Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T19:29:00.735Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Physiological and psychological measures in anxious patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Alyson J. Bond
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London
D. Colin James
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London
Malcolm H. Lader
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London

Sysnopsis

Thirty drug-free patients suffering from chronic anxiety states were compared with 30 normal controls matched for age, sex, and social class on a variety of physiological and psychological measures. The tests included the electroencephalogram, the auditory evoked response, and skin conductance recorded during a passive and an active condition and auditory reaction time, the digit symbol substitution test, and arithmetic. The patients showed increased EEG voltage, shorter latencies of the evoked response, higher skin conductance levels, higher pulse rate and less pupillary constriction, and they responded less to the increase in activation. They also showed impairment on complex psychological tests. It is concluded that pathological anxiety involves an increase in arousal irrelevant to the task and has a disorganizing rather than a facilitating effect on performance.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1974

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Angyal, A. F. (1948). The diagnosis of neurotic traits by means of a new perceptual test. Journal of Psychology, 25, 105135.Google Scholar
Bellis, C. J. (1933). Reaction time and chronological age. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 30, 801803.Google Scholar
Bond, A. J., and Lader, M. H. (1972). Residual effects of hypnotics. Psychopharmacologia, 25, 117132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, A. J., James, D. C., and Lader, M. H. (1974). Sedative effects on physiological and psychological measures in anxious patients. Psychological Medicine. (In press.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostock, H., and Jarvis, M. J. (1970). Changes in the form of the cerebral evoked response related to the speed of simple reaction time. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 29, 137145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brazier, M. A. B., Finesinger, J. E., and Cobb, S. (1945). A contrast between the electroencephalograms of 100 psychoneurotic patients and those of 500 normal adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 101, 443448.Google Scholar
Ellingson, R. J. (1954). The incidence of EEG abnormality among patients with mental disorders of apparently non-organic origin: a critical review. American Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 263275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, H. B. (1965). Manual of the Gibson Spiral Maze. University of London Press: London.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1935). The development of the reactive process from early childhood to maturity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 431450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, E. S. (1958). GSR conditioning in anxiety states, normals, and chronic functional schizophrenic subjects. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 183189.Google Scholar
Kamin, L. J., and Clark, J. W. (1957). The Taylor scale and reaction time. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 262263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lader, M. H. (1967). Palmar skin conductance measures in anxiety and phobic states. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 271281.Google Scholar
Lader, M. H., Bond, A. J., and James, D. C. (1974). Clinical comparison of anxiolytic drug therapy. Psychological Medicine, 4, 381387.Google Scholar
Lader, M. H., and Marks, I. (1972). Clinical Anxiety. Heinemann: London.Google Scholar
Lader, M. H., and Wing, L. (1966). Physiological measures, sedative drugs and morbid anxiety. Maudsley Monographs, No. 14. Oxford University Press: London.Google Scholar
Mandler, G., and Sarason, S. B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 166173.Google Scholar
Martin, B. (1961). The assessment of anxiety by physiological behavioral measures. Psychological Bulletin, 58, 234255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matarazzo, J. D., and Phillips, J. S. (1955). Digit symbol performance as a function of increasing levels of anxiety. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 19, 131134.Google Scholar
Maxwell, A. E. (1972). The WPPSI: a marked discrepancy in the correlations of the subtests for good and poor readers. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 25, 283291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., Kaye, J. J., and Lader, M. (1972). Regular interstimulus interval as a critical determinant of short-term “habituation” of the auditory averaged evoked response. Psychonomic Science, 27, 275278.Google Scholar
Roubiček, J. (1969). Anxiety and higher nervous functions. In Studies of Anxiety, pp. 2125. Edited by M. H. Lader. British Journal of Psychiatry, Special Publication No. 3.Google Scholar
Rubin, L. S. (1964). Autonomic dysfunction as a concomitant of neurotic behavior. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 138, 558574.Google Scholar
Sherman, M., and Jost, H. (1942). Frustration reactions of normal and neurotic persons. Journal of Psychology, 13, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stennett, R. G. (1957). The relationship of alpha amplitude to the level of palmar conductance. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 9, 131138.Google Scholar
Strauss, H. (1945). Clinical and electroencephalographic studies: the electroencephalogram in psychoneurotics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 101, 1927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ulett, G. A., Gleser, G. C., Winokur, G., and Lawler, A. (1953). The EEG and reaction to photic stimulation as an index of anxiety-proneness. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 5, 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Psychological Corporation: New York.Google Scholar
Wenar, C. (1954). Reaction time as a function of manifest anxiety and stimulus intensity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 335340.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, R. T., and Morlock, H. C. (1967). Auditory evoked response and reaction time. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 23, 5056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, L. (1964). Physiological effects of performing a difficult task in patients with anxiety states. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 7, 283294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodworth, R. S., and Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental Psychology. 3rd edn.Methuen: London.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 18, 459482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar