Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:26:17.843Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The benefit of directly comparing autism and schizophrenia, revisited

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2019

Amy E. Pinkham*
Affiliation:
School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas At Dallas, Richardson, TX, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical School, Dallas, TX, USA
Noah J. Sasson
Affiliation:
School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas At Dallas, Richardson, TX, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Amy E. Pinkham, E-mail: amy.pinkham@utdallas.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

In his comment on our article, ‘Comprehensive comparison of social cognitive performance in autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia’ (Pinkham et al., Reference Pinkham, Morrison, Penn, Harvey, Kelsven, Ludwig and Sasson2019), Crespi argues that reporting data indicating behavioral similarities between disorders may be of limited utility because they provide little actionable information about diagnosis, causes, or treatments. Although we agree with his primary point that behavioral overlap can emerge from distinct mechanistic origins, we disagree that similarities in social cognitive performance in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and schizophrenia (SCZ) are without nosological or clinical value, particularly when revealed through well-controlled, large samples using psychometrically validated measures. We believe that behavioral convergence between disorders is important to acknowledge – and contrary to his claim that they obscure mechanistic and clinical distinctions – we think that they can provide a strong and necessary starting point for more targeted studies aimed at uncovering and understanding mechanisms and informing effective treatments. In the response below, we highlight areas of agreement with Crespi but also offer several counterpoints to each of the considerations he uses to support his argument.

First, Crespi comments that similar behavioral outcomes can result from similar, partially overlapping, or even opposing (Crespi & Badcock, Reference Crespi and Badcock2008), biological causes and that the data we report may inadvertently imply mechanistic similarities. While we fully agree that basic performance-based social cognitive comparisons between ASD and SCZ cannot address mechanism, we disagree that behavioral similarities necessarily imply mechanistic ones. Indeed, we explicitly caution against such an interpretation in the paper's discussion and have previously written extensively, both in empirical (Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Pinkham, Penn, Kelsven, Ludwig and Sasson2017; Sasson et al., Reference Sasson, Tsuchiya, Hurley, Couture, Penn, Adolphs and Piven2007; Sasson, Pinkham, Weittenhiller, Faso, & Simpson, Reference Sasson, Pinkham, Weittenhiller, Faso and Simpson2016) and review (Sasson, Pinkham, Carpenter, & Belger, Reference Sasson, Pinkham, Carpenter and Belger2011) papers, about the possibility of different mechanisms within ASD and SCZ producing similar behavioral outcomes. Additionally, although Crespi cites several studies suggesting divergent neurobiological mechanisms of social cognitive ability within ASD and SCZ, we are aware of several other studies, uncited in his letter, that suggest shared mechanisms (e.g. Chen et al., Reference Chen, Uddin, Duan, Zheng, Long, Zhang and Chen2017; Ciaramidaro et al., Reference Ciaramidaro, Bölte, Schlitt, Hainz, Poustka, Weber and Walter2018; Pinkham, Hopfinger, Pelphrey, Piven, & Penn, Reference Pinkham, Hopfinger, Pelphrey, Piven and Penn2008) and several others that report both areas of neurobiological overlap and discrepancy (e.g. Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Yu, Fung, Leung, Wong, Li and McAlonan2010; Sugranyes, Kyriakopoulos, Corrigall, Taylor, & Frangou, Reference Sugranyes, Kyriakopoulos, Corrigall, Taylor and Frangou2011). Such mixed findings may emerge because of sample and methodological differences, or more likely, because some combination of shared and divergent mechanisms underlies social cognitive impairment in ASD and SCZ. We believe that the evaluation of each of these possibilities first requires rigorous, well-powered comparisons that firmly establish behavioral patterns. We hope that our paper provides this, and can be used as a roadmap for investigating mechanisms and guiding treatment.

Crespi also argues that without information on mechanism, knowledge of quantitative levels of deficits is limited in its utility. In our view, this depends on the question one is attempting to answer. As we note in the article, previous findings regarding the degree of overlap in social cognitive performance between disorders have been mixed and hampered by differences between studies that introduce confounds and preclude definitive conclusions. Our goal was to add clarity to this literature by assessing a large, well-characterized, demographically, and intellectually comparable sample of ASD, SCZ, and typically-developing participants using a broad battery of psychometrically-validated social cognitive tasks spanning a range of subdomains (e.g. emotion processing and theory of mind). By doing so, we are able to provide strong evidence that social cognitive impairments are an integral part of both disorders that should be considered when assessing prognosis, support needs, and treatment. Further, unlike many previous comparative studies, our comparison was not conducted on a single social cognitive task but rather on a comprehensive battery, and is the first to administer tasks validated for use in both ASD (Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Pinkham, Kelsven, Ludwig, Penn and Sasson2019) and SCZ (Pinkham, Penn, Green, & Harvey, Reference Pinkham, Penn, Green and Harvey2015). Our findings therefore provide some of the strongest evidence yet that social cognitive impairment in both ASD and SCZ is not relegated to a specific area of social cognition (e.g. theory of mind) but is expansive and encompassing. Finally, as noted in the paper, we did predict specific patterns of social cognitive differences in ASD and SCZ that largely were not supported by our findings. Although we would never characterize our results as definitive, we do believe our study to be the most rigorous social cognitive comparison of ASD and SCZ to date and feel strongly that sharing these results, regardless of whether they align with a priori hypotheses, is important for the field to have and use to inform future investigations.

Second, Crespi notes that diagnosis-based approaches like ours are counter to RDoC principles, and that identification of shared mechanisms is better done through investigations of how ‘biological adaptations connect to psychological maladaptations.’ This issue is one concerning the value of different starting points (e.g. whether investigations should be bottom-up or top-down), and we believe both offer value and depend largely on one's research questions and aims. Although it is undoubtedly helpful to examine genes and brain function in ASD and SCZ to determine how these factors relate to social functioning, behavioral comparative studies can help identify candidate genetic and neural mechanisms and provide the necessary foundation for these examinations. For example, given that social cognition is impaired in ASD and SCZ, it makes sense to investigate the functioning of social cognitive neural networks, which many studies have done. However, without these basic behavioral studies, the search for neurobiological mechanisms becomes like the search for a needle in a haystack, where we run the risk of unfocused studies that are more likely to suffer Type I error. Thus, identifying areas of behavioral overlap can help cull where within the haystack to look. Further, starting with behavioral patterns also has an important practical benefit. It is much easier and economically viable to screen individuals for social cognitive impairments and to then assess associated brain function than it is to screen for abnormal temporal parietal junction or amygdala functioning, for example, and only then test for poor social cognitive performance. Additionally, starting at the behavioral level can help identify meaningful clinical subgroups, such as SCZ patients with and without paranoia, that may help uncover distinguishing mechanisms [e.g. hypermentalizing in SCZ may be specific to those individuals who also experience paranoia (Ciaramidaro et al., Reference Ciaramidaro, Bölte, Schlitt, Hainz, Poustka, Weber and Walter2015; Frith & Corcoran, Reference Frith and Corcoran1996)]. In general, we believe that future studies that focus on accounting for heterogeneity within disorders (e.g. Pu et al., Reference Pu, Nakagome, Satake, Ohtachi, Itakura, Yamanashi and Iwata2019) will be particularly profitable for informing mechanistic understanding.

Finally, in his third point, Crespi states that behavioral tasks that do not show differences between disorders cannot aid in differential diagnosis and that measures will only be helpful to the degree that they indicate differences rather than quantify deficits. The point regarding differential diagnosis is valid; however, behavioral tasks, whether they show differences between disorders or not, are essential to identifying problem areas and developing individualized treatment protocols. At the beginning of his commentary, Crespi asks what is learned by noting that someone has deficits in social cognition. Quite simply, that person and his provider have learned that he has difficulty in an area that will likely benefit from treatment. Also, because we know that there is variability in the degree of social cognitive impairment, at least within SCZ (Hajdúk, Harvey, Penn, & Pinkham, Reference Hajdúk, Harvey, Penn and Pinkham2018), it is important to determine whether or not any individual patient is presenting with social cognitive difficulties. If the individual is functioning within the normative range, then social cognitive treatment is likely not a recommended component of treatment. Further, even in the event that treatment is warranted, we believe that one does not necessarily need to know the exact mechanism of impairment in order for treatment to provide benefit. For example, we still do not know precisely how risperidone and aripiprazole, the only FDA-approved medications for ASD, are beneficial in the treatment of both ASD and SCZ (King & Lord, Reference King and Lord2011); yet, the lack of mechanistic understanding does not undermine their effectiveness. Similarly, Cognitive Enhancement Therapy appears to improve neurocognitive and social cognitive functioning in both ASD and SCZ (Eack et al., Reference Eack, Hogarty, Greenwald, Litschge, Porton, Mazefsky and Minshew2018; Hogarty et al., Reference Hogarty, Flesher, Ulrich, Carter, Greenwald, Pogue-Geile and Garrett2004), despite our lack of knowledge regarding the origin of these impairments. Although mechanistic understanding undoubtedly has the potential to enhance treatment benefit, the lack of clarity about underlying mechanisms existing today should not discourage or preclude individuals with social cognitive impairment from seeking out currently-available evidence-based treatments.

Thus, while it could be argued that demonstrations of divergence are more informative than demonstrations of convergence, we maintain that both are important and worthwhile. The particular benefits of identifying points of behavioral convergence are the elucidation of candidate mechanisms and the ability to reasonably explore whether mechanisms or treatments investigated in one disorder may also apply to the other. In our view, prioritizing divergence above all else jeopardizes the ability to conduct meaningful investigations. The value of studies should not be based on the presence of group differences or statistically significant findings; rather, strong and valid design should be emphasized and data taken at face value. Direct comparisons may reveal either convergence or divergence, and it would be a disservice to the field for researchers to refrain from conducting or reporting a study simply because they find similarity instead of difference.

Financial support

This work was funded by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health at the National Institutes of Health (RO1 MH093432, PIs Harvey, Penn, Pinkham; R15 MH101595, PI Sasson).

Conflict of interest

Dr Pinkham has received consulting fees and travel reimbursement from Roche. Dr Sasson reports no conflicts of interest.

References

Chen, H., Uddin, L. Q., Duan, X., Zheng, J., Long, Z., Zhang, Y., … Chen, H. (2017). Shared atypical default mode and salience network functional connectivity between autism and schizophrenia. Autism Research, 10, 17761786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, C., Yu, K., Fung, G., Leung, M., Wong, C., Li, Q., … McAlonan, G. (2010). Autistic disorders and schizophrenia: Related or remote? An anatomical likelihood estimation. PLoS ONE, 5, e12233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciaramidaro, A., Bölte, S., Schlitt, S., Hainz, D., Poustka, F., Weber, B., … Walter, H. (2015). Schizophrenia and autism as contrasting minds: Neural evidence for the hypo-hyper-intentionality hypothesis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41, 171179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciaramidaro, A., Bölte, S., Schlitt, S., Hainz, D., Poustka, F., Weber, B., … Walter, H. (2018). Transdiagnostic deviant facial recognition for implicit negative emotion in autism and schizophrenia. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 264275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, B., & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 241261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eack, S. M., Hogarty, S. S., Greenwald, D. P., Litschge, M. Y., Porton, S. A., Mazefsky, C. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2018). Cognitive enhancement therapy for adult autism spectrum disorder: Results of an 18-month randomized clinical trial. Autism Research, 11, 519530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D., & Corcoran, R. (1996). Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 26, 521530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajdúk, M., Harvey, P. D., Penn, D. L., & Pinkham, A. E. (2018). Social cognitive impairments in individuals with schizophrenia vary in severity. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 104, 6571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogarty, G. E., Flesher, S., Ulrich, R., Carter, M., Greenwald, D., Pogue-Geile, M., … Garrett, A. (2004). Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: Effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 866876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, B. H., & Lord, C. (2011). Is schizophrenia on the autism spectrum? Brain Research, 1380, 3441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, K. E., Pinkham, A. E., Kelsven, S., Ludwig, K., Penn, D. L., & Sasson, N. J. (2019). Psychometric evaluation of social cognitive measures for adults with autism. Autism Research, 12, 766778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, K. E., Pinkham, A. E., Penn, D. L., Kelsven, S., Ludwig, K., & Sasson, N. J. (2017). Distinct profiles of social skill in adults with autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Autism Research, 10, 878887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinkham, A. E., Hopfinger, J. B., Pelphrey, K. A., Piven, J., & Penn, D. L. (2008). Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 99, 164175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinkham, A. E., Morrison, K. E., Penn, D. L., Harvey, P. D., Kelsven, S., Ludwig, K., & Sasson, N. J. (2019). Comprehensive comparison of social cognitive performance in autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinkham, A. E., Penn, D. L., Green, M. F., & Harvey, P. D. (2015). Social cognition psychometric evaluation: Results of the initial psychometric study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42, 494504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pu, S., Nakagome, K., Satake, T., Ohtachi, H., Itakura, M., Yamanashi, T., … Iwata, M. (2019). Comparison of prefrontal hemodynamic responses and cognitive deficits between adult patients with autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 206, 420427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasson, N., Tsuchiya, N., Hurley, R., Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., Adolphs, R., & Piven, J. (2007). Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 25802588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasson, N. J., Pinkham, A. E., Carpenter, K. L., & Belger, A. (2011). The benefit of directly comparing autism and schizophrenia for revealing mechanisms of social cognitive impairment. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 3, 87100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasson, N. J., Pinkham, A. E., Weittenhiller, L. P., Faso, D. J., & Simpson, C. (2016). Context effects on facial affect recognition in schizophrenia and autism: Behavioral and eye-tracking evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42, 675683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugranyes, G., Kyriakopoulos, M., Corrigall, R., Taylor, E., & Frangou, S. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia: Meta-analysis of the neural correlates of social cognition. PLoS ONE, 6, e25322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed