Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-rkxrd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T18:50:54.700Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spanish Version of the Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ-S)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Mónica Teresa González-Ramírez*
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León (México)
María Eugenia Mendoza-González
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León (México)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mónica T. González Ramírez. C/ Dr. Carlos Canseco 110. Colonia Mitras Centro. C.P. 64310 Monterrey.Nuevo León. (Mexico). Phone: +52-8183338233. Fax: +52-81833338222. E-mail: monygzz77@yahoo.com

Abstract

The concept of prospective memory is relatively recent. Thus, it is necessary to have instruments to assess it, in interaction with retrospective memory. The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ; Smith et al., 2000) is an instrument that has been effectively used for assessment in several languages. Nevertheless, there is still no Spanish adaptation. The aim of this study was to translate the PRMQ and obtain information about its psychometric properties. A back translation process was applied to the PRMQ. The resulting PRMQ-S was self-administered to 520 subjects after applying the pilot test. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to test competing models of its latent structure. Internal consistency was evaluated using Cronbach's alpha. The results showed an adequate internal consistency of the total scale and the Prospective and Retrospective scales were: .89, .84 and .76, respectively. The factor structure was compared with the original study. Our results suggested a disagreement with the tripartite model defended by the authors of the PRMQ. Since there is a high correlation between two factors in the bi-factor model, and the tripartite model showed non-significant loadings to the two specific factors, we recommend that the PRMQ-S be used as one general memory score. We demonstrated adequate psychometric properties of the PRMQ-S if it is used as unique measurement of memory failures. These results are relevant due to the need for a short and accurate instrument in our language that explores memory failures, which has a major impact on the activities of daily living.

El concepto de memoria prospectiva es relativamente reciente. Por lo que es necesario contar con instrumentos para evaluarla en interacción con la memoria retrospectiva. El Cuestionario de Memoria Prospectiva y retrospectiva (PRMQ; Smith et al., 2000) es un instrumento que ha sido eficientemente utilizado para evaluarlas, en varios idiomas. Sin embargo, no se detectó versión alguna en español de dicho cuestionario. El propósito de este estudio fue traducir el PRMQ y obtener información sobre sus propiedades psicométricas. Un proceso de traducción inversa fue aplicado al PRMQ, el resultante PRMQ-S se aplicó a 520 personas, después de una prueba piloto. Para los análisis se utilizó el Análisis Factorial Confirmatorio y el Alfa de Cronbach. Los resultados reflejaron adecuada consistencia interna de la escala completa y de las subescalas de memoria prospectiva y retrospectiva: .89, .84 y .76, respectivamente. La estructura factorial fue comparada con el estudio original. Nuestros resultados sugieren un desacuerdo con el modelo tripartito defendido por los autores del PRMQ. La fuerte correlación entre los dos factores en el modelo bifactorial y el modelo tripartito, que muestra pesos factoriales no significativos, dan la pauta para recomendar que el PRMQ-S sea utilizado como un puntaje general de memoria. Nuestros resultados demuestran adecuadas propiedades psicométricas al ser utilizado como una medida única de fallos en la memoria. Estos resultados son relevantes debido a la necesidad de un instrumento breve en español que evalúe los fallos en la memoria que afectan el desempeño en las actividades cotidianas.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Crawford, J. R., Smith, G. S., Maylor, E. A., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. (2003). The prospective and retrospective questionnaire (PRMQ): Normative data and latent structure in a large non clinical sample. Memory, 11, 261275. doi:10.1080/09658210244000027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J. R., Henry, J. D., Ward, A. L., & Blake, J. (2006). The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ): Latent structure, normative data and discrepancy analysis for proxy-ratings. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 83104. doi:10.1348/014466505X28748CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Richardson, S. L., Guynn, M. J., & Cunfer, A. R. (1995). Aging and prospective memory: examining the influence of self-initiated retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 9961007. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.21.4.996Google ScholarPubMed
Ellis, J., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2000). Prospective memory in 2000: Past, present and future directions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 19. doi:10.1002/acp.767.absCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleming, J., Riley, L., Gill, H., Gullo, M. J., Strong, J., & Shum, D. (2008). Predictors of prospective memory in adults with traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 823831. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080971CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Livner, A., Laukka., E., Karlsson, S., & Bäckman, L. (2009). Prospective and retrospective memory in Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: Similar patterns of impairment. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 283, 235239. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2009.02.296CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maylor, E. A. (1990). Age and prospective memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 42, 471–93. doi:10.1080/14640749008401233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Ayán, M. N., & Ruiz, M. A. (2008). The Reduction of skewness and kurtosis of observed variables by data transformation: Effect on factor structure. Psicológica, 29, 205–127.Google Scholar
Rönnlund, M., Mäntylä, T., & Nilsson, L. (2008). The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ): factorial structure, relations to global subjective memory ratings, and Swedish norms. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 49, 1118. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00600.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, G., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., & Maylor, E. A. (2000). Prospective and Retrospective Memory in Normal Aging and Dementia: A Questionnaire Study. Memory, 8, 311321. doi:10.1080/09658210050117735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shum, D., Fleming, J. M., & Neulinger, K. (2002). Prospective memory and traumatic brain injury: A review. Brain Impairment, 3, 116. doi:10.1375/brim.3.1.1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tirapu-Ustarroz, J., & Muñoz-Cespedes, J. M., (2005). Memoria y funciones ejecutivas. Revista de Neurología, 41, 475484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, R., & Craik, F. I. (1999). Age-related decline in prospective memory: the roles of cue accessibility and cue sensitivity. Psychology and Aging, 14, 264272. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.14.2.264CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed