Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T09:56:11.121Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Role of Surprise in the Discrimination of the Facial Expression of Fear

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2018

Fernando Gordillo
Affiliation:
Universidad Camilo José Cela (Spain)
Lilia Mestas*
Affiliation:
Facultad de Estudios Superiores Zaragoza (Mexico)
Miguel Ángel Pérez
Affiliation:
Universidad Camilo José Cela (Spain)
José M. Arana
Affiliation:
Universidad de Salamanca (Spain)
Eduardo Alejandro Escotto
Affiliation:
Facultad de Estudios Superiores Zaragoza (Mexico)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lilia Mestas. Facultad de Estudios Superiores Zaragoza. Psicología. C/ Batalla 5 de Mayo s/n. Esquina Fuerte de Loreto. 09230 - Mexico DF (Mexico). E-mail: mesherli@comunidad.unam.mx

Abstract

The facilitating role of the facial expression of surprise in the discrimination of the facial expression of fear was analyzed. The sample consisted of 202 subjects that undertook a forced-choice test in which they had to decide as quickly as possible whether the facial expression displayed on-screen was one of fear, anger or happiness. Variations were made to the prime expression (neutral expression, or one of surprise); the target expression (facial expression of fear, anger or happiness), and the prime duration (50 ms, 150 ms or 250 ms). The results revealed shorter reaction times in the response to the expression of fear when the prime expression was one of surprise, with a prime duration of 50 ms (p = .009) and 150 ms (p = .001), compared to when the prime expression was a neutral one. By contrast, the reaction times were longer in the discrimination of an expression of fear when the prime expression was one of surprise with a prime duration of 250 ms (p < .0001), compared to when the prime expression was a neutral one. This pattern of results was obtained solely in the discrimination of the expression of fear. The discussion focuses on these findings and the possible functional continuity between surprise and fear.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

How to cite this article:

Gordillo, F., Mestas, L., Pérez, M. A., Arana, J. M., & Escotto, E. A. (2018). Role of surprise in the discrimination of the facial expression of fear. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21. e3. Doi:10.1017/sjp.2018.5

References

Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: Amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 5459. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411430832 Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J., Sherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2000). The handbook of affective sciences. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Du, S., & Martínez, A. M. (2013). Wait, are you sad or angry? Large exposure time differences required for the categorization of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Vision, 13(4), 114. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.4.13 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572595). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115141. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930125908 Google Scholar
Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2001). A time course analysis of the affective priming effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 143165. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930125768 Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. G. (2014). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over time. Current Biology, 24, 187192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.064 Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Sun, W., Delis, I., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. G. (2016). Four not six: Revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 708730. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000162 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H., Somerville, L. H., Johnstone, T., Alexander, A. L., & Whalen, P. J. (2003). Inverse amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to surprised faces. NeuroReport, 14, 23172322. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000101520.44335.20 Google Scholar
Klauer, K., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In Musch, J. & Klauer, K. (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 749). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional networks in the brain. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Feldman, L. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Feldman, L. (Eds.) (2008). Handbook of emotions (3 rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M. N., Frank, M. G., & O’Sullivan, M. (2008). What’s in a face? Facial expressions as signals of discrete emotions. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Feldman, L. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 211234). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8, 423429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00455.x Google Scholar
Neta, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2011). The primacy of negative interpretations when resolving the valence of ambiguous facial expressions. Psychological Science, 21(7), 901907. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373934 Google Scholar
Öhman, A. (2002). Automaticity and the amygdala: Nonconscious responses to emotional faces. Current Directions in Psychological Science,11, 6266. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00169 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., & Wiens, S. (2003). On the automaticity of autonomic responses in emotion: An evolutionary perspective. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K., & Hill, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 256275). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 773783. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2920 Google Scholar
Posamentier, M. T., & Abdi, H. (2003). Processing Faces and Facial Expressions. Neuropsychology Review, 13(3), 113143. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025519712569 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reisenzein, R., & Studtmann, M. (2007). On the expression and experience of surprise: No evidence for facial feedback, but evidence for a reverse self-inference effect. Emotion, 7, 612627. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.612 Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1984). Emotion and multicomponent process: A model and some cross-cultural data. In Shaver, P. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 3763). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Spinoza, B., Ethics, translated by Boyle, Andrew and revised by Parkinson, G. H. R. (1989). London, UK: J. M. Dent. (Original work published 1677)Google Scholar
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., … Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006 Google Scholar
Tottenham, N., Phuong, J., Flannery, J., Gabard-Durnam, L., & Goff, B. (2013). A negativity bias for ambiguous facial-expression valence during childhood: Converging evidence from behavior and facial corrugator muscle responses. Emotion, 13(1), 92103. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029431 Google Scholar
Vrticka, P., Lordier, L., Bediou, B., & Sander, D. (2014). Human amygdala response to dynamic facial expressions of positive and negative surprise. Emotion, 14(1), 161169. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034619 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vytal, K., & Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 28642885. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21366 Google Scholar
Wagner, H. L. (1993). On measuring performance in category judgment studies of nonverbal behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17(1), 328. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987006 Google Scholar
Zillmann, D. (1971). Excitation transfer in communication-mediated aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 419434. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(71)90075-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar