Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T06:43:03.243Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Four reasons why early detection centers for psychosis should be renamed and their treatment targets reconsidered: we should not catastrophize a future we can neither reliably predict nor change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2019

Steffen Moritz*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
Łukasz Gawęda
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany Psychopathology and Early Intervention Lab, II Department of Psychiatry, The Medical University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
Andreas Heinz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Charitéplatz, Berlin, Germany
Jürgen Gallinat
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Steffen Moritz, E-mail: moritz@uke.uni-hamburg.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Since the 1990s, facilities for individuals at putative risk for psychosis have mushroomed and within a very short time have become part of the standard psychiatric infrastructure in many countries. The idea of preventing a severe mental disorder before its exacerbation is laudable, and early data indeed strongly suggested that the sooner the intervention, the better the outcome. In this paper, the authors provide four reasons why they think that early detection or prodromal facilities should be renamed and their treatment targets reconsidered. First, the association between the duration of untreated psychosis and outcome is empirically established but has become increasingly weak over the years. Moreover, its applicability to those who are considered at risk remains elusive. Second, instruments designed to identify future psychosis are prone to many biases that are not yet sufficiently controlled. None of these instruments allows an even remotely precise prognosis. Third, the rate of transition to psychosis in at-risk patients is likely lower than initially thought, and evidence for the success of early intervention in preventing future psychosis is promising but still equivocal. Perhaps most importantly, the treatment is not hope-oriented. Patients are more or less told that schizophrenia is looming over them, which may stigmatize individuals who will never, in fact, develop psychosis. In addition self-stigma has been associated with suicidality and depression. The authors recommend that treatment of help-seeking individuals with mental problems but no established diagnosis should be need-based, and the risk of psychosis should be de-emphasized as it is only one of many possible outcomes, including full remission. Prodromal clinics should not be abolished but should be renamed and restructured. Such clinics exist, but the transformation process needs to be facilitated.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2019. Published by Cambridge University Press

Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation (PACE) works with young people who might be at risk of developing psychosis. By identifying people who are at risk of psychosis and providing them with appropriate treatment, it is hoped that early symptoms will be reduced, while also delaying or perhaps preventing the development of mental health problems.

– PACE website, 3 November 2018

Introduction

Primum non nocere (‘first, to do no harm’), derived from the Hippocratic Oath, represents the guiding principle in medicine. Yet, in the presence of unambiguous and highly predictive risk factors for a serious or life-threatening disorder, it is useful to consider treatment of not yet affected individuals to prevent the transformation of a liability into a full-blown disorder, even though adverse events might occur. Starting in the early 1990s (Birchwood and MacMillan, Reference Birchwood and MacMillan1993; McGorry et al., Reference McGorry, Edwards, Mihalopoulos, Harrigan and Jackson1996), consideration of the cornerstones of responsible therapeutic action – reduction of symptoms as well as prevention of harm – led to the establishment of early detection centers or prodromal clinics for individuals at putative risk of psychosis, such as the Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation center (PACE; see quote above). This trend built upon a number of empirical studies, often published in top-tier journals that have greatly changed the way we look upon psychosis today (Malla et al., Reference Malla, Iyer, McGorry, Cannon, Coughlan, Singh, Jones and Joober2016). For example, schizophrenia is now regarded as a disorder that is preventable and amenable to change – much in contrast to earlier (somatic) models claiming that psychosis is incomprehensible and chronic (Jaspers, Reference Jaspers1963). Although this constituted a significant and valuable paradigm shift at the time, the empirical situation that initiated and accompanied the emergence of the psychosis high-risk concept has since changed. However, significant corrections in how these centers are named and their role in treatment have not been undertaken.

The evidence that led to the early intervention paradigm

Before we formulate our concerns against the early intervention paradigm and facilities for individuals at risk for psychosis,Footnote 1 we first provide a brief overview of the rationale and key arguments for early detection and intervention. We also wish to emphasize that we have no doubts about the probity of the researchers who advocated early detection and treatment. In addition, many of the relevant research studies were of excellent quality. Yet, we do criticize the taking of scattered empirical findings as facts, promoting the widespread establishment of early detection centers worldwide.

To clarify, early intervention can mean two things (Marshall and Rathbone, Reference Marshall and Rathbone2011). The term is used to describe treatment for individuals (mainly adolescents and young adults) in the early stages of manifest (and diagnosed) psychotic disorders, but it also refers to therapeutic efforts to prevent the eventual transition into psychosis in individuals with prodromal symptoms. Our article is directed at the latter, although insights from first episode research, especially studies pertaining to the duration of untreated psychosis (DUP), played an important role in justifying intervention with individuals at (putative) risk for psychosis.

One of the strongest arguments (Birchwood and MacMillan, Reference Birchwood and MacMillan1993; McGorry et al., Reference McGorry, Edwards, Mihalopoulos, Harrigan and Jackson1996) in favor of early detection is that the DUP is a predictor of a more severe course of the illness (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Lewis, Lockwood, Drake, Jones and Croudace2005) and that the best therapeutic window for antipsychotic intervention is the very early phase of psychosis (Perkins et al., Reference Perkins, Gu, Boteva and Lieberman2005). Correlations between DUP and outcome were medium to strong in the early studies. In a seminal paper by McGorry et al. (Reference McGorry, Edwards, Mihalopoulos, Harrigan and Jackson1996), the DUP was able to explain 15% of the variance in later quality of life, and this rose to 24% when the duration of the prodrome was added (p. 314). Thus, the idea emerged that early treatment might mitigate the course of the illness or even prevent a transition into psychosis.

Two major paradigms are implemented in the detection of a high risk for psychotic disorders. The ultra-high-risk (UHR) approach focuses on the presence of attenuated (subsyndromal) or brief positive symptoms or on genetic vulnerability accompanied by functional decline. In Germany and central Europe, the presence of basic symptoms (BS) is often used as an additional criterion for a high risk of psychosis. The latter approach considers subjective disturbances of perception, cognition, and language that may not be observable by others yet are experienced by the individual as a stressful departure from their ‘normal’ state (Andreou et al., Reference Andreou, Bailey and Borgwardt2019). It has been suggested that basic symptoms manifest at an earlier prodromal stage of psychosis than UHR symptoms (Klosterkötter et al., Reference Klosterkötter, Schultze-Lutter, Bechdolf and Ruhrmann2011). However, there are no studies on the comparative predictive validity of the two approaches. A meta-analysis does suggest that brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms (BLIPS) have greater predictive power than attenuated psychotic symptoms (APS; Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Cappucciati, Borgwardt, Woods, Addington, Nelson, Nieman, Stahl, Rutigliano, Riecher-Rössler, Simon, Mizuno, Lee, Kwon, Lam, Perez, Keri, Amminger, Metzler, Kawohl, Rössler, Lee, Labad, Ziermans, An, Liu, Woodberry, Braham, Corcoran, McGorry, Yung and McGuire2016a).

The probability of a high-risk individual developing overt psychotic symptoms has been estimated at about 25% in the first 3 years from a diagnosis of the high-risk state and about 35% overall (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Cappucciati, Rutigliano, Schultze-Lutter, Bonoldi, Borgwardt, Riecher-Rössler, Addington, Perkins, Woods, McGlashan, Lee, Klosterkötter, Yung and McGuire2015; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schultze-Lutter, Schimmelmann, Maric, Salokangas, Riecher-Rössler, van der Gaag, Meneghelli, Nordentoft, Marshall, Morrison, Raballo, Klosterkötter and Ruhrmann2015). Because a high proportion of high-risk individuals will never experience a psychotic disorder, treatment with antipsychotics is usually discouraged by guidelines (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schultze-Lutter, Schimmelmann, Maric, Salokangas, Riecher-Rössler, van der Gaag, Meneghelli, Nordentoft, Marshall, Morrison, Raballo, Klosterkötter and Ruhrmann2015), although exceptions are common in both research (van der Gaag et al., Reference van der Gaag, Smit, Bechdolf, French, Linszen, Yung, McGorry and Cuijpers2013) and clinical practice (Nieman et al., Reference Nieman, Rike, Becker, Dingemans, van Amelsvoort, de Haan, van der Gaag, Denys and Linszen2009).

The decline effect

As mentioned, the empirical situation pertaining to high-risk research has changed in recent years, and some predictive associations that are at the heart of the early intervention paradigm have become weaker. This development is likely owing to a phenomenon called the ‘decline effect’ (Lehrer, Reference Lehrer2010) and is not unusual in science. Initial results are often stronger than follow-up findings, which replicate the effect to a much lesser extent if at all. We present four arguments for why psychosis high-risk centers (i.e. for ‘future patients’) should be relabeled and its treatment targets reconsidered.

Fear of psychosis may increase the likelihood of depression and promotes suicidality

Many prodromal clinics emphasize that their goal is to delay and perhaps even prevent psychosis (see quote at the beginning of the article) and name a number of unspecific symptoms (some more general, some attenuated positive symptoms) that indicate such a risk. However, most people with these symptoms will not develop psychosis. Although ‘risk calculators’ have been developed to increase predictive accuracy (Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, Yu, Addington, Bearden, Cadenhead, Cornblatt, Heinssen, Jeffries, Mathalon, McGlashan, Perkins, Seidman, Tsuang, Walker, Woods and Kattan2016; Fusar-Poli, Reference Fusar-Poli2017), these are based on retrospective group data and have not been readily validated for predictive purposes. Hence, some authors have suggested rethinking risk prediction based on dynamic modeling derived from moment-by-moment assessments (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, McGorry, Wichers, Wigman and Hartmann2017).

The prevalence of suicidal ideation, lifetime self-harm, and lifetime suicide attempts is high in people at putative risk for psychosis (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Hutton and Wood2015), and the risk of lifetime suicidality is elevated even in non-help-seeking subclinical individuals who experience psychotic-like experiences (Gawęda et al., Reference Gawęda, Pionke, Krężołek, Frydecka, Nelson and Cechnicki2019). Nicolas Rüsch and others (Corcoran et al., Reference Corcoran, First and Cornblatt2010; Rüsch et al., Reference Rüsch, Corrigan, Heekeren, Theodoridou, Dvorsky, Metzler, Müller, Walitza and Rössler2014) posed an important question that is implicit in this article: ‘Are labeling and stigma an acceptable price to pay for early intervention?’ (p. 487). According to an emerging trend in studies, the prospect of later psychosis induces fear, hopelessness, self-stigma, and demoralization as well as a feeling of being ‘damaged’ (Corcoran et al., Reference Corcoran, Malaspina and Hercher2005; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Anglin, Wonpat-Borja, Opler, Greenspoon and Corcoran2013). Stigma, stigma stress, and fear of deterioration are predictors of suicidality (Pompili et al., Reference Pompili, Amador, Girardi, Harkavy-Friedman, Harrow, Kaplan, Krausz, Lester, Meltzer, Modestin, Montross, Bo Mortensen, Munk-Jørgensen, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Saarinen, Zisook, Wilson and Tatarelli2007; Ventriglio et al., Reference Ventriglio, Gentile, Bonfitto, Stella, Mari, Steardo and Bellomo2016; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Mayer, Müller, Heekeren, Theodoridou, Dvorsky, Metzler, Oexle, Walitza, Rössler and Rüsch2016). According to Ventriglio and colleagues (Reference Ventriglio, Gentile, Bonfitto, Stella, Mari, Steardo and Bellomo2016), early insight may induce a change in an individual's self-image from that of a healthy person to an ill person, and this may be one reason why many clinicians do not inform their patients of the diagnosis of schizophrenia (Villani and Kovess-Masféty, Reference Villani and Kovess-Masféty2017), even if it is undisputed. There is early evidence that stigma may even increase the risk of transition to psychosis. In a prospective study of 171 young persons at risk for psychosis, Rüsch and colleagues (Reference Rüsch, Heekeren, Theodoridou, Müller, Corrigan, Mayer, Metzler, Dvorsky, Walitza and Rössler2015) showed that perceived harm due to stigma at baseline was associated with a higher risk of transition to psychosis after one year, even when controlling for baseline symptom severity and functioning. According to another recent study (Miegel et al., Reference Miegel, Jelinek and Moritz2019), ‘fear of becoming psychotic’ is prevalent in many patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; 32.1%) or depression (20.4%) and is significantly associated with suicidality at a medium effect size. This association is unlikely to disappear in the near future since the stigma of schizophrenia has increased rather than diminished over the past decades (Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Schwahn, Holzinger, Corrigan, Grabe, Carta and Angermeyer2012; Angermeyer et al., Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger and Schomerus2013).

To summarize, a strong emphasis on the (relatively low) possibility of schizophrenia (with the best of intentions) may unintentionally foster the development of a psychiatric disorder. This may be caused by an induction of rumination/worry, which represents a prominent transdiagnostic facilitator of prospective mental problems per se (Aldao and Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference Aldao and Nolen-Hoeksema2010).

Longer duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) is weakly correlated with poor outcome

As noted, evidence in favor of a connection between DUP and outcome in schizophrenia and an inverse association between DUP with a response to antipsychotic intervention seemed persuasive in early research but began to crumble after only a short time. As early as 2001, Ho and Andreasen (Reference Ho and Andreasen2001) cast doubt on the connection in light of evidence collected in 2000. Meta-analytic data now show that the DUP is significantly associated with outcome (sometimes with positive symptoms, sometimes with negative symptoms; Penttilä et al., Reference Penttilä, Jääskeläinen, Hirvonen, Isohanni and Miettunen2014), but the connection is weak to very weak (lower than r = 0.2 for all major parameters, thus explaining less than 4% of the variance). Importantly, we still do not know whether the DUP is a primary factor or an epiphenomenon. Whether the duration of the untreated prodrome (McGorry et al., Reference McGorry, Edwards, Mihalopoulos, Harrigan and Jackson1996), clearly the most relevant parameter, is associated with the outcome is even more elusive since only few studies have addressed this (Polari et al., Reference Polari, Lavoie, Yuen, Amminger, Berger, Chen, deHaan, Hartmann, Markulev, Melville, Nieman, Nordentoft, Riecher-Rössler, Smesny, Stratford, Verma, Yung, McGorry and Nelson2018; Rosengard et al., Reference Rosengard, Malla, Mustafa, Iyer, Joober, Bodnar, Lepage and Shah2019).

Problems with concurrent and predictive validity of risk factors

As highlighted by Jim van Os and others (van Os and Guloksuz, Reference van Os and Guloksuz2017; Guloksuz and van Os, Reference Guloksuz and van Os2018), the criteria for transition are often vague. This, in turn, burdens replication. Identification of at-risk individuals also seems to be inflated by recruitment strategies, known as risk enrichment; the pretest risk for psychosis at 38 months was 15% in help-seeking samples selected for clinical high risk (CHR) assessment compared to 0.1% in the general population (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Schultze-Lutter, Cappucciati, Rutigliano, Bonoldi, Stahl, Borgwardt, Riecher-Rössler, Addington, Perkins, Woods, McGlashan, Lee, Klosterkötter, Yung and McGuire2016b). Van Os also notes that transition rates reported in earlier studies (40%) have more than halved (15%) over the years (Guloksuz and van Os, Reference Guloksuz and van Os2018). This may have resulted from indiscriminate application of high-risk criteria to populations with low pretest risk, due to the publicity that the concept has received (Guloksuz and van Os, Reference Guloksuz and van Os2018). CHR is a weak predictor of later psychosis. A recent meta-analysis (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Andreou, Studerus, Heitz, Ittig, Leanza and Riecher-Rössler2019) shows that many individuals with CHR do not experience remission from the symptoms and display a clinical diagnosis at follow-up – mainly mood and anxiety disorders but not psychosis (see also Michel et al., Reference Michel, Ruhrmann, Schimmelmann, Klosterkötter and Schultze-Lutter2018) – and that approximately half show a poor psychosocial outcome (for compatible findings see Lin et al., Reference Lin, Wood, Nelson, Beavan, McGorry and Yung2015).

Further, assessment procedures aimed at predicting later psychosis are prone to severe biases that compromise their prognostic validity. For example, prodromal scales such as the 16-item Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ-16; Ising et al., Reference Ising, Veling, Loewy, Rietveld, Rietdijk, Dragt, Klaassen, Nieman, Wunderink, Linszen and van der Gaag2012) partially rely on items from schizotypal scales such as the Perceptual Aberration Scale and the Magical Ideation Scale. We have known for many years (Peltier, Reference Peltier1985) that such scales have a high (negative) correlation with the tendency to respond in a socially desirable way. In addition, some PQ-16 items, such as ‘I often hear unusual sounds like banging, clicking, hissing, clapping or ringing in my ears,’ are ambiguous in content and may be endorsed by someone who has tinnitus (the item is presumably targeted at hallucinations, but it is not clear). Even if understood correctly, items on sensory irritations are highly problematic as 50–75% of patients with depression (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Hörmann, Schröder, Berger, Jacob, Meyer, Holmes, Späth, Hautzinger, Lutz, Rose and Klein2014b) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Claussen, Hauschildt and Kellner2014a, Reference Moritz, Purdon, Jelinek, Chiang and Hauschildt2018; Röhlinger et al., Reference Röhlinger, Wulf, Fieker and Moritz2015), who usually do not develop schizophrenia, ‘hear’ or ‘see’ their intrusive thoughts from time to time or display other psychotic-like experiences (Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Connor, Clarke, Devlin, Harley and Cannon2012; Hodgekins et al., Reference Hodgekins, Lower, Wilson, Cole, Ugochukwu, Maxwell and Fowler2018).

Schizotypal as well as prodromal scales often tap visual hallucinations (e.g. ‘I have seen things that other people apparently can’t see’ from the PQ-16). The same applies to body symptoms [e.g. ‘I sometimes have had the feeling that my body is abnormal’ (Perceptual Aberration Scale) or ‘I feel that parts of my body have changed in some way, or that parts of my body are working differently than before’ (PQ-16)], although these are common in other disorders too and are regarded as less specific than auditory phenomena in the schizophrenia spectrum (Dudley et al., Reference Dudley, Aynsworth, Mosimann, Taylor, Smailes, Collerton, McCarthy-Jones and Urwyler2019). These items aim to capture bodily delusions but may be responded to positively by individuals who complain about ‘pins and needles’ and neurological symptoms such as polyneuropathy. Endorsement of schizotypal and other psychotic-like experiences are not specific to schizophrenia; patients with psychiatric disorders other than schizophrenia sometimes achieve elevated values or even similar scores on such scales as people with schizophrenia (Scherbarth-Roschmann and Hautzinger, Reference Scherbarth-Roschmann and Hautzinger1991; Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Andresen and Sengutta2019). Similarly, psychotic-like experiences, as measured with the Peters Delusions Inventory, are common in individuals with depression and anxiety (Varghese et al., Reference Varghese, Scott, Welham, Bor, Najman, O'Callaghan, Williams and McGrath2011).

Cut-offs need to be adjusted for culture, country, age, and also education level. Students often display scores as high as those of patients with schizophrenia on scales tapping schizotypy/psychosis-like experiences (Schutte and Malouff, Reference Schutte and Malouff1995). With respect to language and culture, it has been shown that scores on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) are higher in the U.S. population (Raine, Reference Raine1991) than in Britain or Germany (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Andresen and Jahn1997) and that individuals in all of these countries, in turn, score much higher than individuals in China (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Hsiao and Lin1997), Italy (Daneluzzo et al., Reference Daneluzzo, Bustini, Stratta, Casacchia and Rossi1998), and the Caribbean (Barron et al., Reference Barron, Swami, Towell, Hutchinson and Morgan2015). Such cultural differences clearly raise questions about the usefulness of global algorithms (Chung et al., Reference Chung, Kang, Im, Kim, Cho, Lee and Kwon2013).

Most assessment procedures do not readily take into account the compelling evidence that depression and aggravation/overreporting (e.g. in the hope of faster and more intensive treatment) may lead to a considerable inflation of false-positive allocations. This is not a new finding (Schutte and Malouff, Reference Schutte and Malouff1995).

We regard it as a great step forward that assessments in this area are increasingly incorporating interviews. The aforementioned problems do, however, also apply to interview scales such as the Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental State (CAARMS; Yung et al., Reference Yung, Yuen, McGorry, Phillips, Kelly, Dell'Olio, Francey, Cosgrave, Killackey, Stanford, Godfrey and Buckby2005), albeit perhaps to a lesser extent. However, a recent meta-analysis (Oliver et al., Reference Oliver, Kotlicka-Antczak, Minichino, Spada, McGuire and Fusar-Poli2018) concludes that the prognostic accuracy of the CAARMS is acceptable but much lower than previously reported and that its specificity is poor.

We also appreciate that the advocates of the basic symptom concept of Huber and Süllwold (Gross and Huber, Reference Gross and Huber1985; Süllwold, Reference Süllwold1991) recommend that cognitive basic symptoms, the most predictive basic symptoms for subsequent schizophrenia, should be assessed with expert ratings in view of the diagnostic problems faced by self-report scales such as the Frankfurt Complaint Questionnaire (for a discussion see Schultze-Lutter et al., Reference Schultze-Lutter, Addington, Ruhrmann and Klosterkötter2007). And, indeed, expert rating scales for basic symptoms seem to have some predictive value (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schultze-Lutter, Schimmelmann, Maric, Salokangas, Riecher-Rössler, van der Gaag, Meneghelli, Nordentoft, Marshall, Morrison, Raballo, Klosterkötter and Ruhrmann2015). Still, this cannot circumvent the problem that the assessment of cognitive deficits such as the inability to divide attention, which is an item from the Schizophrenia Proneness Inventory for Adults (Schultze-Lutter et al., Reference Schultze-Lutter, Addington, Ruhrmann and Klosterkötter2007), is not verified with objective tests but relies on what the individual discloses, and there is clear evidence that subjective cognitive complaints are poorly related to objective neurocognition but highly correlated with depression (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Ferahli and Naber2004). Moreover, metacognitive problems are common in patients with schizophrenia as well as in those at risk, which also compromises the validity of such self-assessments (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Balzan, Bohn, Veckenstedt, Kolbeck, Bierbrodt and Dietrichkeit2016). In a recent study (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Andresen and Sengutta2019), we found a medium correlation between the endorsement of schizotypal symptoms and items from an infrequency scale (i.e. endorsement/presence of essentially impossible phenomena such as writing with both hands equally well and equally fast), challenging the validity of symptom self-reports. Other biases may reflect the phenomenon that some patients do not disclose psychotic symptoms until after the interviewer has gained their trust. This can lead to the observation of a paradoxical worsening over time in patients who in fact have improved; more insight and less suspiciousness might enable them to acknowledge symptoms they were afraid to disclose earlier, did not recall during the initial interview, or did not deem pathological at baseline, resulting in pseudo-deterioration over time.

Lack of conclusive evidence that early intervention prevents transition to psychosis

A Cochrane meta-analysis indicates that we cannot reliably prevent transition to psychosis (Marshall and Rathbone, Reference Marshall and Rathbone2011), neither with psychotherapy nor with antipsychotic medication that – even when atypical antipsychotics are prescribed – may cause long-term (and partially irreversible) damage such as tardive dyskinesia or metabolic syndrome. This conflicts with more favorable meta-analyses (van der Gaag et al., Reference van der Gaag, Smit, Bechdolf, French, Linszen, Yung, McGorry and Cuijpers2013; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schultze-Lutter, Schimmelmann, Maric, Salokangas, Riecher-Rössler, van der Gaag, Meneghelli, Nordentoft, Marshall, Morrison, Raballo, Klosterkötter and Ruhrmann2015) suggesting that specialized treatment led to a transition risk reduction by 54% at 12 months and 37% at 24- to 48-month follow-ups [for a critical evaluation see Amos (Reference Amos2014) and Preti et al. (Reference Preti, Cella and Raballo2014)]. A more recent network analysis failed to find any advantages of specialized treatments over need-based treatment for prodromal patients (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Cipriani, Ioannidis, Radua, Stahl, Provenzani, McGuire and Fusar-Poli2018), while another recent analysis suggests that there is a ‘slight trend’ that cognitive-behavioral therapy can reduce attenuated positive symptoms at long-term follow-up (Devoe et al., Reference Devoe, Farris, Townes and Addington2019). Research in this area should continue; perhaps one day treatment will be found that can reliably delay or prevent later psychosis for the vast majority of individuals. However, for the time being, it seems to us that treatment confined to the individual's current problems (i.e. need-based intervention) is sufficient (Conrad et al., Reference Conrad, Lewin, Sly, Schall, Halpin, Hunter and Carr2017; Albert et al., Reference Albert, Tomassi, Maina and Tosato2018), in which case diagnostic labels should be avoided. This also applies to the use of antipsychotic medication, which, according to European guidelines for treatment of such patients (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schultze-Lutter, Schimmelmann, Maric, Salokangas, Riecher-Rössler, van der Gaag, Meneghelli, Nordentoft, Marshall, Morrison, Raballo, Klosterkötter and Ruhrmann2015), should only be given in exceptional circumstances for acute symptoms and not for those that are only anticipated.

Early detection centers should be renamed and their treatment targets reconsidered

We would like to offer some recommendations. Individuals suffering from psychological problems should be offered need-based treatment. Although some of their impairments, symptoms, or biases may indeed precede later psychosis, a large subgroup will remain happy (McCreery, Reference McCreery1993) or benign schizotypes (Jackson, Reference Jackson and Claridge1997), and either the abnormalities will subside on their own (developmental transitional syndrome in adolescence) or will develop other nonpsychotic disorders (van Os et al., Reference van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam2009; Armando et al., Reference Armando, Nelson, Yung, Ross, Birchwood, Girardi and Nastro2010; Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Devlin, Wigman, Kehoe, Murtagh, Fitzpatrick and Cannon2014; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Wood, Nelson, Beavan, McGorry and Yung2015; Nam et al., Reference Nam, Hilimire, Schiffman and DeVylder2016; Hodgekins et al., Reference Hodgekins, Lower, Wilson, Cole, Ugochukwu, Maxwell and Fowler2018; Beck et al., Reference Beck, Andreou, Studerus, Heitz, Ittig, Leanza and Riecher-Rössler2019). At the same time, therapists must do everything possible to reduce the impression that the possibility of psychosis is looming over the individual. Contemplating the diagnosis of psychosis may prompt many clinicians to prescribe antipsychotics (Yung, Reference Yung2010), whose adverse effects on the young brain are unknown (Liu and Demjaha, Reference Liu and Demjaha2013). While current predictors explain some variance, the present data do not permit definite conclusions about individual cases; in addition, we still have no treatment that can justify hope in so-called prodromal individuals. As discussed, the sword of Damocles of the possibility of later psychosis is frightening for many, and this can lead to secondary symptoms that trigger or (ironically) perhaps even cause what early detection centers seek to avert. Anticipatory suicides need to be prevented (e.g. the suicide of a person with certain schizotypal symptoms who has seen the suffering of a biological relative with the full-blown disorder). Therefore, therapists should target the immediate problems causing distress in their patients, which even in the manifest cases tend to be depression and low self-esteem rather than the core positive symptoms (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Berna, Jaeger, Westermann and Nagel2017).

Steps in this direction have already been made. A good example of this new trend are facilities such as headspace (Australia) and soulspace (Germany), which are facilities for young individuals in crisis, including those with at-risk symptoms (Bassilios et al., Reference Bassilios, Telford, Rickwood, Spittal and Pirkis2017; McGorry et al., Reference McGorry, Trethowan and Rickwood2019). To avoid stigma, these facilities are separate from institutions for individuals with established psychiatric disorders. While monitoring the individuals for signs of more severe stages of psychopathology, the connection between certain symptoms with subsequent schizophrenia is de-emphasized. Instead of promulgating a categorical view of mental illness, which induces the fear of eventually falling into this undesired category, a continuum view of mental health and mental illness offers a better framework for preventive services and thus avoids stigmatization (Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Angermeyer, Baumeister, Stolzenburg, Link and Phelan2016) but still offers help for manifest problems. Such services should offer staged care ranging from low-threshold self-help and online intervention for less severe cases and face-to-face intervention, which may also include pharmacotherapy, for those with more distressing symptoms. These facilities should use hope-oriented and stigma-free labels; in view of the multitude of outcomes of adolescent (attenuated) positive symptoms, cataclysmic terms such as early detection, prodrome/al and risk should be avoided. At this time, such developments are in their infancy, and many prodromal clinics treat individuals at alleged risk of psychosis as if they are patients with an established psychiatric diagnosis.

To conclude, we should not catastrophize an individual's future that can be neither reliably predicted nor ameliorated.

Acknowledgements

We are indebted to Dr Christina Andreou, Mary Senguatta, and Dr Andreas Bechdolf for their constructive and critical comments on an earlier version of this article.

Financial support

None.

Conflict of interest

None.

Footnotes

1 We are not the first to criticize such facilities. For articles with a somewhat different focus, see, for example, Ajnakina et al. (Reference Ajnakina, David and Murray2019) and Conrad et al. (Reference Conrad, Lewin, Sly, Schall, Halpin, Hunter and Carr2017). Our article repeats a number of arguments made in prior critical reviews (e.g. van Os and Guloksuz Reference van Os and Guloksuz2017), but we focus on the detrimental effects of diagnosis stigma and the multiple methodological problems (e.g. low content and predictive validity and proneness to response biases) of the instruments designed to assess ultra high risk.

References

Ajnakina, O, David, AS and Murray, RM (2019) ‘At risk mental state’ clinics for psychosis – an idea whose time has come – and gone! Psychological Medicine 49, 529534.Google Scholar
Albert, U, Tomassi, S, Maina, G and Tosato, S (2018) Prevalence of non-psychotic disorders in ultra-high risk individuals and transition to psychosis: a systematic review. Psychiatry Research 270, 112.Google Scholar
Aldao, A and Nolen-Hoeksema, S (2010) Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48, 974983.Google Scholar
Amos, AJ (2014) Evidence that treatment prevents transition to psychosis in ultra-high-risk patients remains questionable. Schizophrenia Research 153, 240.Google Scholar
Andreou, C, Bailey, B and Borgwardt, S (2019) Assessment and treatment of individuals at high risk for psychosis. BJPsych Advances 25, 177184.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, MC, Matschinger, H and Schomerus, G (2013) Attitudes towards psychiatric treatment and people with mental illness: changes over two decades. British Journal of Psychiatry 203, 146151.Google Scholar
Armando, M, Nelson, B, Yung, AR, Ross, M, Birchwood, M, Girardi, P and Nastro, PF (2010) Psychotic-like experiences and correlation with distress and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents and young adults. Schizophrenia Research 119, 258265.Google Scholar
Barron, D, Swami, V, Towell, T, Hutchinson, G and Morgan, KD (2015) Examination of the factor structure of the schizotypal personality questionnaire among British and Trinidadian adults. BioMed Research International 2015, 17.Google Scholar
Bassilios, B, Telford, N, Rickwood, D, Spittal, MJ and Pirkis, J (2017) Complementary primary mental health programs for young people in Australia: Access to Allied Psychological Services (ATAPS) and headspace. International Journal of Mental Health Systems 11, 19.Google Scholar
Beck, K, Andreou, C, Studerus, E, Heitz, U, Ittig, S, Leanza, L and Riecher-Rössler, A (2019) Clinical and functional long-term outcome of patients at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis without transition to psychosis: a systematic review. Schizophrenia Research.Google Scholar
Birchwood, M and MacMillan, F (1993) Early intervention in schizophrenia. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 27, 374378.Google Scholar
Cannon, TD, Yu, C, Addington, J, Bearden, CE, Cadenhead, KS, Cornblatt, BA, Heinssen, R, Jeffries, CD, Mathalon, DH, McGlashan, TH, Perkins, DO, Seidman, LJ, Tsuang, MT, Walker, EF, Woods, SW and Kattan, MW (2016) An individualized risk calculator for research in prodromal psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry 173, 980988.Google Scholar
Chen, WJ, Hsiao, CK and Lin, CCH (1997) Schizotypy in community samples: the three-factor structure and correlation with sustained attention. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106, 649654.Google Scholar
Chung, Y-C, Kang, N-I, Im, Y-J, Kim, S-W, Cho, IH, Lee, YM and Kwon, JS (2013) Validation of the Korean version of the Eppendorf Schizophrenia Inventory as a screening measure to detect adolescents at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 7, 7179.Google Scholar
Conrad, AM, Lewin, TJ, Sly, KA, Schall, U, Halpin, SA, Hunter, M and Carr, VJ (2017) Utility of risk-status for predicting psychosis and related outcomes: evaluation of a 10-year cohort of presenters to a specialised early psychosis community mental health service. Psychiatry Research 247, 336344.Google Scholar
Corcoran, C, Malaspina, D and Hercher, L (2005) Prodromal interventions for schizophrenia vulnerability: the risks of being ‘at risk’. Schizophrenia Research 73, 173184.Google Scholar
Corcoran, CM, First, MB and Cornblatt, B (2010) The psychosis risk syndrome and its proposed inclusion in the DSM-V: a risk–benefit analysis. Schizophrenia Research 120, 1622.Google Scholar
Daneluzzo, E, Bustini, M, Stratta, P, Casacchia, M and Rossi, A (1998) Schizotypal personality questionnaire and Wisconsin card sorting test in a population of DSM-III-R schizophrenic patients and control subjects. Comprehensive Psychiatry 39, 143148.Google Scholar
Davies, C, Cipriani, A, Ioannidis, JPA, Radua, J, Stahl, D, Provenzani, U, McGuire, P and Fusar-Poli, P (2018) Lack of evidence to favor specific preventive interventions in psychosis: a network meta-analysis. World Psychiatry 17, 196209.Google Scholar
Devoe, DJ, Farris, MS, Townes, P and Addington, J (2019) Attenuated psychotic symptom interventions in youth at risk of psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 13, 317.Google Scholar
Dudley, R, Aynsworth, C, Mosimann, U, Taylor, J-P, Smailes, D, Collerton, D, McCarthy-Jones, S and Urwyler, P (2019) A comparison of visual hallucinations across disorders. Psychiatry Research 272, 8692.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P (2017) Why ultra high risk criteria for psychosis prediction do not work well outside clinical samples and what to do about it. World Psychiatry 16, 212213.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Cappucciati, M, Rutigliano, G, Schultze-Lutter, F, Bonoldi, I, Borgwardt, S, Riecher-Rössler, A, Addington, J, Perkins, D, Woods, SW, McGlashan, TH, Lee, J, Klosterkötter, J, Yung, AR and McGuire, P (2015) At risk or not at risk? A meta-analysis of the prognostic accuracy of psychometric interviews for psychosis prediction. World Psychiatry 14, 322332.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Cappucciati, M, Borgwardt, S, Woods, SW, Addington, J, Nelson, B, Nieman, DH, Stahl, DR, Rutigliano, G, Riecher-Rössler, A, Simon, AE, Mizuno, M, Lee, TY, Kwon, JS, Lam, MML, Perez, J, Keri, S, Amminger, P, Metzler, S, Kawohl, W, Rössler, W, Lee, J, Labad, J, Ziermans, T, An, SK, Liu, C-C, Woodberry, KA, Braham, A, Corcoran, C, McGorry, P, Yung, AR and McGuire, PK (2016 a) Heterogeneity of psychosis risk within individuals at clinical high risk: a meta-analytical stratification. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 113120.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Schultze-Lutter, F, Cappucciati, M, Rutigliano, G, Bonoldi, I, Stahl, D, Borgwardt, S, Riecher-Rössler, A, Addington, J, Perkins, DO, Woods, SW, McGlashan, T, Lee, J, Klosterkötter, J, Yung, AR and McGuire, P (2016 b) The dark side of the moon: meta-analytical impact of recruitment strategies on risk enrichment in the clinical high risk state for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 732743.Google Scholar
Gawęda, Ł, Pionke, R, Krężołek, M, Frydecka, D, Nelson, B and Cechnicki, A (2019) The interplay between childhood trauma, cognitive biases, psychotic-like experiences and depression and their additive impact on predicting lifetime suicidal behavior in young adults. Psychological Medicine.Google Scholar
Gross, G and Huber, G (1985) Psychopathology of basic stages of schizophrenia in view of formal thought disturbances. Psychopathology 18, 115125.Google Scholar
Guloksuz, S and van Os, J (2018) Need for evidence-based early intervention programmes: a public health perspective. Evidence-Based Mental Health 21, 128130.Google Scholar
Ho, B-C and Andreasen, NC (2001) Long delays in seeking treatment for schizophrenia. Lancet 357, 898900.Google Scholar
Hodgekins, J, Lower, R, Wilson, J, Cole, H, Ugochukwu, U, Maxwell, S and Fowler, D (2018) Clinician-rated and self-reported psychotic-like experiences in individuals accessing a specialist Youth Mental Health Service. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 57, 367381.Google Scholar
Ising, HK, Veling, W, Loewy, RL, Rietveld, MW, Rietdijk, J, Dragt, S, Klaassen, RMC, Nieman, DH, Wunderink, L, Linszen, DH and van der Gaag, M (2012) The validity of the 16-item version of the Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ-16) to screen for ultra high risk of developing psychosis in the general help-seeking population. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 12881296.Google Scholar
Jackson, M (1997) Benign schizotypy? The case of spiritual experience. In Claridge, G (ed.), Implications for Illness and Health. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 171201.Google Scholar
Jaspers, K (1963) General Psychopathology (1923/1946). Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Kelleher, I, Connor, D, Clarke, MC, Devlin, N, Harley, M and Cannon, M (2012) Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychological Medicine 42, 18571863.Google Scholar
Kelleher, I, Devlin, N, Wigman, JTW, Kehoe, A, Murtagh, A, Fitzpatrick, C and Cannon, M (2014) Psychotic experiences in a mental health clinic sample: implications for suicidality, multimorbidity and functioning. Psychological Medicine 44, 16151624.Google Scholar
Klein, C, Andresen, B and Jahn, T (1997) Erfassung der schizotypen Persönlichkeit nach DSM-III-R: Psychometrische Eigenschaften einer autorisierten deutschsprachigen Übersetzung des ‘Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire’ (SPQ) von Raine. [Psychometric assessment of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire]. Diagnostica 43, 347369.Google Scholar
Klosterkötter, J, Schultze-Lutter, F, Bechdolf, A and Ruhrmann, S (2011) Prediction and prevention of schizophrenia: what has been achieved and where to go next? World Psychiatry 10, 165174.Google Scholar
Lehrer, J (2010) The truth wears off. Is there something wrong with the scientific method? The New Yorker 2010, 5257.Google Scholar
Lin, A, Wood, SJ, Nelson, B, Beavan, A, McGorry, P and Yung, AR (2015) Outcomes of nontransitioned cases in a sample at ultra-high risk for psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 249258.Google Scholar
Liu, C-C and Demjaha, A (2013) Antipsychotic interventions in prodromal psychosis. CNS Drugs 27, 197205.Google Scholar
Malla, A, Iyer, S, McGorry, P, Cannon, M, Coughlan, H, Singh, S, Jones, P and Joober, R (2016) From early intervention in psychosis to youth mental health reform: a review of the evolution and transformation of mental health services for young people. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 51, 319326.Google Scholar
Marshall, M and Rathbone, J (2011) Early intervention for psychosis. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, CD004718.Google Scholar
Marshall, M, Lewis, S, Lockwood, A, Drake, R, Jones, P and Croudace, T (2005) Association between duration of untreated psychosis and outcome in cohorts of first-episode patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 975983.Google Scholar
McCreery, C (1993) Schizotypy and Out-of-the-Body-Experiences (dissertation). University of Oxford, Oxford.Google Scholar
McGorry, PD, Edwards, J, Mihalopoulos, C, Harrigan, SM and Jackson, HJ (1996) EPPIC: an evolving system of early detection and optimal management. Schizophrenia Bulletin 22, 305326.Google Scholar
McGorry, P, Trethowan, J and Rickwood, D (2019) Creating headspace for integrated youth mental health care. World Psychiatry 18, 140141.Google Scholar
Michel, C, Ruhrmann, S, Schimmelmann, BG, Klosterkötter, J and Schultze-Lutter, F (2018) Course of clinical high-risk states for psychosis beyond conversion. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 268, 3948.Google Scholar
Miegel, F, Jelinek, L and Moritz, S (2019) Dysfunctional beliefs in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and depression as assessed with the Beliefs Questionnaire (BQ). Psychiatry Research 272, 265274.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Ferahli, S and Naber, D (2004) Memory and attention performance in psychiatric patients: lack of correspondence between clinician-rated and patient-rated functioning with neuropsychological test results. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 10, 623633.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Claussen, M, Hauschildt, M and Kellner, M (2014 a) Perceptual properties of obsessive thoughts are associated with low insight in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 202, 562565.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Hörmann, CC, Schröder, J, Berger, T, Jacob, GA, Meyer, B, Holmes, EA, Späth, C, Hautzinger, M, Lutz, W, Rose, M and Klein, JP (2014 b) Beyond words: sensory properties of depressive thoughts. Cognition and Emotion 28, 10471056.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Balzan, RP, Bohn, F, Veckenstedt, R, Kolbeck, K, Bierbrodt, J and Dietrichkeit, M (2016) Subjective versus objective cognition: evidence for poor metacognitive monitoring in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 178, 7479.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Berna, F, Jaeger, S, Westermann, S and Nagel, M (2017) The customer is always right? Subjective target symptoms and treatment preferences in patients with psychosis. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 267, 335339.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Purdon, C, Jelinek, L, Chiang, B and Hauschildt, M (2018) If it is absurd, then why do you do it? The richer the obsessional experience, the more compelling the compulsion. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 25, 210216.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Andresen, B and Sengutta, M (2019) The specificity of schizotypal scales and some implications for clinical high-risk research. Personality and Individual Differences.Google Scholar
Nam, B, Hilimire, M, Schiffman, J and DeVylder, J (2016) Psychotic experiences in the context of depression: the cumulative role of victimization. Journal of Psychiatric Research 82, 136140.Google Scholar
Nelson, B, McGorry, PD, Wichers, M, Wigman, JTW and Hartmann, JA (2017) Moving from static to dynamic models of the onset of mental disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 74, 528534.Google Scholar
Nieman, DH, Rike, WH, Becker, HE, Dingemans, PM, van Amelsvoort, TA, de Haan, L, van der Gaag, M, Denys, DAJP and Linszen, DH (2009) Prescription of antipsychotic medication to patients at ultra high risk of developing psychosis. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 24, 223228.Google Scholar
Oliver, D, Kotlicka-Antczak, M, Minichino, A, Spada, G, McGuire, P and Fusar-Poli, P (2018) Meta-analytical prognostic accuracy of the Comprehensive Assessment of at Risk Mental States (CAARMS): the need for refined prediction. European Psychiatry 49, 6268.Google Scholar
Peltier, BD (1985) An Investigation of Response Bias in the Chapman Scales. Reno: University of Nevada.Google Scholar
Penttilä, M, Jääskeläinen, E, Hirvonen, N, Isohanni, M and Miettunen, J (2014) Duration of untreated psychosis as predictor of long-term outcome in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 205, 8894.Google Scholar
Perkins, DO, Gu, H, Boteva, K and Lieberman, JA (2005) Relationship between duration of untreated psychosis and outcome in first-episode schizophrenia: a critical review and meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 17851804.Google Scholar
Polari, A, Lavoie, S, Yuen, H-P, Amminger, P, Berger, G, Chen, E, deHaan, L, Hartmann, J, Markulev, C, Melville, F, Nieman, D, Nordentoft, M, Riecher-Rössler, A, Smesny, S, Stratford, J, Verma, S, Yung, A, McGorry, P and Nelson, B (2018) Clinical trajectories in the ultra-high risk for psychosis population. Schizophrenia Research 197, 550556.Google Scholar
Pompili, M, Amador, XF, Girardi, P, Harkavy-Friedman, J, Harrow, M, Kaplan, K, Krausz, M, Lester, D, Meltzer, HY, Modestin, J, Montross, LP, Bo Mortensen, P, Munk-Jørgensen, P, Nielsen, J, Nordentoft, M, Saarinen, PI, Zisook, S, Wilson, ST and Tatarelli, R (2007) Suicide risk in schizophrenia: learning from the past to change the future. Annals of General Psychiatry 6, 10.Google Scholar
Preti, A, Cella, M and Raballo, A (2014) Preventing or masking psychosis? Possible unintended consequences of the ultra high-risk strategy. Schizophrenia Research 153, 241242.Google Scholar
Raine, A (1991) The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin 17, 555564.Google Scholar
Röhlinger, J, Wulf, F, Fieker, M and Moritz, S (2015) Sensory properties of obsessive thoughts in OCD and the relationship to psychopathology. Psychiatry Research 230, 592596.Google Scholar
Rosengard, RJ, Malla, A, Mustafa, S, Iyer, SN, Joober, R, Bodnar, M, Lepage, M and Shah, JL (2019) Association of pre-onset subthreshold psychotic symptoms with longitudinal outcomes during treatment of a first episode of psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry 76, 6170.Google Scholar
Rüsch, N, Corrigan, PW, Heekeren, K, Theodoridou, A, Dvorsky, D, Metzler, S, Müller, M, Walitza, S and Rössler, W (2014) Well-being among persons at risk of psychosis: the role of self-labeling, shame, and stigma stress. Psychiatric Services 65, 483489.Google Scholar
Rüsch, N, Heekeren, K, Theodoridou, A, Müller, M, Corrigan, PW, Mayer, B, Metzler, S, Dvorsky, D, Walitza, S and Rössler, W (2015) Stigma as a stressor and transition to schizophrenia after one year among young people at risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 166, 4348.Google Scholar
Scherbarth-Roschmann, P and Hautzinger, M (1991) Zur Psychometrischen Erfassung von Schizotypie. Methodische Überprüfung und erste Validierung von zwei Skalen zur Erfassung von Risikomerkmalen [Psychometric detection of schizotypy: evaluation and validation of two German scales for risk detection]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie 20, 238250.Google Scholar
Schmidt, SJ, Schultze-Lutter, F, Schimmelmann, BG, Maric, NP, Salokangas, RKR, Riecher-Rössler, A, van der Gaag, M, Meneghelli, A, Nordentoft, M, Marshall, M, Morrison, A, Raballo, A, Klosterkötter, J and Ruhrmann, S (2015) EPA guidance on the early intervention in clinical high risk states of psychoses. European Psychiatry 30, 388404.Google Scholar
Schomerus, G, Schwahn, C, Holzinger, A, Corrigan, PW, Grabe, HJ, Carta, MG and Angermeyer, MC (2012) Evolution of public attitudes about mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 125, 440452.Google Scholar
Schomerus, G, Angermeyer, MC, Baumeister, SE, Stolzenburg, S, Link, BG and Phelan, JC (2016) An online intervention using information on the mental health-mental illness continuum to reduce stigma. European Psychiatry 32, 2127.Google Scholar
Schultze-Lutter, F, Addington, J, Ruhrmann, S and Klosterkötter, J (2007) Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Adult Version (SPI-A). Rom: Giovanni Fioriti Editore.Google Scholar
Schutte, NS and Malouff, JM (1995) Sourcebook of Adult Assessment Strategies. New York, NY: Springer Science and Business Media.Google Scholar
Süllwold, L (1991) Manual zum Frankfurter Beschwerde-Fragebogen (FBF). [Frankfurt Complaint Questionnaire (FCQ) Manual]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Taylor, PJ, Hutton, P and Wood, L (2015) Are people at risk of psychosis also at risk of suicide and self-harm? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 45, 911926.Google Scholar
van der Gaag, M, Smit, F, Bechdolf, A, French, P, Linszen, DH, Yung, AR, McGorry, P and Cuijpers, P (2013) Preventing a first episode of psychosis: meta-analysis of randomized controlled prevention trials of 12 month and longer-term follow-ups. Schizophrenia Research 149, 5662.Google Scholar
van Os, J and Guloksuz, S (2017) A critique of the ‘ultra-high risk’ and ‘transition’ paradigm. World Psychiatry 16, 200206.Google Scholar
van Os, J, Linscott, RJ, Myin-Germeys, I, Delespaul, P and Krabbendam, L (2009) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine 39, 179195.Google Scholar
Varghese, D, Scott, J, Welham, J, Bor, W, Najman, J, O'Callaghan, M, Williams, G and McGrath, J (2011) Psychotic-like experiences in major depression and anxiety disorders: a population-based survey in young adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 389393.Google Scholar
Ventriglio, A, Gentile, A, Bonfitto, I, Stella, E, Mari, M, Steardo, L and Bellomo, A (2016) Suicide in the early stage of schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry 7, 116.Google Scholar
Villani, M and Kovess-Masféty, V (2017) Qu'en est-il de l'annonce du diagnostic de schizophrénie aujourd'hui en France? [Are schizophrenic patients being told their diagnosis today in France?]. L'Encéphale 43, 160169.Google Scholar
Xu, Z, Mayer, B, Müller, M, Heekeren, K, Theodoridou, A, Dvorsky, D, Metzler, S, Oexle, N, Walitza, S, Rössler, W and Rüsch, N (2016) Stigma and suicidal ideation among young people at risk of psychosis after one year. Psychiatry Research 243, 219224.Google Scholar
Yang, LH, Anglin, DM, Wonpat-Borja, AJ, Opler, MG, Greenspoon, M and Corcoran, CM (2013) Public stigma associated with psychosis risk syndrome in a college population: implications for peer intervention. Psychiatric Services 64, 284288.Google Scholar
Yung, AR (2010) Antipsychotic treatment of UHR (‘prodromal’) individuals. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 4, 197199.Google Scholar
Yung, AR, Yuen, HP, McGorry, PD, Phillips, LJ, Kelly, D, Dell'Olio, M, Francey, SM, Cosgrave, EM, Killackey, E, Stanford, C, Godfrey, K and Buckby, J (2005) Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of at-risk mental states. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 39, 964971.Google Scholar