Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-30T16:57:31.534Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between complement component 4A expression, cognitive performance and brain imaging measures in UK Biobank

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 March 2021

Kevin S. O'Connell*
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Ida E. Sønderby
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway Department of Medical Genetics, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Oleksandr Frei
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Dennis van der Meer
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway School of Mental Health and Neuroscience, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Lavinia Athanasiu
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway Department of Medical Genetics, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Olav B. Smeland
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Dag Alnæs
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Tobias Kaufmann
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Lars T. Westlye
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Vidar M. Steen
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Department of Clinical Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway Department of Medical Genetics, Dr Einar Martens' Research Group for Biological Psychiatry, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
Ole A. Andreassen
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Timothy Hughes
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, & Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway Department of Medical Genetics, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Srdjan Djurovic
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Genetics, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Department of Clinical Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
*
Author for correspondence: Kevin S. O'Connell, E-mail: kevin.oconnell@medisin.uio.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Abstract

Background

Altered expression of the complement component C4A gene is a known risk factor for schizophrenia. Further, predicted brain C4A expression has also been associated with memory function highlighting that altered C4A expression in the brain may be relevant for cognitive and behavioral traits.

Methods

We obtained genetic information and performance measures on seven cognitive tasks for up to 329 773 individuals from the UK Biobank, as well as brain imaging data for a subset of 33 003 participants. Direct genotypes for variants (n = 3213) within the major histocompatibility complex region were used to impute C4 structural variation, from which predicted expression of the C4A and C4B genes in human brain tissue were predicted. We investigated if predicted brain C4A or C4B expression were associated with cognitive performance and brain imaging measures using linear regression analyses.

Results

We identified significant negative associations between predicted C4A expression and performance on select cognitive tests, and significant associations with MRI-based cortical thickness and surface area in select regions. Finally, we observed significant inconsistent partial mediation of the effects of predicted C4A expression on cognitive performance, by specific brain structure measures.

Conclusions

These results demonstrate that the C4 risk locus is associated with the central endophenotypes of cognitive performance and brain morphology, even when considered independently of other genetic risk factors and in individuals without mental or neurological disorders.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The major histocompatibility complex (MHC) is located on chromosome 6 and is implicated in a number of autoimmune diseases (Howson, Walker, Clayton, & Todd, Reference Howson, Walker, Clayton and Todd2009; Kamitaki et al., Reference Kamitaki, Sekar, Handsaker, de Rivera, Tooley, Morris and McCarroll2020; Raychaudhuri et al., Reference Raychaudhuri, Sandor, Stahl, Freudenberg, Lee, Jia and de Bakker2012). In addition, genetic variants within this region are consistently associated with risk of schizophrenia (International Schizophrenia Consortium et al., Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sklar2009; Pardiñas et al., Reference Pardiñas, Holmans, Pocklington, Escott-Price, Ripke, Carrera and Walters2018; Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium, 2011; Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014; Shi et al., Reference Shi, Levinson, Duan, Sanders, Zheng, Pe'er and Gejman2009; Stefansson et al., Reference Stefansson, Ophoff, Steinberg, Andreassen, Cichon, Rujescu and Collier2009). These associations corroborate serological studies which identified altered levels of inflammatory markers in schizophrenia patients, including complement proteins (Hakobyan, Boyajyan, & Sim, Reference Hakobyan, Boyajyan and Sim2005; Laskaris et al., Reference Laskaris, Zalesky, Weickert, Di Biase, Chana, Baune and Cropley2019; Maes et al., Reference Maes, Delange, Ranjan, Meltzer, Desnyder, Cooremans and Scharpé1997; Mayilyan, Arnold, Presanis, Soghoyan, & Sim, Reference Mayilyan, Arnold, Presanis, Soghoyan and Sim2006; Mayilyan, Dodds, Boyajyan, Soghoyan, & Sim, Reference Mayilyan, Dodds, Boyajyan, Soghoyan and Sim2008a; Mayilyan, Weinberger, & Sim, Reference Mayilyan, Weinberger and Sim2008b). These findings suggest the involvement of an immune component in psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia.

In order to better understand the mechanisms underlying the MHC genetic association with schizophrenia, a fine-mapping molecular investigation of the region was conducted and identified that variants within the complement component 4 (C4) gene locus are responsible for at least part of the association signal (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). The C4 protein is one of a number of proteins that make up the complement system (Charles, Janeway, Travers, Walport, & Shlomchik, Reference Charles, Janeway, Travers, Walport, Shlomchik and Gibbs2001), part of the innate immune system. Complement components were initially shown to modulate neurogenesis in murine primary cortical cell cultures (van Beek et al., Reference van Beek, Nicole, Ali, Ischenko, MacKenzie, Buisson and Fontaine2001). Further investigation of the role of complement components in the central nervous system of genetically modified mice identified its major role in modulating synaptic plasticity (Hong et al., Reference Hong, Beja-Glasser, Nfonoyim, Frouin, Li, Ramakrishnan and Stevens2016; Stephan, Barres, & Stevens, Reference Stephan, Barres and Stevens2012; Stokowska et al., Reference Stokowska, Atkins, Morán, Pekny, Bulmer, Pascoe and Pekna2017; Vasek et al., Reference Vasek, Garber, Dorsey, Durrant, Bollman, Soung and Klein2016). More recently, complement components were implicated in neuronal migration (Gorelik et al., Reference Gorelik, Sapir, Haffner-Krausz, Olender, Woodruff and Reiner2017) and apoptosis (Niculescu et al., Reference Niculescu, Weerth, Niculescu, Cudrici, Rus, Raine and Rus2004) in the central nervous system. Additional evidence for the activity of the complement system in the brain, and its involvement in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia is summarized in recent reviews (Druart & Le Magueresse, Reference Druart and Le Magueresse2019; Nimgaonkar, Prasad, Chowdari, Severance, & Yolken, Reference Nimgaonkar, Prasad, Chowdari, Severance and Yolken2017; Tenner, Stevens, & Woodruff, Reference Tenner, Stevens and Woodruff2018; Woo, Pouget, Zai, & Kennedy, Reference Woo, Pouget, Zai and Kennedy2019).

The C4 gene is present as one of two isotypes (C4A and C4B) and the structural variation between these isotypes, as well as their copy number, was shown to significantly alter the expression level of C4 in post-mortem brain tissue (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). A model of this relationship can be used to predict C4A gene expression in the brain based on an individual's genotype. Using this procedure, predicted C4A gene expression was associated with risk of schizophrenia in an independent sample (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). Finally, C4 proteins localized to the synapses in post-mortem human brains, and C4 was also demonstrated to modulate synaptic pruning in mice (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016), and human-derived neural cultures (Sellgren et al., Reference Sellgren, Sheridan, Gracias, Xuan, Fu and Perlis2017, Reference Sellgren, Gracias, Watmuff, Biag, Thanos, Whittredge and Perlis2019).

Independent of these findings, variants within the MHC region were also associated with cognitive performance (Athanasiu et al., Reference Athanasiu, Giddaluru, Fernandes, Christoforou, Reinvang, Lundervold and Le Hellard2017; Donohoe et al., Reference Donohoe, Walters, Hargreaves, Rose, Morris, Fahey and Corvin2013; Zhang, Lv, Fan, Tang, & Yi, Reference Zhang, Lv, Fan, Tang and Yi2017) and brain structure (Walters et al., Reference Walters, Rujescu, Franke, Giegling, Vásquez, Hargreaves and Owen2013) in patients with schizophrenia. Based on these studies, Donohoe et al. (Reference Donohoe, Holland, Mothersill, McCarthy-Jones, Cosgrove, Harold and Morris2018) showed that increased predicted C4A expression was associated with poorer performance in memory recall measures in a cohort of psychosis patients and healthy controls, as well as in patients only. The direction of effect in control participants was similar to that observed in patients, however, the effect size was smaller and non-significant. In addition, they demonstrated that higher predicted C4A expression was associated with lower cortical activity in the middle temporal cortex during visual processing in healthy participants (Donohoe et al., Reference Donohoe, Holland, Mothersill, McCarthy-Jones, Cosgrove, Harold and Morris2018). In support of these findings, complement-dependent synapse elimination was recently identified as a mechanism for memory loss (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yue, Hu, Shen, Ma, Li and Gu2020). These results highlight that C4A expression in the brain may be associated with cognitive and behavioral traits not only in patients with psychiatric disorders but also in healthy individuals.

Based on this, our primary aim was to investigate if predicted brain C4A expression is associated with cognitive performance in a large adult population-based sample (UK Biobank), without mental or neurological disorders. We hypothesized that higher predicted C4A expression would be associated with lower cognitive performance, however, we did not start with any a priori assumptions regarding the specific cognitive tasks investigated. Our secondary aims were to investigate if predicted brain C4A expression is associated with differences in brain structure and if observed effects on cognitive performance may be mediated by C4A-associated differences in brain structure.

Methods

The UK Biobank cohort

The UK Biobank cohort and available data are described elsewhere (Bycroft et al., Reference Bycroft, Freeman, Petkova, Band, Elliott, Sharp and Marchini2018). Briefly, the UK Biobank project is a prospective cohort study with genetic and phenotypic data collected on approximately 500 000 individuals from across the UK. Multimodal imaging assessments are underway, with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain currently available for a subset of individuals (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Alfaro-Almagro, Bangerter, Thomas, Yacoub, Xu and Smith2016). All data used in this study were obtained from the UK Biobank (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk) through application 27412.

We limited the cohort to 409 629 Caucasian individuals (Datafield-22006). This subset is defined as those individuals who self-identified as ‘White British’ and that had similar genetic ancestry based on a principal component analysis (online Supplementary Fig. S1). Individuals with a diagnosed mental or neurological disorder were excluded (Datafields-41202,41204; F/G codes). One from each pair of individuals with a kinship coefficient above 0.053 was also removed prior to analyses (Datafield-2201122012).

The final cohort sample size, after exclusions, with available genetic data was 329 773 (median age 59, range: 40–74). The sample included 152 966 men (median age 59, range: 40–74) and 176 807 women (median age 58, range: 40–71).

All participants provided informed consent prior to enrolment. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Genotyping and quality control

Genotyping of the UK Biobank cohort was performed on two similar arrays. Approximately 50 000 samples were genotyped on the UK BiLEVE array and the remaining 450 000 samples were genotyped on the UK Biobank Axiom array. Further details regarding genotyping and quality control procedures for the UK Biobank are well documented (Bycroft et al., Reference Bycroft, Freeman, Petkova, Band, Elliott, Sharp and Marchini2018).

Imputation of C4 structural variation and genetically predicted C4a expression

Direct genotypes for variants (n = 3213) within the MHC region were used to impute C4 structural variation. This analysis was performed using the 222 haplotype-integrated variant and C4 reference panel (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). The distribution of C4 structural variants was similar to previously described (online Supplementary Table S1) (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016; Kamitaki et al., Reference Kamitaki, Sekar, Handsaker, de Rivera, Tooley, Morris and McCarroll2020). The imputed C4 structural alleles were then used to determine C4 isotype (C4A, C4B, C4L, and C4S) copy numbers. Here C4A and C4B refer to the two isotypes of the C4 gene, while C4L and C4S refer to ‘long’ and ‘short’ forms of the gene due to the presence or absence of a human endogenous retroviral (HERV) insertion, respectively. We calculated values for the predicted expression of the C4A gene in human brain tissue, based on the previously identified relationship between C4 isotype copy number and C4A gene expression (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). The predicted C4A expression values ranged between 0 and 2.35 (mean = 1.08, standard deviation = 0.36) (online Supplementary Fig. S2). A summary of this methodology is presented in Fig. 1. Predicted C4B expression values were calculated following a similar approach. Predicted C4A and C4B expression values were used for association with cognitive tasks and brain imaging measures since these variables allow for use of standard linear regression analyses instead of ordinal regression using structural variants.

Fig. 1. A schematic diagram of the methodology used to obtain predicted expression values for the C4A gene within brain tissue, as described by Sekar et al. (Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). First, (a) individual genotypes are determined and SNP haplotypes are then inferred from this data. (b) The SNP haplotypes can be grouped into haplogroups and each haplogroup corresponds to a specific C4 locus structure. Four of these structures are common (represented here) and 11 are less common (<10% frequency combined). HERV, human endogenous retroviral insertion. (c) Structures with higher copy numbers of C4A and C4L (both C4AL and C4BL) isotypes show higher C4A expression in brain tissue. (d) C4A gene expression can be predicted based on the data outlined in panels A–C. AL, AS, BL, and BS refer to the copy number of each of these isotypes in the C4 locus structure. Structures containing the AS combination are omitted from panels A to C since they are rare, with a frequency of approximately 1% (online Supplementary Table S1) (Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). This figure is a schematic and was not generated from actual genotype, expression or other data.

Cognitive tasks

We obtained performance measures on seven cognitive tasks from the UK Biobank, and processed them as previously described (Kendall et al., Reference Kendall, Rees, Escott-Price, Einon, Thomas, Hewitt and Kirov2017, Reference Kendall, Bracher-Smith, Fitzpatrick, Lynham, Rees, Escott-Price and Kirov2019). Briefly, measures for analysis included the Pairs Matching task (episodic memory, Datafield-399, outcome: total number of errors), the Reaction Time task (simple processing speed, Datafield-20023, outcome: mean reaction time to correct responses), the Fluid Intelligence test (reasoning and problem solving, Datafield-20016, total number of correct answers), the Digit Span task (numeric working memory, Datafield-4282, outcome: maximum number of digits remembered), the Symbol Digit Substitution task (complex processing speed, Datafield-20195, outcome: number of correct substitutions), and the Trail Making A and B tasks (visual attention, Datafields-20156,20157, outcome: time taken to complete these tests). All data were recoded so that higher scores indicate better performance. The number of participants that completed each of these performance measures, with available predicted C4A and C4B expression values and brain imaging data, is provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Numbers of participants that completed each of the seven cognitive tasks, with available predicted C4A and C4B expression values and brain imaging data

Image acquisition and processing

Imaging assessments were conducted at three centers, using the same hardware, software and protocols. A detailed description of the processes for data acquisition, processing and quality control is available (Alfaro-Almagro et al., Reference Alfaro-Almagro, Jenkinson, Bangerter, Andersson, Griffanti, Douaud and Smith2018). The data release from UK Biobank used in this study included 33 003 participants. C4A and C4B expression values were predicted for 27 087 of these participants.

We processed T1-weighted MRI scans from all individuals using the standardized recon-all pipeline of FreeSurfer (Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove and Dale2002; Fischl, Reference Fischl2012). Furthermore, for each scanner site, we regressed age and sex from the Euler number of both left and right hemispheres and individuals whose Euler numbers were less than 3 standard deviations below the residualized Euler numbers were excluded as outliers (n = 618) (Kaufmann et al., Reference Kaufmann, van der Meer, Doan, Schwarz, Lund, Agartz and Westlye2019). Analyzed brain imaging measures included surface area and mean thickness of 34 cortical regions, total cortical surface area, and mean cortical thickness, the volumes of seven sub-cortical regions, and total intracranial volume (ICV). The total surface area, thickness or volume of each region was calculated by summing the right and left hemispheres.

Statistical analyses

To determine the relationship between cognitive performance and predicted C4A and C4B expression, we performed linear regression analyses with each cognitive task as the outcome variable, predicted C4A or C4B expression as the predictor variable and common covariates, which included age, age-squared, sex, genotyping batch, the first 10 genetic principal components and educational attainment. A summary of the effects of these covariates on C4A and C4B expression is provided in online Supplementary Table S2. Age-squared was included since this allows the model to accommodate a non-linear relationship between age and the outcome variable if one exists. Educational attainment was determined by the highest qualification obtained by each individual at the time of assessment (Datafield-6138). No significant associations were identified between predicted C4B expression and cognitive tasks (online Supplementary Table S3), and therefore predicted C4B expression was not tested for associations with brain imaging measures.

To investigate the relationship between brain imaging measures and predicted C4A expression values, brain imaging measures were first normalized in R 3.5.0 by an inverse normal transformation of the residual of linear regression on the phenotype correcting for covariates, as previously described (Sønderby et al., Reference Sønderby, Gústafsson, Doan, Hibar, Martin-Brevet and Abdellaoui2018). This transformation results in normally distributed covariate-corrected values that were used for downstream analysis. Covariates included the common covariates mentioned above as well as Euler number (Rosen et al., Reference Rosen, Roalf, Ruparel, Blake, Seelaus, Villa and Satterthwaite2018). Regional measures of surface area and mean thickness were corrected for total cortical surface area and total mean cortical thickness, respectively. Subcortical volumes were corrected for ICV.

To determine the association between of predicted C4A expression and brain structure, we performed linear regression analyses with the covariate-corrected brain imaging measure as the outcome and predicted C4A expression as the predictor variable in the model.

Finally, to determine if the effects of predicted C4A expression on cognitive tasks were mediated by brain imaging measures, additional linear regression analyses were performed with each cognitive task as the outcome variable, predicted C4A expression, a regional non-covariate-corrected brain imaging measure and covariates. Covariates included the common covariates, Euler number (Rosen et al., Reference Rosen, Roalf, Ruparel, Blake, Seelaus, Villa and Satterthwaite2018), and educational attainment. Regional measures were corrected for using global measures as described above. Mediation analysis was then performed using the R package mediation v4.4.6, using the bootstrapping method and 5000 simulations per test (Writing Committee for the ENIGMA-CNV Working Group et al., Reference van der Meer, Sønderby, Kaufmann, Walters, Abdellaoui and Andreassen2019). All significant results are also shown in the context of a mediation model (Fig. 2). A previous study investigating the effects of brain imaging measures on cognitive performance in the UK Biobank has shown significant positive correlations between all of the brain imaging measures included in this study and increased cognitive performance (Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob, & Deary, Reference Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob and Deary2019). Those results correspond to path b in the mediation analyses performed in this study (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. A summary of the results from the significant (FDR <0.05) linear regression models of predicted C4A expression values on cognitive performance and brain imaging measures. The results are presented in the context of a mediation model. (i) Higher predicted C4A expression was significantly associated with the results from three cognitive tasks. Path c = Cognitive task ~ C4A expression (ii) Predicted C4A expression was significantly associated with some measures of cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Path a = Brain imaging measure ~ C4A expression. (iii) A summary of the brain imaging measures identified to significantly mediate the effect of predicted C4A expression on cognitive performance. Path ab = Cognitive task ~ C4A expression mediated by brain imaging measures. The proportion of the total effect (Panel i, Path c) mediated by changes in the corresponding brain imaging measure is shown (Prop. Med = ab/c). Negative proportion values indicate inconsistent mediation. Inconsistent mediation occurs when the direction of effect of the direct effect (c’) and the indirect effect (ab) is in the opposite direction. The standardized β (Std. β) is shown to indicate the size and direction of effect of higher C4A expression on each outcome measure. The green and red headers indicate an increase or decrease in each outcome measure, respectively.

Since sex-specific C4A risk effects were recently identified (Kamitaki et al., Reference Kamitaki, Sekar, Handsaker, de Rivera, Tooley, Morris and McCarroll2020), additional analysis was performed as above with the inclusion of an interaction term between C4A expression and sex (online Supplementary Table S4). The number of male and female participants that completed each of the performance measures, with available predicted C4A and C4B expression values and brain imaging data, is provided in online Supplementary Table S5.

The distributions of residuals from all models were examined and determined to be normal indicating that linearity assumptions were not violated. Effect sizes reported are the standardized estimates of beta (β) from the linear regressions. The partial correlation coefficient (r) was computed from the t-statistics for the main cognitive and brain structure analyses (online Supplementary Tables S6–S9). The distribution of values for significantly associated cognitive performance tests and brain imaging measures were plotted against ‘binned’ predictions of C4A expression levels (online Supplementary Figs S3–S5) and analysis of variance tests and post-hoc Tukey tests were used to determine the differences between these ‘bins’ (online Supplementary Tables S10–S12). Empirical p values were converted to False Discovery Rate (FDR) q-values using the R package qvalue v2.14.1. FDR was computed independently for the analyses of cognitive tests (n = 7), brain morphology (n = 79) and mediation (n = 33). Results were considered significant if FDR <0.05. Plots were generated using R library ggplot2 v2.2.1 (Wickham, Reference Wickham2009, p. 2) and the R package ggseg v1.5.1.

Results

Effect of C4a expression on cognitive performance

Predicted C4A expression was significantly (FDR < 0.05) associated with three of the seven cognitive tests (Fig. 2i, Table 2, online Supplementary Table S6). Specifically, higher predicted C4A expression was associated with reduced cognitive performance in the pairs matching (Std. β = −0.006, t-value = −3.28, FDR = 0.009), fluid intelligence (Std. β = −0.008, t-value = −2.86, FDR = 0.032), and symbol digit substitution (Std. β = −0.008, t-value = −2.75, FDR = 0.043) cognitive tasks. Analysis of the association between predicted C4A expression and cognitive performance measures indicates a linear relationship, not a distinct range of expression above or below which the observed changes occur (online Supplementary Table S10). No significant C4A–sex interactions were identified for any of the cognitive tests (online Supplementary Table S4).

Table 2. A summary of the results from the significant linear regression models of predicted C4A expression values on cognitive performance

All models also included age squared, educational attainment, genotyping batch, and the first 10 genetic principal components as covariates (data not shown). r = Partial correlation coefficient. Std. β = Standardized β. Std. Error = Standard Error.

Effect of C4a expression on brain imaging measures

Predicted C4A expression was significantly (FDR < 0.05) associated with three cortical surface area measures (Fig. 3a, online Supplementary Table S7). Specifically, higher C4A expression was associated with reduced surface area for the transverse temporal measure (Std. β = −0.016, t-value = −2.68, FDR = 0.045), and increased surface area of the insula (Std. β = 0.029, t-value = 4.70, FDR = 1.735 × 10−4), and middle temporal (Std. β = 0.025, t-value = 4.15, FDR = 7.458 × 10−4) measures, respectively (Fig. 2ii).

Fig. 3. The effect of C4A expression on regional measures of (a) cortical surface area and (b) mean cortical thickness. The colors correspond to the standardized β (Std. β) coefficient for each brain region from the linear regressions. Black demarcations around a brain region indicate that it passes the multiple comparisons–corrected significance threshold of FDR <0.05. a, Insula. b, Transverse temporal. c, Middle temporal. d, Cuneus. e, Pericalcarine. f, Posterior cingulate. g, Isthmuscingulate. h, Parahippocampal. i, Entorhinal. j, Medial orbitofrontal.

When considering mean cortical thickness, predicted C4A expression was significantly associated with eight measures, the majority (6 of 8) of which were negatively associated with C4A expression (Fig. 3b, online Supplementary Table S8). Specifically, the parahippocampal (Std. β = −0.026, t-value = −4.22, FDR = 7.458 × 10−4), insula (Std. β = −0.024, t-value = −3.96, FDR = 1.277 × 10−3), isthmuscingulate (Std. β = −0.021, t-value = −3.38, FDR = 9.865 × 10−3), entorhinal (Std. β = −0.020, t-value = −3.22, FDR = 0.014), medial orbitofrontal (Std. β = −0.019, t-value = −3.14, FDR = 0.016) and posterior cingulate (Std. β = −0.019, t-value = −3.08, FDR = 0.017) measures (Fig. 2ii).

No significant associations were identified between predicted C4A expression and subcortical volumes. In addition, no other regional brain measures, or global measures including total cortical surface area, total mean cortical thickness and ICV, were significantly associated with predicted C4A expression (online Supplementary Tables S7–S9). As with cognitive performance, further analysis of the association between predicted C4A expression and regional brain imaging measures indicates that this relationship is linear and that there is not a distinct range of expression above or below which the observed changes occur (online Supplementary Tables S11–S12 and online Supplementary Figs S3–S5). Hemisphere-specific results are provided in the supplement (online Supplementary Tables S13–S15). A summary of the effects of predicted C4A expression on brain imaging measures, and how these results are incorporated into the mediation analyses are shown in Fig. 2ii.

No significant C4A–sex interactions were identified for any brain imaging measures (online Supplementary Tables S16–S18).

Indirect effect of C4a expression on cognitive performance – mediation by brain imaging measures

Mediation analyses highlighted that increases in insula surface area and medial orbitofrontal thickness are linked to significant (FDR < 0.05) inconsistent mediation of the effect of higher predicted C4A expression on two measures of cognitive performance (Fig. 2iii), i.e. the changes in brain imaging measures partially suppress the negative effects of higher C4A expression on cognitive performance. None of the included brain imaging measures was identified as significant mediators of the effect of predicted C4A expression on fluid intelligence scores (online Supplementary Table S19).

Discussion

Here we identified novel significant associations between predicted brain C4A expression and cognitive performance in a large adult volunteer sample of individuals without mental or neurological disorders. Additionally, we showed that predicted C4A expression was significantly associated with regional cortical thickness and surface area. Further analysis of these associations revealed that their relationships are linear, and that there is no distinct threshold value for predicted C4A expression, highlighting that multiple factors likely influence cognition and brain morphology in these individuals within the normal range. Finally, we identified significant inconsistent partial mediation of the effects of C4A expression on cognitive performance, by specific brain imaging measures. This indicates that the differences observed in brain morphology may help to protect against C4A-associated cognitive deficits. In addition, our observations of lower cognitive performance and differences in brain imaging measures are highly unlikely to be secondary to any mental or neurological disorders or the treatment thereof since we excluded individuals with diagnosed mental or neurological disorders, and the remaining individuals within the UK Biobank tend to be healthier than the general population (Fry et al., Reference Fry, Littlejohns, Sudlow, Doherty, Adamska, Sprosen and Allen2017).

The main finding of this study is the negative association between predicted C4A expression in the brain and episodic memory (Pairs Matching task), reasoning and problem solving (Fluid Intelligence test) and complex processing speed (Symbol Digit Substitution task). Our regression modelling shows that the effects of predicted C4A expression, in some instances, are comparable in size to known modifiers of cognitive performance, such as with age for fluid intelligence and with sex for symbol digit substitution (Table 2). As expected, when comparing these effect sizes to those of rare copy number variants (CNVs) with known cognitive effects, a study on the same UK Biobank participants showed that most such CNVs had a greater effect on cognitive performance than that observed for predicted C4A expression in this study (Kendall et al., Reference Kendall, Bracher-Smith, Fitzpatrick, Lynham, Rees, Escott-Price and Kirov2019). These results are in line with previous findings, that higher predicted C4A expression is associated with poorer performance in memory recall measures in psychosis patients (Donohoe et al., Reference Donohoe, Holland, Mothersill, McCarthy-Jones, Cosgrove, Harold and Morris2018) and that the complement system modulates memory loss (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yue, Hu, Shen, Ma, Li and Gu2020), and further demonstrate that these effects are present within unaffected individuals. Predicted C4B expression was not associated with cognitive performance, the effect of the C4 locus was limited to C4A as suggested by previous findings (Donohoe et al., Reference Donohoe, Holland, Mothersill, McCarthy-Jones, Cosgrove, Harold and Morris2018; Sekar et al., Reference Sekar, Bialas, de Rivera, Davis, Hammond, Kamitaki and McCarroll2016). Moreover, we did not identify any strong correlation between schizophrenia polygenic risk score and predicted C4A expression (data not shown), implying that predicted C4A expression is not a proxy for schizophrenia polygenic risk in the UK Biobank sample analyzed.

Cognitive impairments reliably distinguish between schizophrenia patients and healthy controls, with large effect sizes in meta-analyses (Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone, & Seidman, Reference Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone and Seidman2009). Moreover, similar observations, with smaller effects, for measures of processing speed, attention and memory have also been identified when comparing first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients to healthy controls (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Xiang, Wang, Everall, Tang, Yang and Jia2016). At a molecular level, shared common variants contributing to both schizophrenia risk and cognitive performance have also been identified (Smeland et al., Reference Smeland, Bahrami, Frei, Shadrin, O'Connell, Savage and Andreassen2019). These studies highlight cognitive impairment as a core heritable feature of schizophrenia (Barch & Ceaser, Reference Barch and Ceaser2012; Bora, Yücel, & Pantelis, Reference Bora, Yücel and Pantelis2010), which may manifest in both affected patients and healthy individuals with some genetic burden for the disorder. Cognitive deficits have been associated with poorer functional outcomes regardless of age, sex or chronicity of the disorder (Fett et al., Reference Fett, Viechtbauer, Dominguez, Penn, van Os and Krabbendam2011). This lead to the suggestion that common mechanisms might modulate individual differences within these cognitive domains, e.g. related to the structure, function and/or connectivity of prefrontal, parietal, cingulate and insula brain regions (Barch & Ceaser, Reference Barch and Ceaser2012). Our brain imaging results highlight that C4A expression may potentially act as one of the causative factors in such mechanisms.

We identified significant associations between predicted C4A expression and cortical surface area and/or mean cortical thickness within temporal, cingulate and insula cortex, amongst others (Figs 1ii and 2). In line with previous observations of structural brain abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia (Cobia, Csernansky, & Wang, Reference Cobia, Csernansky and Wang2011; van Haren et al., Reference van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins and Kahn2011; Kubota et al., Reference Kubota, Miyata, Yoshida, Hirao, Fujiwara, Kawada and Murai2011; Assunção Leme et al., Reference Assunção Leme, Gadelha, Sato, Ota, de Mari, Melaragno and Jackowski2013; Moberget et al., Reference Moberget, Doan, Alnæs, Kaufmann, Córdova-Palomera, Lagerberg and Westlye2018; Alnæs et al., Reference Alnæs, Kaufmann, van der Meer, Córdova-Palomera, Rokicki and Moberget2019), and more recent associations between schizophrenia polygenic score and structure in unaffected individuals (Alnæs et al., Reference Alnæs, Kaufmann, van der Meer, Córdova-Palomera, Rokicki and Moberget2019; Neilson et al., Reference Neilson, Shen, Cox, Clarke, Wigmore, Gibson and Lawrie2019; Westlye, Alnæs, van der Meer, Kaufmann, & Andreassen, Reference Westlye, Alnæs, van der Meer, Kaufmann and Andreassen2019), higher predicted C4A expression was mostly associated with smaller cortical surface area and lower mean cortical thickness (7/11 brain imaging measures, Fig. 2ii). These results, together with our findings on cognitive performance, provide further evidence that some of the common genetic underpinnings of schizophrenia may have similar effects in individuals without mental disorders, in line with dimensional and polygenic risk models (Boyle, Li, & Pritchard, Reference Boyle, Li and Pritchard2017; Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Wray, Stone, Visscher, O'Donovan and Sklar2009; Timpson, Greenwood, Soranzo, Lawson, & Richards, Reference Timpson, Greenwood, Soranzo, Lawson and Richards2018).

In contrast to these results, higher predicted C4A expression was also associated with increased cortical surface area and mean cortical thickness in a subset of brain regions (4/11 brain imaging measures, Fig. 2ii). Among these regions with an increased cortical surface area are the insula and the middle temporal cortices. This is contrary to what is observed in schizophrenia patients where the cortical surface area of these regions is reduced (Assunção Leme et al., Reference Assunção Leme, Gadelha, Sato, Ota, de Mari, Melaragno and Jackowski2013; Cobia et al., Reference Cobia, Csernansky and Wang2011; Kubota et al., Reference Kubota, Miyata, Yoshida, Hirao, Fujiwara, Kawada and Murai2011; van Haren et al., Reference van Haren, Schnack, Cahn, van den Heuvel, Lepage, Collins and Kahn2011). Interestingly, however, a larger cortical surface area has previously been identified in unaffected relatives of schizophrenia patients when compared to non-relative controls (Goghari, Rehm, Carter, & MacDonald, Reference Goghari, Rehm, Carter and MacDonald2007). That study showed that relatives had increased gray matter volume and surface area in the left hemisphere, bilaterally in the parahippocampal gyri, and in the left middle temporal lobe, thereby implicating the cingulate and temporal regions which are known to be associated with higher level cognitive, affective, and memory functions (Goghari et al., Reference Goghari, Rehm, Carter and MacDonald2007). The authors suggested two possible explanations for these observed increases in the gray matter of relatives; (i) abnormal cell migration and deficient pruning, and (ii) a protective or compensatory factor against the development of psychosis or loss of associated functioning (Córdova-Palomera et al., Reference Córdova-Palomera, Kaufmann, Bettella, Wang, Doan, van der Meer and Westlye2018; Goghari et al., Reference Goghari, Rehm, Carter and MacDonald2007). Given the molecular functions of complement C4 in the brain, our results could support their suggestion of altered cell migration and synaptic pruning. Moreover, our mediation analyses also suggest the presence of compensatory factors against C4A-associated cognitive deficits in individuals without mental disorders.

Previous large scale studies investigating the differences in brain imaging measures between schizophrenia patients and healthy controls show prolific effects of the disorder on numerous measures of cortical surface area and thickness (van Erp et al., Reference van Erp, Walton, Hibar, Schmaal, Jiang, Glahn and Turner2018), as well as subcortical volumes (van Erp et al., Reference van Erp, Hibar, Rasmussen, Glahn, Pearlson, Andreassen and Turner2016). Although these effects are considered small to medium, they are much larger than the effects of C4A expression observed in the present study. Thus, although the changes in brain structure in schizophrenia may be influenced by the level of C4A expression, a large number of genetic and environmental factors likely contribute, as suggested by previous studies (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Baker, Holmes, Jahanshad, Ge, Jung and Smoller2016).

Brain imaging measures were previously shown to correlate positively with general cognitive performance in the UK Biobank (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob and Deary2019). Since we had identified a significant negative effect of C4A expression on cognitive task performance and significant effects on brain imaging measures (predominantly in the negative direction) (Fig. 2), we expected ex ante to observe consistent mediation via the indirect effect (Fig. 2iii, path ab), i.e. that some proportion of the effect of C4A expression on cognitive performance would be accounted for by the effect of C4A expression on brain imaging measures. All of our observations, however, were of inconsistent mediation, i.e. that changes in brain structure, directly or indirectly related to higher C4A expression, may act in a protective or compensatory manner against C4A-associated cognitive deficits. Significant C4A-associated increases in insula surface area were shown to partially mediate the effects of C4A expression on cognitive performance (Fig. 2iii). Specifically, increased insula surface area suppressed the negative effects of C4A expression on episodic memory (Pairs Matching task) and complex processing speed (Symbol Digit Substitution task) by approximately 15% (Fig. 2iii). Despite the significant correlation identified between C4A expression and insula surface area, these mediation results suggest that this relationship is driven by additional components other than C4A expression. Rather, the increase in insula surface area is the result of some undetermined mechanism in response to increased C4A expression. A similar compensatory relationship was identified between C4A expression, cognitive performance, and mean medial orbitofrontal cortical thickness (Fig. 2iii). Increased medial orbitofrontal cortical thickness suppressed the negative effects of C4A expression on episodic memory (Pairs Matching task) by approximately 9% (Fig. 2iii). In this instance, however, predicted C4A expression was negatively associated with mean medial orbitofrontal cortical thickness. Thus, the observed relationship between C4A expression and medial orbitofrontal cortical thickness is likely driven by increased C4A expression, and the observed protective effect is likely driven by another distinct mechanism in order to compensate for the effects of increased C4A expression.

Partial mediation of the effects of C4A expression on cognitive performance, by changes in brain imaging measures, suggests that additional mechanisms play a role in modulating this relationship. Furthermore, given the healthier bias of UK Biobank participants (Fry et al., Reference Fry, Littlejohns, Sudlow, Doherty, Adamska, Sprosen and Allen2017), further exaggerated by our removal of individuals with mental or neurological disorders, it is tempting to speculate that these participants may share other protective or compensatory factors, in addition to the brain imaging differences identified in this study, which might mask the true effect of C4A expression on cognitive performance. Thus, the true effect would likely be greater in an unbiased population cohort. Future studies should identify additional factors associated with changes in C4A expression and cognitive performance in order to determine other mechanisms that might contribute to their relationship.

A limitation to the current study is that the UK Biobank has an older age distribution in comparison to patients included in most schizophrenia studies, which are commonly conducted on individuals within an age range more closely matching the age of onset of the disorder (18–25 years). As a result, despite controlling for age in our analyses, we cannot exclude a potential effect of aging on the results. Studies in prospective cohorts are required to address this limitation. A second limitation is the reduced sample size for some of the cognitive tasks. Since the identified significant effects of C4A expression of cognitive tasks were small, and predominantly identified for those tasks with the largest sample sizes, these reduced numbers may have resulted in false negatives. Future studies with larger samples for these cognitive tasks are required to determine their true relationship with C4A expression. Finally, the significant effects of C4A expression on cognitive performance and brain morphology identified in this study are very small. By comparison, the effects of brain imaging measures on cognitive performance are magnitudes greater than the effects of C4A expression on cognitive performance (online Supplementary Table S20). This highlights that a large number of additional genetic and environmental factors contribute to these phenotypes.

In conclusion, we observed that higher predicted C4A expression is associated with lower cognitive performance and regional cortical surface area and thickness. Moreover, we provide evidence that the observed changes in cognitive performance, as a result of predicted C4A expression, may be mediated by C4A-associated changes in brain structure. These results demonstrate that C4 locus affects cognition and brain morphology in individuals without mental or neurological disorders.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000179.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge support from the Research Council of Norway (grant 273291, 248980, 248778 and 248828 for OAA; grant 249795 for LTW), and the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (grant 2018094 for SD; grant 2020060 for IES; grant 2017-112 for OAA; grant 2019101 for LTW), the European Research Council under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (ERC Starting Grant 802998: BRAINMINT for LTW). This research has been conducted using the UK Biobank Resource (Application number 27412). Dr Andreassen reports personal fees from Lundbeck outside the submitted work. The other authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Footnotes

*

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

References

Alfaro-Almagro, F., Jenkinson, M., Bangerter, N. K., Andersson, J. L. R., Griffanti, L., Douaud, G., … Smith, S. M. (2018). Image processing and quality control for the first 10000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank. NeuroImage, 166, 400424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alnæs, D., Kaufmann, T., van der Meer, D., Córdova-Palomera, A., Rokicki, J., & Moberget, T., … Karolinska Schizophrenia Project Consortium. (2019). Brain heterogeneity in schizophrenia and its association with polygenic risk. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(7), 739748. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assunção Leme, I. B., Gadelha, A., Sato, J. R., Ota, V. K., de Mari, J. J., Melaragno, M. I., … Jackowski, A. P. (2013). Is there an association between cortical thickness, age of onset, and duration of illness in schizophrenia? CNS Spectrums, 18(6), 315321. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852913000333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Athanasiu, L., Giddaluru, S., Fernandes, C., Christoforou, A., Reinvang, I., Lundervold, A. J., … Le Hellard, S. (2017). A genetic association study of CSMD1 and CSMD2 with cognitive function. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 61, 209216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2016.11.026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., & Ceaser, A. (2012). Cognition in schizophrenia: Core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 2734. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., Yücel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2010). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and affective psychoses: Implications for DSM-V criteria and beyond. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(1), 3642. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, E. A., Li, Y. I., & Pritchard, J. K. (2017). An expanded view of complex traits: From polygenic to omnigenic. Cell, 169(7), 11771186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bycroft, C., Freeman, C., Petkova, D., Band, G., Elliott, L. T., Sharp, K., … Marchini, J. (2018). The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature, 562(7726), 203209. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0579-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charles, A, Janeway, J., Travers, P., Walport, M., & Shlomchik, M. J. (2001). The complement system and innate immunity. In Gibbs, Sarah (Ed.), Immunobiology: The immune system in health and Disease. New York, USA: Garland Science. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK27100/.Google Scholar
Cobia, D. J., Csernansky, J. G., & Wang, L. (2011). Cortical thickness in neuropsychologically near-normal schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 133(1–3), 6876. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Córdova-Palomera, A., Kaufmann, T., Bettella, F., Wang, Y., Doan, N. T., van der Meer, D., … Westlye, L. T. (2018). Effects of autozygosity and schizophrenia polygenic risk on cognitive and brain developmental trajectories. European Journal of Human Genetics: EJHG, 26(7), 10491059. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41431-018-0134-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, S. R., Ritchie, S. J., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Deary, I. J. (2019). Structural brain imaging correlates of general intelligence in UK Biobank. Intelligence, 76, 101376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donohoe, G., Holland, J., Mothersill, D., McCarthy-Jones, S., Cosgrove, D., Harold, D., … Morris, D. W. (2018). Genetically predicted complement component 4A expression: Effects on memory function and middle temporal lobe activation. Psychological Medicine, 48(10), 16081615. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donohoe, G., Walters, J., Hargreaves, A., Rose, E. J., Morris, D. W., Fahey, C., … Corvin, A. (2013). Neuropsychological effects of the CSMD1 genome-wide associated schizophrenia risk variant rs10503253. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 12(2), 203209. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Druart, M., & Le Magueresse, C. (2019). Emerging roles of complement in psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 573. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A.-K. J., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M.-G., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B. (2012). Freesurfer. NeuroImage, 62(2), 774781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., … Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341355. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, A., Littlejohns, T. J., Sudlow, C., Doherty, N., Adamska, L., Sprosen, T., … Allen, N. E. (2017). Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. American Journal of Epidemiology, 186(9), 10261034. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwx246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goghari, V. M., Rehm, K., Carter, C. S., & MacDonald, A. W. (2007). Regionally specific cortical thinning and gray matter abnormalities in the healthy relatives of schizophrenia patients. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 17(2), 415424. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorelik, A., Sapir, T., Haffner-Krausz, R., Olender, T., Woodruff, T. M., & Reiner, O. (2017). Developmental activities of the complement pathway in migrating neurons. Nature Communications, 8, 15096. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakobyan, S., Boyajyan, A., & Sim, R. B. (2005). Classical pathway complement activity in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Letters, 374(1), 3537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.10.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, S., Beja-Glasser, V. F., Nfonoyim, B. M., Frouin, A., Li, S., Ramakrishnan, S., … Stevens, B. (2016). Complement and microglia mediate early synapse loss in Alzheimer mouse models. Science (New York, N.Y.), 352(6286), 712716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad8373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hou, C.-L., Xiang, Y.-T., Wang, Z.-L., Everall, I., Tang, Y., Yang, C., … Jia, F.-J. (2016). Cognitive functioning in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis, first-degree relatives of patients with psychosis and patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 174(1–3), 7176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.04.034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howson, J. M. M., Walker, N. M., Clayton, D., & Todd, J. A., & Type 1 Diabetes Genetics Consortium. (2009). Confirmation of HLA class II independent type 1 diabetes associations in the major histocompatibility complex including HLA-B and HLA-A. Diabetes, Obesity & Metabolism, 11(Suppl. 1), 3145. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1463-1326.2008.01001.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamitaki, N., Sekar, A., Handsaker, R. E., de Rivera, H., Tooley, K., Morris, D. L., … McCarroll, S. A. (2020). Complement genes contribute sex-biased vulnerability in diverse disorders. Nature, 582, 577581. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2277-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufmann, T., van der Meer, D., Doan, N. T., Schwarz, E., Lund, M. J., Agartz, I., … Westlye, L. T. (2019). Common brain disorders are associated with heritable patterns of apparent aging of the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 22(10), 16171623. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0471-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendall, K. M., Bracher-Smith, M., Fitzpatrick, H., Lynham, A., Rees, E., Escott-Price, V., … Kirov, G. (2019). Cognitive performance and functional outcomes of carriers of pathogenic copy number variants: Analysis of the UK Biobank. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 214(5), 297304. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2018.301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendall, K. M., Rees, E., Escott-Price, V., Einon, M., Thomas, R., Hewitt, J., … Kirov, G. (2017). Cognitive performance among carriers of pathogenic copy number variants: Analysis of 152,000 UK Biobank subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 82(2), 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubota, M., Miyata, J., Yoshida, H., Hirao, K., Fujiwara, H., Kawada, R., … Murai, T. (2011). Age-related cortical thinning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 125(1), 2129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.10.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laskaris, L., Zalesky, A., Weickert, C. S., Di Biase, M. A., Chana, G., Baune, B. T., … Cropley, V. (2019). Investigation of peripheral complement factors across stages of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 204, 3037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.11.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, P. H., Baker, J. T., Holmes, A. J., Jahanshad, N., Ge, T., Jung, J.-Y., … Smoller, J. W. (2016). Partitioning heritability analysis reveals a shared genetic basis of brain anatomy and schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(12), 16801689. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maes, M., Delange, J., Ranjan, R., Meltzer, H. Y., Desnyder, R., Cooremans, W., & Scharpé, S. (1997). Acute phase proteins in schizophrenia, mania and major depression: Modulation by psychotropic drugs. Psychiatry Research, 66(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-1781(96)02915-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayilyan, K. R., Arnold, J. N., Presanis, J. S., Soghoyan, A. F., & Sim, R. B. (2006). Increased complement classical and mannan-binding lectin pathway activities in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Letters, 404(3), 336341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayilyan, K. R., Dodds, A. W., Boyajyan, A. S., Soghoyan, A. F., & Sim, R. B. (2008a). Complement C4B protein in schizophrenia. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry: The Official Journal of the World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry, 9(3), 225230. https://doi.org/10.1080/15622970701227803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayilyan, K. R., Weinberger, D. R., & Sim, R. B. (2008b). The complement system in schizophrenia. Drug News & Perspectives, 21(4), 200210. https://doi.org/10.1358/dnp.2008.21.4.1213349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Giuliano, A. J., Goff, K. P., Faraone, S. V., & Seidman, L. J. (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 23(3), 315336. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, K. L., Alfaro-Almagro, F., Bangerter, N. K., Thomas, D. L., Yacoub, E., Xu, J., … Smith, S. M. (2016). Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nature Neuroscience, 19(11), 15231536. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moberget, T., Doan, N. T., Alnæs, D., Kaufmann, T., Córdova-Palomera, A., Lagerberg, T. V., … Westlye, L. T. (2018). Cerebellar volume and cerebellocerebral structural covariance in schizophrenia: A multisite mega-analysis of 983 patients and 1349 healthy controls. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(6), 15121520. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neilson, E., Shen, X., Cox, S. R., Clarke, T.-K., Wigmore, E. M., Gibson, J., … Lawrie, S. M. (2019). Impact of polygenic risk for schizophrenia on cortical structure in UK Biobank. Biological Psychiatry, 86(7), 536544. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.04.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niculescu, T., Weerth, S., Niculescu, F., Cudrici, C., Rus, V., Raine, C. S., … Rus, H. (2004). Effects of complement C5 on apoptosis in experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. Journal of Immunology (Baltimore, Md.: 1950), 172(9), 57025706. https://doi.org/10.4049/jimmunol.172.9.5702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nimgaonkar, V. L., Prasad, K. M., Chowdari, K. V., Severance, E. G., & Yolken, R. H. (2017). The complement system: A gateway to gene-environment interactions in schizophrenia pathogenesis. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(11), 15541561. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardiñas, A. F., Holmans, P., Pocklington, A. J., Escott-Price, V., Ripke, S., Carrera, N., … Walters, J. T. R. (2018). Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nature Genetics, 50(3), 381389. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0059-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
International Schizophrenia Consortium, Purcell, S. M., Wray, N. R., Stone, J. L., Visscher, P. M., O'Donovan, M. C., … Sklar, P. (2009). Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature, 460(7256), 748752. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08185.Google ScholarPubMed
Raychaudhuri, S., Sandor, C., Stahl, E. A., Freudenberg, J., Lee, H.-S., Jia, X., … de Bakker, P. I. W. (2012). Five amino acids in three HLA proteins explain most of the association between MHC and seropositive rheumatoid arthritis. Nature Genetics, 44(3), 291296. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.1076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, A. F. G., Roalf, D. R., Ruparel, K., Blake, J., Seelaus, K., Villa, L. P., … Satterthwaite, T. D. (2018). Quantitative assessment of structural image quality. NeuroImage, 169, 407418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium. (2011). Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci. Nature Genetics, 43(10), 969976. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature, 511(7510), 421427. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekar, A., Bialas, A. R., de Rivera, H., Davis, A., Hammond, T. R., Kamitaki, N., … McCarroll, S. A.(2016). Schizophrenia risk from complex variation of complement component 4. Nature, 530(7589), 177183. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sellgren, C. M., Gracias, J., Watmuff, B., Biag, J. D., Thanos, J. M., Whittredge, P. B., … Perlis, R. H. (2019). Increased synapse elimination by microglia in schizophrenia patient-derived models of synaptic pruning. Nature Neuroscience, 22(3), 374385. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0334-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sellgren, C. M., Sheridan, S. D., Gracias, J., Xuan, D., Fu, T., & Perlis, R. H. (2017). Patient-specific models of microglia-mediated engulfment of synapses and neural progenitors. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(2), 170177. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, J., Levinson, D. F., Duan, J., Sanders, A. R., Zheng, Y., Pe'er, I., … Gejman, P. V. (2009). Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia. Nature, 460(7256), 753757. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeland, O. B., Bahrami, S., Frei, O., Shadrin, A., O'Connell, K., Savage, J., … Andreassen, O. A. (2019). Genome-wide analysis reveals extensive genetic overlap between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 25, 844853. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0332-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sønderby, I. E., Gústafsson, Ó., Doan, N. T., Hibar, D. P., Martin-Brevet, S., & Abdellaoui, A., … 16p11.2 European Consortium, for the ENIGMA-CNV working group. (2018). Dose response of the 16p11.2 distal copy number variant on intracranial volume and basal ganglia. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(3), 584602. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0118-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefansson, H., Ophoff, R. A., Steinberg, S., Andreassen, O. A., Cichon, S., Rujescu, D., … Collier, D. A. (2009). Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia. Nature, 460(7256), 744747. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephan, A. H., Barres, B. A., & Stevens, B. (2012). The complement system: An unexpected role in synaptic pruning during development and disease. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 369389. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stokowska, A., Atkins, A. L., Morán, J., Pekny, T., Bulmer, L., Pascoe, M. C., … Pekna, M. (2017). Complement peptide C3a stimulates neural plasticity after experimental brain ischaemia. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 140(2), 353369. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aww314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tenner, A. J., Stevens, B., & Woodruff, T. M. (2018). New tricks for an ancient system: Physiological and pathological roles of complement in the CNS. Molecular Immunology, 102, 313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.molimm.2018.06.264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timpson, N. J., Greenwood, C. M. T., Soranzo, N., Lawson, D. J., & Richards, J. B. (2018). Genetic architecture: The shape of the genetic contribution to human traits and disease. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 19(2), 110124. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Beek, J., Nicole, O., Ali, C., Ischenko, A., MacKenzie, E. T., Buisson, A., & Fontaine, M. (2001). Complement anaphylatoxin C3a is selectively protective against NMDA-induced neuronal cell death. Neuroreport, 12(2), 289293. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200102120-00022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Writing Committee for the ENIGMA-CNV Working Group, van der Meer, D., Sønderby, I. E., Kaufmann, T., Walters, G. B., Abdellaoui, A., … Andreassen, O. A. (2019). Association of copy number variation of the 15q11.2 BP1-BP2 region with cortical and subcortical morphology and cognition. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(4), 420430. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.3779.Google Scholar
van Erp, T. G. M., Hibar, D. P., Rasmussen, J. M., Glahn, D. C., Pearlson, G. D., Andreassen, O. A., … Turner, J. A. (2016). Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(4), 547553. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Erp, T. G. M., Walton, E., Hibar, D. P., Schmaal, L., Jiang, W., Glahn, D. C., … Turner, J. A. (2018). Cortical brain abnormalities in 4474 individuals with schizophrenia and 5098 control subjects via the enhancing neuro imaging genetics through meta analysis (ENIGMA) consortium. Biological Psychiatry, 84(9), 644654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Haren, N. E. M., Schnack, H. G., Cahn, W., van den Heuvel, M. P., Lepage, C., Collins, L., … Kahn, R. S. (2011). Changes in cortical thickness during the course of illness in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(9), 871880. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vasek, M. J., Garber, C., Dorsey, D., Durrant, D. M., Bollman, B., Soung, A., … Klein, R. S. (2016). A complement-microglial axis drives synapse loss during virus-induced memory impairment. Nature, 534(7608), 538543. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, J. T. R., Rujescu, D., Franke, B., Giegling, I., Vásquez, A. A., Hargreaves, A., … Owen, M. J. (2013). The role of the major histocompatibility complex region in cognition and brain structure: A schizophrenia GWAS follow-up. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 877885. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12020226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, C., Yue, H., Hu, Z., Shen, Y., Ma, J., Li, J., … Gu, Y. (2020). Microglia mediate forgetting via complement-dependent synaptic elimination. Science (New York, N.Y.), 367(6478), 688694. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaz2288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westlye, L. T., Alnæs, D., van der Meer, D., Kaufmann, T., & Andreassen, O. A. (2019). Population-based mapping of polygenic risk for schizophrenia on the human brain: New opportunities to capture the dimensional aspects of severe mental disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 86(7), 499501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.08.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2009). Ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York, USA: Springer-Verlag. https://www.springer.com/gp/book/9780387981413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woo, J. J., Pouget, J. G., Zai, C. C., & Kennedy, J. L. (2019). The complement system in schizophrenia: Where are we now and what's next? Molecular Psychiatry, 25(1), 114130. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0479-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, C., Lv, Q., Fan, W., Tang, W., & Yi, Z. (2017). Influence of CFH gene on symptom severity of schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 13, 697706. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S132108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. A schematic diagram of the methodology used to obtain predicted expression values for the C4A gene within brain tissue, as described by Sekar et al. (2016). First, (a) individual genotypes are determined and SNP haplotypes are then inferred from this data. (b) The SNP haplotypes can be grouped into haplogroups and each haplogroup corresponds to a specific C4 locus structure. Four of these structures are common (represented here) and 11 are less common (<10% frequency combined). HERV, human endogenous retroviral insertion. (c) Structures with higher copy numbers of C4A and C4L (both C4AL and C4BL) isotypes show higher C4A expression in brain tissue. (d) C4A gene expression can be predicted based on the data outlined in panels A–C. AL, AS, BL, and BS refer to the copy number of each of these isotypes in the C4 locus structure. Structures containing the AS combination are omitted from panels A to C since they are rare, with a frequency of approximately 1% (online Supplementary Table S1) (Sekar et al., 2016). This figure is a schematic and was not generated from actual genotype, expression or other data.

Figure 1

Table 1. Numbers of participants that completed each of the seven cognitive tasks, with available predicted C4A and C4B expression values and brain imaging data

Figure 2

Fig. 2. A summary of the results from the significant (FDR <0.05) linear regression models of predicted C4A expression values on cognitive performance and brain imaging measures. The results are presented in the context of a mediation model. (i) Higher predicted C4A expression was significantly associated with the results from three cognitive tasks. Path c = Cognitive task ~ C4A expression (ii) Predicted C4A expression was significantly associated with some measures of cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Path a = Brain imaging measure ~ C4A expression. (iii) A summary of the brain imaging measures identified to significantly mediate the effect of predicted C4A expression on cognitive performance. Path ab = Cognitive task ~ C4A expression mediated by brain imaging measures. The proportion of the total effect (Panel i, Path c) mediated by changes in the corresponding brain imaging measure is shown (Prop. Med = ab/c). Negative proportion values indicate inconsistent mediation. Inconsistent mediation occurs when the direction of effect of the direct effect (c’) and the indirect effect (ab) is in the opposite direction. The standardized β (Std. β) is shown to indicate the size and direction of effect of higher C4A expression on each outcome measure. The green and red headers indicate an increase or decrease in each outcome measure, respectively.

Figure 3

Table 2. A summary of the results from the significant linear regression models of predicted C4A expression values on cognitive performance

Figure 4

Fig. 3. The effect of C4A expression on regional measures of (a) cortical surface area and (b) mean cortical thickness. The colors correspond to the standardized β (Std. β) coefficient for each brain region from the linear regressions. Black demarcations around a brain region indicate that it passes the multiple comparisons–corrected significance threshold of FDR <0.05. a, Insula. b, Transverse temporal. c, Middle temporal. d, Cuneus. e, Pericalcarine. f, Posterior cingulate. g, Isthmuscingulate. h, Parahippocampal. i, Entorhinal. j, Medial orbitofrontal.

Supplementary material: PDF

O'Connell et al. supplementary material

Figures S1-S5

Download O'Connell et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.8 MB
Supplementary material: File

O'Connell et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S20

Download O'Connell et al. supplementary material(File)
File 195.8 KB