Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T04:45:51.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Examining the feasibility of implementing behavioural economics strategies that encourage home dinner vegetable intake among low-income children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 March 2017

Tashara M Leak*
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, University of California, 50 University Hall, #7360, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA Department of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN, USA
Alison Swenson
Affiliation:
Department of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN, USA
Aaron Rendahl
Affiliation:
School of Statistics, University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN, USA
Zata Vickers
Affiliation:
Department of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN, USA
Elton Mykerezi
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Economics, University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN, USA
Joseph P Redden
Affiliation:
Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Traci Mann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Marla Reicks
Affiliation:
Department of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Minnesota, Saint Paul, MN, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email tashara.leak@berkeley.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To examine the feasibility of implementing nine behavioural economics-informed strategies, or ‘nudges’, that aimed to encourage home dinner vegetable intake among low-income children.

Design

Caregivers were assigned six of nine strategies and implemented one new strategy per week (i.e. 6 weeks) during three dinner meals. Caregivers recorded child dinner vegetable intake on the nights of strategy implementation and rated the level of difficulty for assigned strategies. Baseline data on home vegetable availability and child vegetable liking were collected to assess overall strategy feasibility.

Setting

Participants’ homes in a large Midwestern metropolitan area, USA.

Subjects

Low-income caregiver/child (aged 9–12 years) dyads (n 39).

Results

Pairwise comparisons showed that child dinner vegetable intake for the strategy ‘Serve at least two vegetables with dinner meals’ was greater than intake for each of two other strategies: ‘Pair vegetables with other foods the child likes’ and ‘Eat dinner together with an adult(s) modelling vegetable consumption’. Overall, caregivers’ mean rating of difficulty for implementing strategies was 2·6 (1=‘not difficult’, 10=‘very difficult’). Households had a mean of ten different types of vegetables available. Children reported a rating ≥5 for seventeen types of vegetable on a labelled hedonic scale (1=‘hate it’, 5–6=‘it’s okay’, 10=‘like it a lot’).

Conclusions

Behavioural economics-informed strategies are feasible to implement during dinner meals, with some strategies differing by how much they influence vegetable intake among low-income children in the home.

Type
Short Communications
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2017 

Children in the USA are not meeting recommendations for vegetable intake( 1 , 2 ). One factor that may influence child vegetable intake is whether they like vegetables( Reference Anzman-Frasca, Savage and Marini 3 Reference Fildes, van Jaarsveld and Wardle 5 ). Low-income children may experience additional barriers, such as limited home vegetable availability( Reference Kirkpatrick, Dodd and Reedy 6 , Reference Fram, Ritchie and Rosen 7 ). Thus interventions that aim to improve child vegetable intake should consider vegetable liking and availability( Reference Tak, te Velde and Brug 8 ).

One approach used in the school setting to improve child vegetable intake is the implementation of strategies informed by behavioural economics, or ‘nudges’( Reference Just and Wansink 9 , Reference Thaler and Sunstein 10 ). Behavioural economics, a sub-field grounded in principles of psychology and economics( Reference Just and Wansink 9 ), is based on the premise that the social and physical environment can be framed in a way to ‘nudge’ individuals to make healthy choices, in a manner that does not limit the choice set itself( Reference Thaler and Sunstein 10 , Reference Heshmat 11 ). For example, school cafeterias have been manipulated to increase child vegetable intake by improving aspects of convenience, attractiveness and normativeness( Reference Hanks, Just and Wansink 12 ). A systematic review of studies implementing nudges in school settings has shown the utility of these approaches in improving eating behaviour among children( Reference Kessler 13 ).

The home environment is a promising setting for studies implementing nudges because children consume about 66 % of their daily energy intake at home( Reference Poti and Popkin 14 ). In a home-based, 4-week, randomized-controlled, proof-of-concept trial, Cravener et al. showed that offering pre-school children vegetables as a default snack, paired with positive incentives, improved vegetable intake( Reference Cravener, Schlechter and Loeb 15 ). The home is a relatively new setting for the application of these principles and limited studies are available particularly among older children. In addition to offering vegetables as a default option, other less studied strategies may be effective and feasible ways to improve child vegetable intake in the home setting. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to determine the feasibility of implementing nine behavioural economics-informed strategies that aimed to encourage dinner vegetable intake among children aged 9–12 years residing in households receiving food assistance (a proxy for low-income status).

Methods

The methodology for the present study was published previously( Reference Leak, Swenson and Vickers 16 ). It included information about study recruitment, a description about how the nine strategies were identified, details about measures/instruments and training procedures for caregivers to record child dinner vegetable consumption( Reference Leak, Swenson and Vickers 16 ).

Study participants

Inclusion criteria for caregivers included caring for a child between the ages of 9 and 12 years, being the primary food preparer in the home, preparing dinner at least three nights per week, participating in public food assistance programmes (e.g. the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program) and reading, writing and speaking English. Caregivers received $US 100 and their 9–12-year-old received $US 20 for their participation. Approval was obtained from the University of Minnesota Twin Cities Institutional Review Board. Caregivers and their child signed informed consent and assent forms, respectively.

Procedures

Participants were recruited and participated in the study during one of the following three time frames (i.e. time of year): (i) from September to November 2013; (ii) from January to March 2014; or (iii) from March to May 2014. There were relatively equal numbers of parent/child dyads in each of the three time frames. A baseline home visit was conducted to obtain sociodemographic and household information, home vegetable availability and child vegetable liking data. After the baseline home visit, caregivers were randomly assigned six of nine strategies (Table 1). Over a 6-week period, a different strategy was implemented on a weekly basis during at least three dinner meals that week. The order of strategy implementation was randomly assigned.

Table 1 Nine behavioural economics strategies evaluated for feasibility

During a telephone call at the beginning of the first week, researchers assigned caregivers a strategy. Caregivers were referred to a Strategy Guidebook for instructions and examples for how the strategy could be implemented( Reference Leak, Swenson and Vickers 16 ). Caregivers recorded child vegetable intake on the three dinner meals per week when the strategy was implemented using a Dinner Vegetable Record Form. At the end of each week during an audio-recorded telephone call with a trained researcher, caregivers reported child vegetable consumption from the Dinner Vegetable Record Form. Caregivers were also asked to rate the difficulty of using the strategy. At the end of the call, researchers assigned a new strategy. This procedure was followed until caregivers completed six strategies.

Measures

Sociodemographic and household characteristics

Caregivers completed a questionnaire during the baseline home visit to report age, gender, race/ethnicity, highest educational attainment and employment status, as well as child age, gender and race/ethnicity, and household composition. An amended version of the US Department of Agriculture’s six-item Food Security Survey was used to assess household food security( 17 ).

Feasibility of strategies

On the three nights that caregivers implemented a strategy, caregivers recorded child vegetable consumption on the Dinner Vegetable Record Form, along with the vegetable type(s) and amount consumed. Caregivers also rated the level of difficulty for each of the strategies they were assigned (1=‘not difficult’, 10=‘very difficult’). A shortened version of a previously validated home food inventory was used to document the presence of thirty-six vegetables, including four types of legumes( Reference Fulkerson, Nelson and Lytle 18 ). During the baseline home visit, a researcher documented the presence of these vegetables (1=‘yes’, 0=‘no’). Children also provided liking ratings for these thirty-six vegetable types using a labelled hedonic scale (1=‘hate it’, 5–6=‘it’s okay’, 10=‘like it a lot’).

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics were calculated to examine the distribution of sociodemographic and household characteristics.

Mean dinner vegetable intake was calculated over the three days each strategy was implemented in its assigned week. Vegetable intakes for the strategies were then compared using a mixed-model ANOVA with a random effect for child. Fixed independent variables included strategies, the week of strategy implementation (as a continuous measure) and the time of year when the strategy was implemented. The week × time of year interaction was also included in the model. Multiple pairwise comparisons of the strategies were evaluated using the Tukey Honestly Significant Difference test. Least-square means for dinner vegetable intake per each of the nine strategies and 95 % confidence intervals were reported.

Potential covariates were tested separately in the mixed model to determine effects on differences in mean child dinner vegetable intake between each strategy. Covariates included caregiver’s age, race/ethnicity, caregiver education and employment status, number of individuals residing in the household, household food security, home vegetable availability and average vegetable liking rating score across thirty-six vegetable types. Differences were not observed and thus the final mixed-model ANOVA was not adjusted for any covariates. Level of significance was set at α=0·05.

Mean difficulty rating for each strategy, mean total number of different vegetables available in homes, mean vegetable liking across all vegetables and mean number of vegetable types liked by children were calculated.

Results

Based on previously published sample size calculations( Reference Leak, Swenson and Vickers 16 ), forty-seven caregiver/child dyads were recruited and completed the baseline home visit. Eight families withdrew from the study and thus thirty-nine caregiver/child dyads were included in the final analyses. Sociodemographic and household characteristics for the thirty-nine caregiver/child dyads are described in Table 2. Most caregivers were female (97·4 %) and Black/African American (47·4 %) or White (36·8 %). The majority had some college education (63·2 %) and had low or very low food security status (65·7 %). About half of the children were boys (51·3 %). Mean child age was 10·4 years.

Table 2 Sociodemographic and household characteristics of low-income caregiver/child dyads (n 39) from a large Midwestern metropolitan area, USA, 2013–2014

GED, General Educational Development.

Feasibility of strategies

Mean child dinner vegetable intake ranged from 0·77 to 1·20 cups by week of implementation of the nine different strategies (Table 3). Significant effects were observed for strategy (P=0·02), but not for week of strategy implementation (P=0·99), time of year when the strategy was implemented (P=0·54) or week × time of year interaction (P=0·47). Children consumed significantly more vegetables when the ‘Serve at least two vegetables with dinner meals’ strategy was implemented compared with when ‘Pair vegetables with other foods child likes’ (0·43 more cups; P=0·01) and ‘Eat dinner together with an adult(s) modelling vegetable consumption’ (0·39 more cups; P=0·04) were implemented. Mean dinner vegetable intake for the remaining six strategies was not different when compared with ‘Serve at least two vegetables with dinner meals’, ‘Pair vegetables with other foods child likes’ and ‘Eat dinner together with an adult(s) modelling vegetable consumption’ strategies (Table 3).

Table 3 Impact of behavioural economics strategies on child mean dinner vegetable intake among low-income caregiver/child dyads (n 39) from a large Midwestern metropolitan area, USA, 2013–2014

a,bMean values within a column with unlike superscript letters were significantly different according to Tukey Honestly Significant Difference pairwise comparisons (P<0·05).

* Within a week, a caregiver may have provided Dinner Vegetable Food Record data from one to three dinners.

Least-squares means and se from mixed-model ANOVA.

Caregivers rated the difficulty of implementing the strategies with an overall mean of 2·6, with mean scores for individual strategies ranging from 2·1 to 2·9 on a 10-point scale. Households had on average 10 (sd 3·9) different types of vegetables present in the home. On average, children had tried 24 (sd 4·9) different vegetable types. The mean vegetable liking rating for children was 6·7 (sd 1·1). They rated 17 (sd 5·0) types of vegetable ≥5 on the 10-point scale.

Discussion

Findings suggest that the nine tested strategies had a similar effect on mean child dinner vegetable intake. The ‘Serve at least two vegetables with dinner meals’ strategy may be particularly effective at encouraging child vegetable intake in the home environment, a finding underscored by previous studies reporting that serving more than one vegetable significantly increased both the selection( Reference Bucher, Siegrist and van der Horst 19 ) and consumption of vegetables( Reference Roe, Meengs and Birch 20 ) in settings outside the home. Serving more than one type of food (e.g. vegetables) is a visual cue that nudges people into eating more( Reference Wadhera and Capaldi-Phillips 21 ).

In the current study, one strategy was implemented per week; however, in previous school-based interventions, multiple behavioural economics strategies have been implemented simultaneously in cafeterias in what is known as ‘smarter lunchroom makeovers’( Reference Hanks, Just and Wansink 12 , Reference Song, Grutzmacher and Munger 22 ). Making these small environmental changes simultaneously has resulted in positive eating behaviour changes for children in school settings. Results from the current study indicate that single strategies implemented in the home had similar effects on dinner vegetable intake among children. Therefore, the use of multiple strategies by caregivers simultaneously may also be an effective means to improve dietary intake at home. Simultaneous implementation of multiple strategies in school cafeterias may be feasible because of the availability of dedicated staff and resources; however, further research is needed to determine whether caregivers can easily manage implementing multiple strategies at home.

A variety of vegetable types were available in homes and children generally liked vegetables, both of which support the low ratings of difficulty by caregivers. As other studies have reported, availability and child food preferences often influence food preparation and mealtime practices( Reference Brown and Wenrich 23 , Reference Berge, Hoppmann and Hanson 24 ). To avoid food waste, low-income caregivers may limit the type of vegetables they purchase to those their children like. Brown and Wenrich reported that food waste is a barrier for low-income caregivers regarding preparation of unfamiliar vegetables( Reference Brown and Wenrich 23 ). Interviews and grocery shopping observations by Daniel also showed that avoidance of food waste by low-income parents limits choice of foods to those their children prefer( Reference Daniel 25 ).

The present study has several limitations. First, baseline and follow-up child dinner vegetable intake record data were not collected. These data would have helped determine the cumulative effect of implementing the behavioural economics strategies. Also, there may have been measurement error when caregivers reported child dinner vegetable intake on the nights when strategies were implemented. Another limitation is that there was no within-group control. Future studies should consider collecting child dinner vegetable intake on nights when strategies are not implemented. Lastly, the Strategy Guidebook allowed for caregivers to implement the strategies in various ways, which could have influenced the resulting vegetable intake. Despite these limitations, the current study addresses a major gap in the literature regarding the feasibility of implementing behavioural economics strategies in the home setting. Given the success of nudges in the school environment, incorporating similar strategies in the home could potentially further improve child vegetable intake. Future studies should consider allowing parents to self-select strategies they would like to implement in order to account for vegetable availability, as well as child vegetable preferences.

Acknowledgements

Financial support: This material is based upon work (completed at the University of Minnesota) that was supported by the National Institute of Food and Agriculture, US Department of Agriculture (award number 2012-68001-19631). Any opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the view of the US Department of Agriculture. The National Institute of Food and Agriculture, US Department of Agriculture had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: M.R., Z.V., E.M., J.P.R., T.M., A.S., and T.M.L. formulated the research questions and designed the study. T.M.L. and A.S. conducted the study, while A.R. and T.M.L. analysed the data. T.M.L., Z.V. and M.R. wrote the article. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and the University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board approved all procedures involving human subjects/patients. Written informed consent and assent was obtained from all parents/caregivers and child participants, respectively.

References

1. US Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service (2016) Food Patterns Equivalents Intakes from Food: Mean Amounts Consumed per Individual, by Gender and Age. What We Eat in America, NHANES 2011–2012. http://www.ars.usda.gov/ba/bhnrc/fsrg (accessed August 2016).Google Scholar
2. US Department of Health and Human Services & US Department of Agriculture (2015) 2015–2020 Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 8th ed. http://health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/guidelines/ (accessed August 2016).Google Scholar
3. Anzman-Frasca, S, Savage, JS, Marini, ME et al. (2012) Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children’s liking of vegetables. Appetite 58, 543553.Google Scholar
4. Caton, SJ, Ahern, SM, Remy, E et al. (2013) Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavour–flavour and flavour–nutrient learning. Br J Nutr 109, 20892097.Google Scholar
5. Fildes, A, van Jaarsveld, CH, Wardle, J et al. (2014) Parent-administered exposure to increase children’s vegetable acceptance: a randomized controlled trial. J Acad Nutr Diet 114, 881888.Google Scholar
6. Kirkpatrick, SI, Dodd, KW, Reedy, J et al. (2012) Income and race/ethnicity are associated with adherence to food-based dietary guidance among US adults and children. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 624635.Google Scholar
7. Fram, MS, Ritchie, LD, Rosen, N et al. (2015) Child experience of food insecurity is associated with child diet and physical activity. J Nutr 145, 499504.Google Scholar
8. Tak, NI, te Velde, SJ & Brug, J (2009) Long-term effects of the Dutch Schoolgruiten Project – promoting fruit and vegetable consumption among primary-school children. Public Health Nutr 12, 12131223.Google Scholar
9. Just, DR & Wansink, B (2009) Smarter lunchrooms: using behavioral economics to improve meal selection. Choices 24, 17.Google Scholar
10. Thaler, RH & Sunstein, CR (2008) Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
11. Heshmat, S (2011) Eating Behavior and Obesity: Behavioral Economics Strategies for Health Professionals. New York: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
12. Hanks, AS, Just, DR & Wansink, B (2013) Smarter lunchrooms can address new school lunchroom guidelines and childhood obesity. J Pediatr 162, 867869.Google Scholar
13. Kessler, HS (2016) Simple interventions to improve healthy eating behaviors in the school cafeteria. Nutr Rev 74, 198209.Google Scholar
14. Poti, JM & Popkin, BM (2011) Trends in energy intake among US children by eating location and food source, 1977–2006. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 11561164.Google Scholar
15. Cravener, TL, Schlechter, H, Loeb, KL et al. (2015) Feeding strategies derived from behavioral economics and psychology can increase vegetable intake in children as part of a home-based intervention: results of a pilot study. J Acad Nutr Diet 115, 17981807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Leak, TM, Swenson, A, Vickers, Z et al. (2015) Testing the effectiveness of in-home behavioral economics strategies to increase vegetable intake, liking, and variety among children residing in households that receive food assistance. J Nutr Educ Behav 47, e1e9.Google Scholar
17. US Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service (2016) Six-Item Short Form of the Food Security Survey Module. http://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/food-nutrition-assistance/food-security-in-theus/survey-tools.aspx#six (accessed August 2016).Google Scholar
18. Fulkerson, JA, Nelson, MC, Lytle, L et al. (2008) The validation of a home food inventory. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 5, 55.Google Scholar
19. Bucher, T, Siegrist, M & van der Horst, K (2014) Vegetable variety: an effective strategy to increase vegetable choice in children. Public Health Nutr 17, 12321236.Google Scholar
20. Roe, LS, Meengs, JS, Birch, LL et al. (2013) Serving a variety of vegetables and fruit as a snack increased intake in preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr 98, 693699.Google Scholar
21. Wadhera, D & Capaldi-Phillips, ED (2014) A review of visual cues associated with food on food acceptance and consumption. Eat Behav 15, 132143.Google Scholar
22. Song, HJ, Grutzmacher, S & Munger, AL (2016) Project ReFresh: testing the efficacy of school-based classroom and cafeteria intervention in elementary school children. J Sch Health 86, 543551.Google Scholar
23. Brown, JL & Wenrich, TR (2012) Intra-family role expectations and reluctance to change identified as key barriers to expanding vegetable consumption patterns during interactive family-based program for Appalachian low-income food preparers. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 11881200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Berge, JM, Hoppmann, C, Hanson, C et al. (2013) Perspectives about family meals from single-headed and dual-headed households: a qualitative analysis. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 16321639.Google Scholar
25. Daniel, C (2016) Economic constraints on taste formation and the true cost of healthy eating. Soc Sci Med 148, 3441.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Nine behavioural economics strategies evaluated for feasibility

Figure 1

Table 2 Sociodemographic and household characteristics of low-income caregiver/child dyads (n 39) from a large Midwestern metropolitan area, USA, 2013–2014

Figure 2

Table 3 Impact of behavioural economics strategies on child mean dinner vegetable intake among low-income caregiver/child dyads (n 39) from a large Midwestern metropolitan area, USA, 2013–2014