In Gross, J. J. (Eds.), Handbook of emotion regulation (1sted.). Guilford Press New York, NY: 229248.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Perry, N. B. (2016). The development of emotion regulation: Implications for child adjustment, Developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Maladaptation and psychopathology. vol. 3, Wiley Hoboken, NJ: 187242. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy306 Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Perry, N. B., & Dollar, J. M. (2016). A biopsychosocial model of the development of self-regulation in infancy. In Balter, L., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (3rded.). Routledge New York, NY: 320. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315764931 Google Scholar
Chen, X., Chen, H., Li, D., & Wang, L. (2009). Early childhood behavioral inhibition and social and school adjustment in Chinese children: A 5-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 80(6), 16921704. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01362.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y.Fox, N. A. (2009). Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(9), 928935. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09df CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., & Pressman, S. D. (2006). Positive affect and health. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 122125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00420.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S., Alper, C. M., Doyle, W. J., & Treanor, J. J. (2006). Positive emotional style predicts resistance to illness after experimental exposure to rhinovirus or influenza A virus. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(6), 809815. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000245867.92364.3c CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 558577. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M. (2014). Moving ahead in the study of the development of emotion regulation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 203207. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414522170 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (1994). Families in troubled times: Adapting to change in rural America. Aldine de Gruyter, Hawthorne, NY, https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003058809 Google Scholar
Coplan, R. J., & Prakash, K. (2003). Spending time with teachers: Characteristics of preschoolers who frequently elicit versus initiate interactions with teachers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18(1), 143158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2006(03)00009-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coplan, R. J., Prakash, K., O’Neil, K., & Armer, M. (2004). Do you ‘want’ to play? Distinguishing between conflicted-shyness and social disinterest in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 244258. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.244 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cougle, J. R., Zvolensky, M. J., & Hawkins, K. A. (2013). Delineating a relationship between problematic anger and cigarette smoking: A population-based study. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 15(1), 297301. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/nts122 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creemers, H. E., Dijkstra, J. K., Vollebergh, W. A., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Huizink, A. C. (2010). Predicting lifetime and regular cannabis use during adolescence; the roles of temperament and peer substance use: The TRAILS study. Addiction, 105(4), 699708. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02819.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creemers, H. E., Korhonen, T., Kaprio, J., Vollebergh, W. A., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C.Huizink, A. C. (2009). The role of temperament in the relationship between early onset of tobacco and cannabis use: The TRAILS study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 104(1-2), 113118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.04.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, L. J., Raffaelli, M., & Shen, Y. L. (2006). Linking self-regulation and risk proneness to risky sexual behavior: Pathways through peer pressure and early substance use. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16(4), 503525. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00505.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 47(3), 765780. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021316 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. Guilford Press, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of peer status in young preschoolers. Child Development, 61(4), 11451152. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130882 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhabhar, F. S. (2014). Effects of stress on immune function: The good, the bad, and the beautiful. Immunologic Research, 58(2-3), 193210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12026-014-8517-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diaz, A., Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., VanSchyndel, S., Spinrad, T. L., Berger, R.Southworth, J. (2017). Relations of positive and negative expressivity and effortful control to kindergarteners’ student-teacher relationship, academic engagement, and externalizing problems at school. Journal of Research in Personality, 67(1), 314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.11.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., & Buss, K. A. (2014). Approach and positive affect in toddlerhood predict early childhood behavior problems. Social Development, 23(2), 267287. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., Calkins, S. D., Shriver, L., Keane, S. P., Shanahan, L., & Wideman, L. (2021). Emerging self-regulatory skills in childhood predict cardiometabolic risk in adolescence. Comprehensive Psychoneuroendocrinology, 7, 100070. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpnec.2021.100070 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. (2012). Temperamental surgency and emotion regulation as predictors of childhood social competence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(2), 178194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2018). Temperamental anger and positive reactivity and the development of social skills: Implications for academic competence during preadolescence. Early Education and Development, 29(5), 747761. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1409606 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., Stifter, C. A., & Buss, K. A. (2017). Exuberant and inhibited children: Person-centered profiles and links to social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 53(7), 12221229. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000323 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, L. R. (2006). Children’s emotionality and social status: A meta-analytic review. Social Development, 15(3), 394417. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00348.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dougherty, L. R., Klein, D. N., Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., & Olino, T. M. (2010). Temperamental positive and negative emotionality and children’s depressive symptoms: A longitudinal prospective study from age three to age ten. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(4), 462488. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2010.29.4.462 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durbin, C. E., Klein, D. N., Hayden, E. P., Buckley, M. E., & Moerk, K. C. (2005). Temperamental emotionality in preschoolers and parental mood disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 2837. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.28 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiden, R. D., Lessard, J., Colder, C. R., Livingston, J., Casey, M., & Leonard, K. E. (2016). Developmental cascade model for adolescent substance use from infancy to late adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 52(10), 16191633. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000199 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M.Guthrie, I. K. (2001). The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development, 72(4), 11121134. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T., & Eggum-Wilkens, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 495525. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenmann, J. C., Laurson, K. R., DuBose, K. D., Smith, B. K., & Donnelly, J. E. (2010). Construct validity of a continuous metabolic syndrome score in children. Diabetology & Metabolic Syndrome, 2(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1758-5996-2-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Motivation and Emotion, 27(1), 726. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023622324898 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 5468. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Altamirano, L. J., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Rhea, S. A.Hewitt, J. K. (2016). Stability and change in executive function abilities from late adolescence to early adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 326340. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gambetti, E., & Giusberti, F. (2009). Dispositional anger and risk decision-making. Mind & Society, 8(1), 720. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-008-0052-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2012). Etiology of preschool behavior problems: Contributions of temperament attributes in early childhood. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(2), 197211. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaydosh, L., Schorpp, K. M., Chen, E., Miller, G. E., & Harris, K. M. (2018). College completion predicts lower depression but higher metabolic syndrome among disadvantaged minorities in young adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(1), 109114. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1714616114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN.Google Scholar
Haller, M., Handley, E., Chassin, L., & Bountress, K. (2010). Developmental cascades: Linking adolescent substance use, affiliation with substance use promoting peers, and academic achievement to adult substance use disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 22(4), 899915. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000532 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Cornell, J. L., Kropp, R. Y., & Tschann, J. M. (2005). Oral verses vaginal sex among adolescents: Perceptions, attitudes, and behavior. Pediatrics, 115(4), 845851. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2004-2108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harburg, E., Julius, M., Kaciroti, N., Gleiberman, L., & Schork, M. A. (2003). Expressive/suppressive anger-coping responses, gender, and types of mortality: A 17-year follow-up (Tecumseh, Michigan, 1971-1988). Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 588597. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.PSY.0000075974.19706.3B CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartiala, O., Magnussen, C. G., Kajander, S., Knuuti, J., Ukkonen, H., Saraste, A.Raitakari, O. T. (2012). Adolescence risk factors are predictive of coronary artery calcification at middle age: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 60(15), 13641370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jacc.2012.05.045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauber, R. P., Rice, M. H., Howell, C. C., & Carmon, M. (1998). Anger and blood pressure readings in children. Applied Nursing Research, 11(1), 211. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0897-1897(98)80021-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herndon, K. J., Bailey, C. S., Shewark, E. A., Denham, S. A., & Bassett, H. H. (2013). Preschoolers' emotion expression and regulation: Relations with school adjustment. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174(6), 642663. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2012.759525 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 127155. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four-factor index of social status, (Unpublished manuscript), Department of Sociology, Yale University, New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
Hymel, S., Wagner, E., & Butler, L. J. (1990). Reputational bias: View from the peer group. In Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. D. (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood. Cambridge University Press New York, NY: 156186.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (2018). Kinds of individuals defined by patterns of variables. Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 11971209. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941800055X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Fox, N. (2006). Biology, culture, and temperamental biases. In Damon, W., Lerner, R., & Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. vol. 3, Wiley New York, NY: 167–122, https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0304,Google Scholar
Keogh, B. K. (2003). Temperament in the classroom: Understanding individual differences. Paul H Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Kerr, M., Tremblay, R. E., Pagani, L., & Vitaro, F. (1997). Boys' behavioral inhibition and the risk of later delinquency. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54(9), 809816. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830210049005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, K. M., Fleming, C. B., Monahan, K. C., & Catalano, R. F. (2011). Changes in self-control problems and attention problems during middle school predict alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana use during high school. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25(1), 6979. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021958 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, K. M., Lengua, L. J., & Monahan, K. C. (2013). Individual differences in the development of self-regulation during pre-adolescence: Connections to context and adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(1), 5769. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9665-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitayama, S., Park, J., Boylan, J. M., Miyamoto, Y., Levine, C. S., Markus, H. R.Ryff, C. D. (2015). Expression of anger and ill health in two cultures: An examination of inflammation and cardiovascular risk. Psychological Science, 26(2), 211220. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614561268 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kober, H. (2014). Emotion regulation in substance use disorders. In Gross, J. J. (Eds.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nded.). Guilford Press New York, NY: 428446.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Penney, S. J., & Doobay, A. F. (2007). Early positive emotionality as a heterogeneous trait: Implications for children’s self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 10541066. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 343354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.343 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreek, M. J., Nielsen, D. A., Butelman, E. R., & LaForge, K. S. (2005). Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Neurobiology of Addiction, 8(11), 14501457. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1583 Google ScholarPubMed
Lahat, A., Degnan, K. A., White, L. K., McDermott, J. M., Henderson, H. A., Lejuez, C. W.Fox, N. A. (2012). Temperamental exuberance and executive function predict propensity for risk taking in childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 24(3), 847856. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000405 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(1), 99128. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. (2007). Applications of latent-variable models in educational psychology: The need for methodological-substantive synergies. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32(1), 151170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martel, M. M., Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Fitzgerald, H. E., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I.Zucker, R. A. (2007). Childhood and adolescent resiliency, regulation, and executive functioning in relation to adolescent problems and competence in a high-risk sample. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2), 541563. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407070265 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClowry, S. G., Giangrande, S. K., Tommasini, N. R., Clinton, W., Foreman, N. S., Lynch, K.Ferketich, S. L. (1994). The effects of child temperament, maternal characteristics, and family circumstances on the maladjustment of school-age children. Research in Nursing & Health, 17(1), 2535. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.4770170105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCreary, D. R., & Sadava, S. W. (2000). Stress, alcohol use and alcohol-related problems: The influence of negative and positive affect in two cohorts of young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61(3), 466474. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2000.61.466 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. The New England Journal of Medicine, 338(3), 171179. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJM199801153380307 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, S. B., & Stipek, D. (2006). Contemporaneous and longitudinal associations between social behavior and literacy achievement in a sample of low-income elementary school children. Child Development, 77(1), 103117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00859.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, S. M., Slavich, G. M., & Georgiades, K. (2014). The social environment and depression: The roles of life stress. In Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of depression. Guilford Press New York, NY: 296314.Google Scholar
Morales, S., Tang, A., Bowers, M. E., Miller, N. V., Buzzell, G. A., Smith, E.Fox, N. A. (2021). Infant temperament prospectively predicts general psychopathology in childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001996 Google ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. Mplus: Statistical modeling software (Version 8.0) [Computer software]. 2017. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén., http://www.statmodel.com.Google Scholar
Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Kit, B. K., & Flegal, K. M. (2014). Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011-2012. Journal of the American Medical Association, 311(8), 806814. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2014.732 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardini, D., Lochman, J., & Wells, K. (2004). Negative emotions and alcohol use initiation high-risk boys: The moderating effect of good inhibitory control. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(5), 505518. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000037780.22849.23 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Erath, S. A., Wojslawowicz, J. C., & Buskirk, A. A. (2006). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathology perspective. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Theory and method. vol. 1, 2nd. Wiley Hoboken, NJ: 419493, https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939383.ch12,Google Scholar
Pekrun, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2012). Academic emotions and student engagement. In Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., & Wylie, C. (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engagement. Springer Boston, MA: 259282. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_12 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., Dollar, J. M., Keane, S. P., O’Brien, M., & Shanahan, L. (2017). Self-regulation as a predictor of patterns of change in externalizing behaviors from infancy to adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 497510. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000992 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pianta, R. C., & Stuhlman, M. W. (2004). Teacher-child relationships and children’s success in the first years of school. School Psychology Review, 33(3), 444458. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2004.12086261 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polak-Toste, C. P., & Gunnar, M. R. (2006). Temperamental exuberance: Correlates and consequences. In Marshall, P. J., & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), The development of social engagement: Neurobiological perspectives. Oxford University Press New York, NY: 1945. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195168716.003.0002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 925971. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, S. P. (2012). Positive emotionality. In Zentner, M., & Shiner, R. L. (Eds.), Handbook of temperament. Guilford Press New York, NY: 105123.Google Scholar
Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76(1), 212226. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00840.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 5475. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raver, C. C. (2002). Emotions matter: Making the case for the role of young children’s emotional development for early school readiness. Social Policy Report, 16(3), 36. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2379-3988.2002.tb00041.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior assessment system for children (2nd edn. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN.Google Scholar
Romer, D., & Hennessy, M. (2007). A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: The role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention Science, 8(2), 89101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-007-0064-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romer, D., & Jamieson, P. (2001). The role of perceived risk in starting and stopping smoking. In Slovic, P. (Eds.), Smoking: Risk, perception, & policy. Sage Publications, Inc Thousand Oaks, CA: 6480. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452232652.n4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose-Krasnor, L., & Denham, S. (2009). Social-emotional competence in early childhood. In Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Laursen, B. (Eds.), Social, emotional, and personality development in context: Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups. Guilford Press New York, NY: 162179.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: Temperament and personality in development. Guilford Press, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. (2006). Temperament. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Richard, L. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development. vol. 3, 6th. Wiley Hoboken, NJ: 99166, https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0303,Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 2139, https://www.jstor.org/stable/23087906 Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years. In Molfese, V. J., & Molfese, D. L. (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Hillsdale, NJ: 85119.Google Scholar
Rubin, K., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development. vol. 3, 6th. Wiley Hoboken, NJ: 571645, https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0310,Google Scholar
Rydell, A., Berlin, L., & Bohlin, G. (2003). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and adaptation among 5- to 8-year-old children. Emotion, 3(1), 3047. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.30 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rydell, A., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2007). Emotion regulation in relation to social functioning: An investigation of child self-reports. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 293313. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620600783526 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., & Witherington, D. (2006). Emotional development: Action, communication, and understanding. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development. vol. 3, 6th. Wiley Hoboken, NJ: 226299, https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0305,Google Scholar
Sallquist, J. V., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Reiser, M., Hofer, C., Zhou, Q.Eggum, N. (2009). Positive and negative emotionality: Trajectories across six years and relations with social competence. Emotion, 9(1), 1528. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013970 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(8), 10081015. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199908000-00017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankman, S. A., Tenke, C. E., Bruder, G. E., Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., & Klein, D. N. (2005). Low positive emotionality in young children: Association with EEG asymmetry. Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 8598. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579405050054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school age children: The development and validation of a new criterion q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 906916. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiner, R. L., Buss, K. A., McClowry, S. G., Putnam, S. P., Saudino, K. J., & Zentner, M. (2012). What is temperament now? Assessing progress in temperament research on the twenty-fifth anniversary of Goldsmith et al., (1987). Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 436444. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00254.x Google Scholar
Shortt, J. W., Capaldi, D. M., Dishion, T. J., Bank, L., & Owen, L. D. (2003). The role of adolescent friends, romantic partners, and siblings in the emergence of the adult antisocial lifestyle. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 521533. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.521 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriver, L. H., Dollar, J. M., Lawless, M., Wideman, L., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P.Shanahan, L. (2019). Longitudinal associations between emotion regulation and adiposity in late adolescence: Indirect effects through eating behaviors. Nutrients, 11(3), 517. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11030517 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigfusdottir, I. D., Farkas, G., & Silver, E. (2004). The role of depressed mood and anger in the relationship between family conflict and delinquent behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33(6), 509522. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOYO.0000048065.17118.63 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spilt, J. L., Hughes, J. N., Wu, J. Y., & Kwok, O. M. (2012). Dynamics of teacher-student relationships: Stability and change across elementary school and the influence on children’s academic success. Child Development, 83(4), 11801195. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01761.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 5559. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Dollar, J. M. (2016). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention. vol. 4, 3rd. Wiley Hoboken, NJ: 546607, https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy411,Google Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2002). The effect of excessive crying on the development of emotion regulation. Infancy, 3(2), 133152. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20(2), 401421. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000199 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, P. Z., Armstrong, L. M., & Cole, P. M. (2013). Relations between temperament and anger regulation over early childhood. Social Development, 22(4), 755772. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00674.x Google ScholarPubMed
Thomas, S. P., Groer, M., Davis, M., Droppleman, P., Mozingo, J., & Pierce, M. (2000). Anger and cancer: An analysis of the linkages. Cancer Nursing, 23(5), 344349. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002820-200010000-00003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trentacosta, C. J., & Shaw, D. S. (2009). Emotional self-regulation, peer rejection, and antisocial behavior: Developmental associations from early childhood to early adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 356365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turcot, V., Lu, Y., Highland, H. M., Schurmann, C., Justice, A. E., Fine, R. S.Loos, R. F. (2018). Protein-altering variants associated with body mass index implicate pathways that control energy intake and expenditure in obesity. Nature Genetics, 50(1), 2641. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-017-0011-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valiente, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Swanson, J., & Reiser, M. (2008). Prediction of children’s academic competence from their effortful control, relationships, and classroom participation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 6777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.67 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valiente, C., Swanson, J., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2012). Kindergartners' temperament, classroom engagement, and student-teacher relationship: Moderation by effortful control. Social Development, 21(3), 558576. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00640.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, F. L., Chassin, L., Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2015). Effortful control predicts adolescent antisocial-aggressive behaviors and depressive symptoms: Co-occurrence and moderation by impulsivity. Child Development, 86(6), 18121829. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12406 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, F. L., Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., & Spinrad, T. L. (2016). Role of temperament in early adolescent pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems using a bifactor model: Moderation by parenting and gender. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt2), 14871504. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415001224 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weeks, M., Coplan, R. J., & Kingsbury, A. (2009). The correlates and consequences of early appearing social anxiety in young children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(7), 965972. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.06.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wideman, L., Calkins, S. D., Janssen, J. A., Lovelady, C. A., Dollar, J. M., Keane, S. P.Shanahan, L. (2016). Rationale, design and methods for the RIGHT Track Health Study: Pathways from childhood self-regulation to cardiovascular risk in adolescence. BMP Public Health, 1(16), 459. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3133-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. R., Fox, N. A., Lejuez, C. W., Reynolds, E. K., Henderson, H. A., Perez-Edgar, K. E.Pine, D. S. (2010). Early temperament, propensity for risk-taking and adolescent substance-related problems: A prospective multi-method investigation. Addictive Behaviors, 35(12), 11481151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Correlations and descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Figure 1. Standardized estimates and model fit. Model Fit: χ2(46, N = 383) = 103.20, p = .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .05 [CI = .04, .07]. All model paths shown; Significant paths bolded and include coefficients; A = adolescent report; B = biological measure; P = parent report; T = teacher report.

Figure 2

Table 2. Standardized estimates of significant indirect effects, standard errors, and 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals