Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T08:48:14.428Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The pre-political origins and policy consequences of environmental justice concern

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2022

Matt Motta*
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, USA
*

Abstract

While the effects of climate change will impact most Americans, they will likely have a disproportionate influence on the socioeconomic well-being of marginalized communities. Few researchers, however, have investigated public support for policies aimed at ameliorating climate-related disparities. Fewer still have considered how political and (critically) pre-political psychological dispositions might shape environmental justice concern (EJC) and subsequently influence policy support—both of which, I argue, could present roadblocks for effective climate communication and policy action. In this registered report, I (1) propose and validate a new measure of EJC, (2) explore its political correlates and pre-political antecedents, and (3) test for a link between EJC and policy support. In addition to psychometrically validating the EJC scale, I find that pre-political value orientations are associated with EJC, which, in turn, mediates the effects of pre-political values on taking action to mitigate the unequal effects of climate change.

Type
Life Science in Politics: Methodological Innovations and Political Issues
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Association for Politics and the Life Sciences

With the planet increasingly unlikely to avoid warming in excess of 1.5 degrees Celsius by 2030, the social and environmental consequences of human-caused climate change have the potential to impact daily life for nearly all Americans (IPCC, Reference Allen, Babiker, Chen, de Coninck, Connors, van Diemen and Zickfeld2018). Unfortunately, a growing line of research suggests that America’s poor are particularly likely to experience economic hardship, food insecurity, and negative public health outcomes as a result of increasing average global temperatures (Pachauri et al., Reference Pachauri, Allen, Barros, Broome, Cramer, Christ and van Ypserle2014; Shi et al., Reference Shi, Chu, Anguelovski, Aylett, Debats, Goh and VanDeveer2016).

Environmental justice involves not only the documentation of inequity with respect to the effects of climate change on marginalized populations, but also the extent to which public policies can be tailored to help the most marginalized members of society adapt to the effects of a changing climate (Phillips & Sexton, Reference Phillips and Sexton1999). While the potentially undue burdens of climate change on the poor are well studied, less work investigates the nature and policy consequences of Americans’ attitudes toward environmental justice.

In this registered report, I aim to better understand Americans’ support for policy efforts that address the effects of climate change on the poor by asking why some people might be more likely to express concern about environmental justice than others. Critically, I further ask whether these concerns might be associated with support for specific policy actions informed by environmental justice considerations.

Answering these questions can help scholars better understand the opinion dynamics of environmental justice and solve an important climate communications challenge. For climate communication campaigns to effectively increase support for policies aimed at ameliorating climate-induced inequality, communicators must first identify audiences (i.e., demographic subgroups) that are more likely to oppose policies guided by environmental justice and then make an effort to understand their reasons for doing so. This information can help communicators target skeptical groups with messages aimed at increasing support for environmental justice by validating as opposed to challenging their prior social and political attitudes (e.g., Benegal & Scruggs, Reference Benegal and Scruggs2018; Bolsen et al., Reference Bolsen, Palm and Kingsland2019; Lunz Trujillo et al., Reference Lunz Trujillo, Motta, Callaghan and Sylvester2021; Motta et al., Reference Motta, Ralston and Spindel2021).

Understanding the pre-political antecedents of environmental justice concern could have particularly important implications for climate communication (Bayes et al., Reference Bayes, Bolsen and Druckman2020; Harrison, Reference Harrison1998). If EJC is the result of not just contemporary political polarization but also differences in pre-political value orientations, that could pose a major challenge for climate communicators hoping to inspire support for environmental justice policies. As basic human values are both widely held and highly stable, changing Americans’ value orientations is unlikely to be an effective communications strategy. Instead, communicators must find ways to craft pro-EJC messages within individuals’ value frameworks (Lupia, Reference Lupia2016; see Lunz Trujillo et al., Reference Lunz Trujillo, Motta, Callaghan and Sylvester2021, for a demonstration of this principle).

In this report, I aim to provide climate communicators with the information they need to facilitate effective communication about the benefits of policies that promote environmental justice. Specifically, I plan to advance the fields understanding of environmental justice by (1) developing and validating a new measure of concern about the effects of climate change on America’s poorest (hereafter environmental justice concern, or EJC), (2) assessing both the political correlates (e.g., partisan identity, attitudes toward scientific experts) and pre-political antecedents (i.e., bedrock value orientations) of EJC, and (3) devising multimethod tests to assess how EJC influences support for specific environmental justice policies.

In a demographically representative survey of N = 2,007 U.S. adults conducted in December 2021, I find—consistent with theoretical expectations—that Americans who self-identify as politically liberal and hold less racially resentful attitudes are more likely to express concern about the effects of climate change on the most marginalized members of society. I also find that EJC is associated with pre-political value orientations, such that highly self-transcendent people (i.e., those who tend to put others’ concerns ahead of their own) are more likely to express concern about environmental justice, while those who are highly self-enhancing (i.e., those who tend to put the welfare of themselves and their kin ahead of others’) are less likely to do so.

Further, and again consistent with theoretical expectations, I find that EJC is associated with support for taking policy action to mitigate the unequal effects of climate change on the most marginalized (e.g., offering discounted flood insurance to poor Americans who live in areas likely to be impacted by major tropical storms). Correspondingly, observational mediation analyses suggest that EJC partially mediates the effect of basic human value orientations on environmental justice policy attitudes.

I conclude by discussing how researchers can use the measures developed and validated in this study to facilitate future research about unanswered questions in the study of environmental justice opinion. I also discuss how science communicators and policy professionals can leverage these insights to build effective climate communication strategies and to encourage support for climate policy action that accounts for the welfare of the most marginalized.

Environmental injustice and climate policy

While the effects of climate change have the potential to negatively impact environmental, economic, and health outcomes for most Americans, they are especially likely to do so for America’s poor. Recognition and concern about climate-related injustice are important, as this can help formulate policies aimed at adapting to the effects of climate change in a more equitable way.

Climate change, for example, is likely to make coastal flooding more common in the coming years (IPCC, Reference Allen, Babiker, Chen, de Coninck, Connors, van Diemen and Zickfeld2018). The effects of flooding on local infrastructure and economies are particularly precarious in areas designated as 100-year floodplains (Wobus et al., Reference Wobus, Gutmann, Jones, Rissing, Mizukami, Lorie and Martinich2017)—that is, regions of the United States where the probability of inundation exceeds 1%.

Unfortunately, demographic assessments of 100-year floodplains (Bauer, Reference Bauer2017; Peri et al., Reference Peri, Rosoff and Yager2017) suggest that residents tend to be disproportionately poor and non-White compared to residents of surrounding areas (FEMA, 2021). This implies that flooding risks are not distributed equally throughout the population and tend to map onto existing forms of social and economic inequality.

One way to ameliorate climate injustice concerns is by offering Americans subsidized flood insurance from the federal government. Recognizing that poorer individuals may have less capacity to simply move out of flood zones as the effects of climate change become increasingly palpable, and that they may have less ability to afford protective insurance for their homes and property, Americans can apply for subsidized flood insurance through the National Flood Insurance Program (NFIP), established in 1968.

However, there are several issues with the NFIP in its current configuration. In addition to being more than $20 billion in debt, the program tends to assess risk (i.e., the harms of experiencing major flooding) on the basis of geography and home values, meaning that poorer individuals (who presumably have less costly homes and other property) may receive a lower risk valuation. Correspondingly, a recent survey of NFIP policies found that although poorer individuals are overrepresented in floodplains, more than 40% of subsidized policies were applied to homes worth more than $500,000, and 12% were applied to homes worth $1 million or more (CBO, 2007; Sigaud, Reference Siguad2018).

Environmental justice considerations can help policymakers pursue solutions to problems like this one. For example, adjusting the process by which the NFIP calculates risk—to include not only home values and geography but also individuals’ income (and, consequently, their ability to afford insurance)—could help poorer Americans better afford flood insurance (i.e., by receiving larger subsidies). Correspondingly, removing or decreasing subsidizes for the wealthy could help the NFIP recoup some of its growing debts and improve the program’s solvency. While legislation has been introduced to amend the NFIP in this way in the U.S. Senate (Holman, Reference Holman2019), the measures have not advanced out of committee.

There are many other ways in which policies informed by concerns about environmental justice can help reduce effects of climate change on America’s most marginalized populations. For example, recognizing that poorer Americans have less capacity to move in the event of naturally occurring disasters like wildfires—which are thought to be made more common by climate change (IPCC, Reference Allen, Babiker, Chen, de Coninck, Connors, van Diemen and Zickfeld2018; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2019)—the government could expand the Federal Emergency Management Agency’s housing relocation policies to provide more generous aid to individuals in the most financial need (current moving allocation rates are capped at just $4,000; FEMA, 2007) and/or to allow individuals to move in preemption of climate disaster (see also Herrman, Reference Herrman2017; Kuhl et al., Reference Kuhl, Kirshen, Ruth and Douglas2014).

Moreover, individuals who lack access to air-conditioning in areas experiencing extreme heat anomalies—which are thought to be caused by climate change—may be more likely to experience severe respiratory and cardiovascular illness (Ostro et al., Reference Ostro, Rauch, Green, Malig and Basu2010). Funding to establish or increase the operation of cooling centers (i.e., air-conditioned public areas) can help those without air-conditioning, especially in large cities, find opportunities to escape the heat. Poorer Americans may also have a more difficult time adjusting to the increased food prices that result from the effects of increasing global temperatures on agricultural productivity. Expanding federal food assistance programs could help the poorest Americans adjust to increasing prices for essential goods.

Conceptualizing environmental justice

These examples describe how the effects of climate change map onto existing forms of socioeconomic inequality and injustice that exist in American public life. Environmental justice concerns the extent to which these unequal outcomes can be ameliorated through climate policy action.

There is no singular definition of what environmental justice entails (Phillips & Sexton, Reference Phillips and Sexton1999; Sexton & Adgate, Reference Sexton and Adgate1999). However, pursuant to the foregoing review, I conceptualize environmental justice as the extent to which climate change adaptation policies address inequalities related to the probability that the most socioeconomically disadvantaged groups in society will experience negative physical, social, and/or economic effects of climate change.

Following from my conceptualization of environmental justice, I further conceptualize environmental justice concern as (1) entailing the recognition of the possibility of climate-related injustice on the basis of socioeconomic inequality and (2) being troubled by its (potentially) pernicious effects on America’s poor. Americans, of course, vary in their knowledge of the potential impacts of climate change on human life (Dunlap, 2010; Leiserowitz et al., Reference Leiserowitz, Maibach, Roser-Renouf, Feinberg and Rosenthal2018; Malka et al., Reference Malka, Krosnick and Langer2009), and therefore may also vary in the extent to which they are aware of potential climate-related injustices. Therefore, I argue that concern does not necessarily entail a priori awareness of specific climate-related injustices or the likelihood that they will occur, but (1) accepting the possibility that these harms might come to fruition and (2) viewing those outcomes as problematic conditional on them actually happening, or being made aware of the possibility that they might.

Before moving on, it is important to recognize that socioeconomic inequality in the United States is highly racialized. The experience of poverty, as well as Americans’ views toward the “deserving” versus “undeserving” poor, are strongly shaped by their negative racial attitudes; such that White Americans who harbor resentment toward (in particular) Black Americans tend to view them as less deserving of federal antipoverty assistance (Gilens, Reference Gilens1999; Winter, Reference Winter2008). Correspondingly, several studies have documented that White Americans’ negative racial attitudes have “spilled over” (Benegal, Reference Benegal2018; see also Tesler, Reference Tesler2012) to shape environmental attitudes. As Benegal and Holman (Reference Benegal and Holman2021) review, Americans who hold more racially prejudicial views tend to be less concerned about the effects of climate change and less supportive of climate change mitigation policies. As the authors review, this may be attributable, in part, to political elites making an effort to link efforts to reduce greenhouse gas emissions to the alleged exploitation of White workers in the coal industry, and the racialization of social services more broadly. Correspondingly, in the analyses that follow, I make an effort to account for the influence of Americans’ negative racial attitudes when studying the opinion dynamics of environmental justice opinion.

Additionally, this definition of EJC raises the thorny issue of how to characterize the approximately one-third of Americans who believe that climate change is not primarily the result of human activities, and the approximately two-fifths who reject that the climate is changing at all (Funk & Kennedy, Reference Funk and Kennedy2016; Motta et al., Reference Motta, Chapman, Stecula and Haglin2019; Williams, Reference Williams2011). I conceptually (and, later on, empirically) classify people who reject human-caused climate change—but nevertheless express being troubled about the possibility environmental injustice—as expressing EJC, as one can plausibly reject the possibility of human-caused (i.e., anthropogenic) climate change but nevertheless believe that the climate is changing, and therefore recognize the potential of these harms.

However, I neither conceptually nor empirically classify individuals who reject that the climate is changing at all as being concerned about environmental justice—even if they report that climate-related inequity is problematic. Because concern entails both being troubled by inequity and some reasonable level of recognition that these harms could actually occur as a result of climate change, not believing in climate change at all implies that even those who might theoretically be troubled by inequality may have no reasonable expectation that those concerns will ever be actualized.

The political correlates and pre-political antecedents of environmental justice concern political and social-psychological considerations

Because environmental justice lies at the intersection of two salient and divisive policy debates—the way government responds to mitigating and adapting to the effects of climate change, as well as efforts to bolster the welfare of marginalized communities—there is good reason to suspect that EJC is both politically and socially divisive.

For example, past research suggests that concern about the effects climate change on everyday life is highly politicized. Ideological conservatives are less likely to be concerned than ideological liberals about the negative effects of climate change (Brulle et al., Reference Brulle, Carmichael and Jenkins2012; Carmichael et al., Reference Carmichael, Brulle and Huxster2017). This ideological asymmetry may result, at least in part, from conservatives being less likely to believe that climate change is occurring and caused by human activity (Funk & Kennedy, Reference Funk and Kennedy2016; McCright & Dunlap, Reference McCright and Dunlap2011; Motta et al., Reference Motta, Chapman, Stecula and Haglin2019).

The rejection of scientific consensus on the reality, severity, and causes of climate change on the ideological right and beyond is the result of several social, psychological, and political forces. For example, conservatives are more likely to hold negative views toward climate scientists (Gauchat et al., Reference Gauchat, O’Brien and Mirosa2017) and the scientific community more generally (Gauchat, Reference Gauchat2012; Merkley, Reference Merkley2020; Motta, Reference Motta2018; Oliver & Rahn, Reference Oliver and Rahn2016), which may encourage resistance to evidence-based documentation of climate change.

The rise of right-wing populist political elites (e.g., former president Donald Trump) and social movements (e.g., the Tea Party) may help explain the connection between what some scholars call “anti-intellectualism”—the dislike and distrust of people who can credibly claim superior knowledge (expertise) on a particular subject (Motta, Reference Motta2018; Oliver & Rahn, Reference Oliver and Rahn2016)—and ideological conservatism. Populists’ rhetorical efforts to raise suspicion about the motivations of many types of social and political elites (including scientific experts) may breed resentment of those groups in the population, and thereby engender climate skepticism (see Merkley, Reference Merkley2020). Correspondingly, a growing line of political and science communication research finds that Americans are highly receptive to the ways that partisan elites talk about climate-related issues and public acceptance of scientific consensus on climate change (Merkley & Stecula, Reference Merkley and Stecula2018, Reference Merkley and Stecula2021).

Comparatively low levels of climate change acceptance on the ideological right may also result from what social psychologists refer to as “system-justifying” tendencies. Although these considerations are not unique to the ideological right, conservatives tend to be more motivated than liberals to view the status quo as fair and legitimate (Jost et al., Reference Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski and Sulloway2003). Correspondingly, conservatives with system-justifying tendencies have been shown to be motivated to reject the reality and severity of human-caused climate change (Feygina et al., Reference Feygina, Jost and Goldsmith2010), as doing so requires admitting that the current environmental regulation policies are insufficient to stave off an existential threat to human life. In the case of environmental justice considerations, expressing concern would also require admitting to the existence of systematic unfairness with respect to who is most likely to experience the harms of increasing average global temperatures. Consequently, system-justifying tendencies may be associated with low levels of concern about environmental justice as well.

Finally, as discussed earlier, environmental justice may prove to be politically and socially divisive because it pertains to the welfare of America’s poorest. Americans tend to view poor people as being disproportionately non-White (e.g., Winter, Reference Winter2008), which ties views toward public assistance—especially when referred to as “welfare”—to the public’s negative racial attitudes (Gilens, Reference Gilens1996, Reference Gilens1999; Rasinski, Reference Rasinski1989; see also Huber & Paris, Reference Huber and Paris2013). Consequently, it could be the case that highly racially resentful individuals—individuals who embrace implied but not necessarily explicit forms of racial prejudice (see Kinder & Sanders, Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Mendelberg, Reference Mendelberg2001)—are less likely to express EJC. Consistent with this view, a growing line of research suggests that efforts to tie former president Barack Obama’s race to a variety of different policy debates has facilitated the spillover (Tesler, Reference Tesler2012) of negative racial attitudes to public opinion in many areas, including climate change (Benegal, Reference Benegal2018).

With these considerations in mind, I suspect that contemporary grounds for social and political disagreement influence EJC, such that political conservatives (H1a), individuals who hold anti-intellectual attitudes (H1b), people with greater system-justifying tendencies (H1c), and more racially resentful individuals (H1d) will be more likely to express lower EJC.

Basic human value orientations

Concern about environmental justice, however, may not only be the result of political conflict. A growing line of research in political psychology suggests that Americans’ ideological affinities (Goren et al., Reference Goren, Smith and Motta2022; Piurko et al., Reference Piurko, Schwartz and Davidov2011), policy attitudes (Goren et al., Reference Goren, Motta and Smith2020; Rathbun et al., Reference Rathbun, Kertzer, Reifler, Goren and Scotto2016), and other politically relevant attitudes and behaviors (Goren & Motta, Reference Motta and Goren2021), result from differences in pre-political basic human values. For example, individuals who express stronger self-transcending values—that is, the tendency to put the needs of others (including universal concerns about the environment) ahead of their own—tend to be more likely to self-identify as ideological liberals (Goren et al., Reference Goren, Smith and Motta2022) and to support social welfare policies (Goren et al., Reference Goren, Motta and Smith2020).

Research in human values theory (Schwartz & Sagiv, Reference Schwartz and Sagiv1995), suggests that nearly all people, across cultures and national contexts, subscribe to the same set of 10 basic value orientations. These values are thought to antecede the development of political preferences (Goren, Reference Goren2013; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Oxley, Hibbing, Alford and Hibbing2011), and they are highly stable throughout the life course (Vecchione et al., Reference Vecchione, Schwartz, Alessandri, Dring, Castellani and Caprara2016; Vecchione et al., Reference Vecchione, Schwartz, Davidov, Cieciuch, Alessandri and Marsicano2020).

Correspondingly, based on insights from human values theory, I suspect that people who hold self-transcending value orientations—that is, individuals who tend to place the welfare of others (including the environment) ahead of their own—should be more likely to express EJC (H2a). In contrast, individuals who express more self-enhancing values—those who place a stronger emphasis on the well-being of themselves and their close kin—should be less likely to express EJC (H2b). However, self-enhancement effects could be conditioned by individuals’ own socioeconomic status (RQ1), as the self-enhancing goals of individuals of lower socioeconomic status may align with environmental justice concerns.

Linking environmental justice concern to policy action

In addition to investigating the sociopolitical correlates and pre-political antecedents of EJC, I further suspect that EJC has tangible policy consequences.

Past research suggests that people who express concern about the effects of climate change (in general) tend to be more supportive of efforts to mitigate climate change (Ehret et al., Reference Ehret, Van Boven and Sherman2018; Marquart-Pyatt et al., Reference Marquart-Pyatt, Shwom, Dietz, Dunlap, Kaplowitz, McCright and Zahran2011). Consequently, I suspect that EJC is correlated with support for policies aimed at remedying inequality in economic, health, and other outcomes attributable to climate change (H3).

Of course, as noted earlier, EJC attitudes are themselves likely to be the result of differences in both sociopolitical and pre-political value orientations (as discussed when proposing H2). Consequently, I further suspect that EJC mediates the effect of basic value orientations on policy support, such that self-transcending (H4a) and self-enhancing (H4b) values are associated with increased policy support (the former) and opposition (the latter).

Note that I limit these mediational expectations to correspond only with the effects of basic human value orientations, channeled through EJC. Because the sociopolitical factors thought to influence EJC are conceptually more proximal to the policy outcomes I study in this mediational framework (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Oxley, Hibbing, Alford and Hibbing2011; see also Goren et al., Reference Goren, Motta and Smith2020; Goren et al., Reference Goren, Smith and Motta2022), assessing how EJC might mediate the effect of those factors on policy attitudes could pose a test that is overly favorable to detecting mediation. For example, because political ideology is more proximal to policy orientations than more abstract value orientations, evidence of a (potentially strong) link between ideology and EJC would reduce the baseline effect of ideology on policy attitudes by comparatively more than it might otherwise for, in this case, basic human values.

Consequently, I err on the side of analytical conservatism and focus my expectations on just the mediating effect of EJC with respect to basic human values. This approach has the added benefit of allowing me to contribute to past research on the association between pre-political values and policy attitudes (e.g., Goren et al., Reference Goren, Motta and Smith2020) by unpacking a potential psychological mechanism that might link the two (see also Mosier, Reference Mosier2019).

Thus, collectively, support for H1H4 would suggest that EJC is the result not just of political but also of pre-political influences, which, in turn, shape support for efforts to ameliorate climate-related inequality. In the following sections, I outline a series of multimethod tests designed to assess the viability of these expectations.

Analytical strategy

I test H1H4 using a preregistered (https://osf.io/auhe2) multimethod (i.e., both observational and experimental) empirical setup, embedded in a demographically representative public opinion survey, conducted online via Lucid Theorem. Lucid uses quota sampling to achieve representativeness on several demographic factors (e.g., age, race, income, educational attainment, partisan identification).

I randomly assigned half (N = 1,022) of the respondents who consented to participate to complete four batteries relevant to this study: (1) a new measure of EJC, fashioned from four questions that assess concern (or lack of concern) about the effects of climate change on environmental justice (see Figure 1); (2) five Likert-style questions gauging support (or opposition) to policies aimed at decreasing climate-related inequality (see Table 1); (3) a standard 21-item battery measuring all 10 Schwartz value dimensions (aggregated into the self-enhancing and self-transcending value profiles); and (4) a series of standard questions measuring respondents’ anti-expert attitudes (i.e., anti-intellectualism; see Oliver & Rahn, Reference Oliver and Rahn2016), negative racial attitudes (i.e., racial resentment; see Kinder & Sanders, Reference Kinder and Sanders1996); system-justifying tendencies (Feygina et al., Reference Feygina, Jost and Goldsmith2010), and political orientations (i.e., ideological self-placement; see Ellis & Stimson, Reference Ellis and Stimson2012; on partisan identification, see Krosnick & Berent, Reference Krosnick and Berent1993).

Table 1. Summary of items used to measure environmental justice concern and policy orientations.

Figure 1. Psychometric validation of the EJC scale. Panels a and b present category characteristic curves and item information functions for individuals expressing the highest levels of concern about environmental injustice across items. Panel c is a skree plot resulting from principal factor analyses (with varimax rotation). Panel d is a histogram summarizing the distribution of the EJC scale extracted from the graded response modeling procedure summarized in Panels a and b. Please refer to the main text for additional information about these analyses, and to Table 1 for the full question wording associated with each constituent item.

Batteries were presented in random order, with question-level randomization and rotated response options. Additionally, while data from Lucid have been shown to closely mirror demographic benchmarks and to replicate well-studied experimental effects (Coppock & McClellan, Reference Coppock and McClellan2019), some researchers have expressed concern about inattentive responding on the platform (Aronow et al., Reference Aronow, Kalla, Orr and Ternovski2020). Consequently, I embedded a series of attention and bot-verification checks (i.e., captchas) throughout the survey.

I assigned the remaining half of the respondents (N = 985) to complete the same series of tasks, with an important amendment. Respondents were administered the foregoing items in a fixed order, such that (1) and (2) were administered (in that order) at the end of the survey.

Prior to presenting (1), I attempted to manipulate EJC through a short thought-listing (perspective-taking) task. The remaining respondents were asked to generate a list of “strategies for keeping cool” on an anomalous late-spring day when temperatures exceeded 105 degrees Fahrenheit, while imagining that they were a “a parent of two working two jobs, and living in a major urban area in an apartment without access to air-conditioning”—that is, to imagine that they were in the position of some of society’s most marginalized.

This procedure provides an opportunity to directly manipulate EJC, which, as I note later, is critical for assessing not only the new measure’s validity, but also its policy consequences (H4).

Data and measures

Data

As indicated in the Analytical strategy section, data for this study come from a large (N = 2,007) and demographically representative sample of US adults, fielded December 3–4, 2021. Data were collected in partnership with Lucid Theorem, an online opt-in sampling service that uses quota sampling techniques to target demographic representativeness to nationally representative benchmarks on respondents’ age, race, gender, household income, educational attainment, census region, and political party. To account for any remaining deviations between the sample and the U.S. adult population, I constructed survey weights to adjust for respondents’ age, race, gender, income, and educational attainment (see Motta, Reference Motta2020; Motta & Goren, Reference Motta and Goren2021, for examples of how this procedure is implemented in politically relevant studies using the Lucid platform).

Measures

Pursuant to the analytical strategy outlined earlier, the primary outcome variables in this study are measures of environmental justice concern (EJC; H1H2) and environmental justice policy attitudes (H3H4). The items used to construct each scale are presented in full in Table 1. I index the EJC items (and, at times, for analytical simplicity, the environmental justice policy items) together using a graded response modeling application of item response theory. Prior to engaging in hypothesis testing using each of these two measures, I provide a series of analyses designed to assess the psychometric validity of the EJC scale (see the section on Validating the EJC scale).

The primary set of independent variables in this study are respondents’ self-enhancing and self-transcending values. I measure self-transcending value orientations using a standard series of questions asking whether respondents agree or disagree (on a 7-point Likert scale) with five items designed to measure benevolence and universal values (e.g., “It is very important to me to help the people around me. I want to care for other people”). I measure self-enhancing values using four items designed to tap achievement and power values (e.g., “It is very important to show others my abilities. I want people to admire what I do.”) Consistent with conventional practice (e.g., Goren et al., Reference Goren, Motta and Smith2020; see also Goren & Motta Reference Motta and Goren2021, for an example of past research using an identical approach to measuring self-enhancing and self-transcending values), I compute average scores on both sets of items (α = 0.86 and 0.87, respectively).

A full list of items used to construct the basic human values measures, as well as all other described here, can be found in the Supplementary Materials. Note also that all independent variables used throughout this study are scored to range from 0 to 1, such that a score of 0 indicates the minimum observed value and 1 corresponds to the maximum.

I also control for respondents’ symbolic ideology (H1a), anti-intellectual attitude endorsement (H1b), system justification (H1c), and racial resentment in all multivariate models presented throughout this study. I measure symbolic ideology—that is, respondents’ psychological attachments to liberal/conservative ideological labels (Ellis & Stimson, Reference Ellis and Stimson2012)—using a standard 7-point self-placement scale (Kinder & Kalmoe, Reference Kinder and Kalmoe2017).

Here, it is important to recognize that conventional approaches to measuring symbolic ideology measures are necessarily unidimensional (i.e., assessed using a left-right scale), which may induce systematic measurement error if people hold conflicting operational (i.e., issue-based; see Ellis & Stimson, Reference Ellis and Stimson2012) ideological preferences in social, economic, and other policy domains (Feldman & Johnston, Reference Feldman and Johnston2014; Treier & Hillygus, Reference Treier and Hillygus2009). Moreover, as symbolic ideology is typically measured with a single survey question (or a set of branched questions, as in the American National Election Studies), it may be prone to random measurement error as well (Ansolabehere et al., Reference Ansolabehere, Rodden and Snyder2008; Ksiazkiewicz et al., Reference Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke and Krueger2016).

Consequently, for completeness, I administered short policy question batteries designed to gauge respondents’ social and economic operational views. Although operational ideology can be measured in many different ways, I make use of a scale implemented by Goren and colleagues (Reference Goren, Motta and Smith2020) that studies both symbolic and operational ideological preferences in relation to basic human values, and thereby serves as useful analytical benchmark for this study.

Additionally, I measure anti-intellectual attitude endorsement using a standard question (Merkley, Reference Merkley2020; Motta, Reference Motta2018) developed by Oliver and Rahn (Reference Oliver and Rahn2016) that asks respondents the extent to which they agree with the following statement: “I’d rather put my trust in the wisdom of ordinary people than the opinions of experts and intellectuals.” Respondents could indicate agreement or disagreement on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.”

I control for system-justifying tendencies using Kay and Jost’s (Reference Kay and Jost2003) abbreviated (eight-item) questionnaire, which asks respondents to agree or disagree with statement such as “Society is set up so that people usually get what they deserve.” Again, I measure agreement/disagreement using 7-point Likert scales and average responses to create a single scale (α = 0.77).

Finally, I measure racial resentment using a standard four-item indicator developed by Kinder and Sanders (Reference Kinder and Sanders1996) and featured regularly in the American National Election Studies. The scale gauges respondents’ agreement or disagreement with statements such as “It’s really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if blacks would only try harder they could be just as well-off as whites.” I again offered response options on a 7-point Likert scale and averaged responses to create a single index (α = 0.77). As negative racial attitudes likely vary by respondents’ racial identity, I offer sensitivity analyses in which I reestimate the effects of racial resentment among self-identified White participants only.

All models account for a variety of demographic controls, including respondents’ gender (a dichotomous indicator of whether respondents self-identify as women), racial and ethnic identity (dichotomous indicators of whether respondents self-identify as Black [non-Hispanic] or Hispanic), age group (dichotomous indicators of whether respondents are aged 18–24, 25–44, 44–65, or older than 65), household income (an intervalized 24-point scale, scored to range 0–1), and educational attainment (a dichotomous indicator of whether respondents hold a college degree).

Additionally, for the purposes of assessing the proposed EJC scale’s discriminant validity, I include a series of questions that measure concern about the effects of climate change outside of environmental justice domains (i.e., concern about the national security effects of climate change on U.S. military base integrity and international resource conflict; see Motta et al., Reference Motta, Ralston and Spindel2021, for additional information about these measures). In addition to correlating these items with the EJC scale, I control for concern about the effects of climate change on national security in all multivariate models that assess the effects of EJC on policy attitudes. Additional information about these measures is available in the Supplementary Materials.

Results

Validating the EJC scale

Before moving on, I want to recognize that because I am introducing a new measure of environmental justice concern to the study of environmental justice attitudes, it is important to psychometrically validate my approach to measuring it. I summarize the results of several efforts to validate this new measures in Figure 1. Note that each validation check was included as part of a preregistered analysis plan, available at https://osf.io/auhe2 (see the Analytical strategy section). The data and syntax necessary to replicate these analyses (and all analyses presented in this article) are available at the following time-stamped preregistration components site: https://osf.io/3p9zv.

Panels a and b in Figure 1 validate the previously discussed item response theory approach to measuring EJC by presenting the results of each graded response modeling procedure. Panel a presents category characteristic curves, which denote the probability (y-axis) that respondents earn a particular score on the latent EJC scale derived from the graded response model (x-axis), for those expressing high levels of concern about the effects of environmental injustice. Demonstrative of strong cohesion between the items, the S-shaped curves in Figure 1 suggest that individuals expressing strong concern are highly unlikely to earn low scores on the latent scale, and very likely to earn high scores on each measure. This means that, for both concern and policy attitudes, all four items comprising each scale are similarly strong indicators of the underlying construct.

Panel b in Figure 1 provides further psychometric validation of this measurement approach by displaying item information functions for individuals indicating high levels of EJC. The height of each curve (y-axis) denotes how well each item differentiates between whether respondents earn higher (versus lower) scores at each corresponding point on the latent variable (x-axis). As Figure 1 demonstrates, high levels of concern on all four items (versus not demonstrating high levels of concern) is an informative indicator of whether respondents fall above (if highly concerned) or below (if less concerned) the scale’s midpoint. Moreover, as the figure suggests, indicating strong levels of concern about the health effects of environmental injustice is a particularly strong indicator of whether respondents fall above or below the scale’s midpoint.

While Figure 1 suggests that both sets of items cohere with one another and are similarly strong indicators of where respondents are placed on each latent scale resulting from the item response theory procedure, it is nevertheless important to further document that the set of EJC items loads onto a single factor—that is, that the concern items constitute a singular (as opposed to multidimensional) construct. Panel C in Figure 1 is a skree plot, which presents eigenvalue scores resulting from a principle factor analysis with varimax rotation (y-axis) across several potential factor solutions. I find that a single-factor solution produces eigenvalue scores in excess of 1 (EV = 2.79), which indicates that a single-factor solution best fits the data and further underscores the unidimensionality of each measure.

In analyses not presented in the figure, I further probe the psychometric validity of the EJC scale by assessing its construct and discriminant validity (see Carmines & Zeller, Reference Carmines and Zeller1979; Fink, Reference Fink, Phillips, Mallinson, Wilson, Gruber and Backhouse2010). If the resulting EJC scale is has construct validity, efforts to manipulate concern about the unequal effects of climate change (as I do in the experimental protocol described earlier) should be associated with significantly higher scores on the EJC scale. Demonstrative of the scale’s construct validity, simple difference of means tests suggest that individuals assigned to participate (M = 0.64) versus not participate (M = 0.63) in the perspective-taking scored slightly higher on the EJC scale (∆M = 0.01), although these differences failed to attain conventional levels of two-tailed significance (t = 0.58, p = n.s.).

I caution, of course, that the preregistered thought-listing task protocol can be thought of as an “intent to treat” (ITT) design. This means that failure to document differences across the treatment and control groups could result from some individuals failing to participate in the thought-listing task. In post hoc analyses that account for the ITT nature of this intervention by removing individuals from the analysis who spent only a limited amount of time engaging with the task (less than 30 seconds), I observe a larger (∆M = 0.03) difference between the treatment (M = 0.66) and control (M = 0.63) groups, which approaches levels of two-tailed significance at the p = .06 level (t = 1.91). These analyses provide further, albeit limited, evidence in favor of the EJC scale’s construct validity.

Moreover, it should be the case that EJC should be associated with, yet analytically distinct from, measures that assess concern about the effects of climate change in other domains (discriminant validity). Specifically, while I would expect EJC to be correlated with more general concerns about the social and environmental effects of climate change (e.g., the idea that melting ice caps may cause sea levels to rise), as both measures pertain to concern about climate change and its potential effects, the effects of EJC on more specific concerns about the role that climate change might play in undermining national security (e.g., damaging the structural integrity of naval bases; see Motta et al., Reference Motta, Ralston and Spindel2021) should be relatively muted after accounting for the role of concern about the effects of a changing climate more generally.

In analyses presented in the Supplemental Materials, I show that while EJC and a three-item index measuring more general concerns about climate change (from the General Social Survey) are indeed correlated with one another (r = 0.53), EJC bears little association with more specific concerns about the effects of climate change on national security after accounting for more general concerns. Compared with a naive model that regresses national security concerns on EJC, accounting for more general climate concerns reduces the size of EJC’s effect by 32 percentage points (β Naive = 0.40, p < .05; β Full = 0.08, p < .05; ∆β = –0.32; recall that because all items are keyed to range 0–1, parameter estimates can be interpreted as percentage point change). Moreover, the amount of variance explained in the naive model accounts for just 24% of the total variance explained in national security concerns, whereas a model containing both EJC and more general concern items explains more than double (61%; ∆R 2 = 37%). Correspondingly, it appears that EJC is both conceptually related to concerns about the effects of climate change more generally, yet analytically distinct from more specific concerns about the effects of climate change in other politically relevant applications.

Finally, having psychometrically validated this approach to measuring EJC, I derived latent, quasi-Bayesian predictions from the graded response model (using the predict command in Stata 15) to assess respondents’ scores on a resulting EJC scale. I plot the distribution of the scale as a histogram, summarized in Panel D. I recoded the scale from their original metrics to range from 0 (indicating low levels of concern) to 1 (indicating high levels of concern). The resulting scale indicates that the modal respondent earns scores approaching the scale’s maximum (approximately 30%), with a fair amount of variation between expressing the lowest and highest observed levels of concern (M = 0.63, SD = 0.32). Most Americans, it seems, are at least somewhat concerned about the unequal effects that climate change might have on America’s poor.

Assessing the correlates of EJC

Having established the psychometric validity of the EJC scale, I next assess the extent to which self-transcending values and self-enhancing values as associated with increased (for the former) and/or decreased (for the latter) concern about the unequal effects of climate change. Pursuant to the preregistered analysis plan, I do this through a series of models that regress the EJC index, using ordinary least squares (OLS), and each constituent item (using ordered logistic regression) on respondents’ negative racial attitudes, political ideology, system-justifying tendencies, and anti-expert attitudes (H1), as well as their self-transcending (H2a) and self-enhancing values (H2b), and a series of demographic controls (respondents’ race, gender, age, educational attainment, and household income). Supplementally, I interact the self-enhancement values measure with the household income control to test for the possibility of moderated effects (RQ1).

The results are presented in Table 2. Turning first to the sociopolitical correlates of EJC, I find that—consistent with my preregistered expectations—increased ideological conservatism (H1a) and racial resentment (H1d) are negatively and significantly associated with EJC. This is true of each constituent item used to build the EJC index and the index itself. Contrary to expectations, however, I find that system-justifying attitudes (H1b) and anti-expert attitudes (H1c) bear no statistically discernible relationship with EJC.

Table 2. The effects of sociopolitical factors and pre-political values on EJC.

* p < .05, two-tailed.

Note. Ordered logistic (columns 1–4) and OLS (column 5) regression parameters presented with standard errors in parentheses. Please refer to Table 1 for a description of all outcome variables.

Perhaps more importantly, for my purposes in this article, I find that basic human value orientations are strongly associated with EJC. Consistent with H2a, I find that self-transcending values are positively and significantly associated with increased EJC across all model specifications. Because all variables are keyed to range 0–1, the results of the OLS model summarizing the effects of self-transcending values on the EJC index (column 5) can be interpreted as the percentage point change in EJC, given movement from the minimum to maximum observed values on the self-transcendence index. The results suggest that being highly self-transcendent (versus being less self-transcendent) is associated with a 47 percentage point increase in EJC.

Conversely, and consistent with H2a, I find that self-enhancing values are negatively and significantly associated with EJC across all model specifications. Again turning to the summary OLS model presented in table’s final column, I find that movement from the minimum to maximum observed values on the self-enhancement scale are associated with a 12 percentage point decrease in EJC.

Additionally, as stipulated in the pre-analysis plan (RQ1), I test for the possibility that levels of EJC may be elevated for individuals who place a strong emphasis on self-enhancing values but are less well-off financially—that is, individuals who act in their own self-interest by expressing concern about for environmental injustice. To do this, I modify the models presented in Figure 1, such that I interact the self-enhancing value scale with an interval measure of respondents’ household income. In analyses presented in the Supplementary Materials, I find that the effect of self-enhancing values is no different for individuals who have high versus low household incomes (p = n.s. in all cases).

Collectively, the results suggest that basic human values are both substantively and significantly associated with EJC, in theoretically anticipated ways. The strong link between value orientations and EJC further raises the possibility that values might also play a role in shaping support for taking action to ameliorate the unequal effects of climate change on America’s poor. I consider this possibility both observationally and experimentally in the analyses that follow.

The effects of values and EJC on policy attitudes

Finally, and pursuant to the pre-analysis plan, I test for the possibility that EJC is associated with support for taking policy action to combat the unequal effects of climate change through a series of ordered logistic regression models that regress each environmental justice policy support item—as well as (for analytical simplicity) a summary index constructed using the same graded response modeling procedures employed to construct the EJC scale (full model output is available in the Supplementary Materials)—on the EJC index, plus all covariates in the models used to test H1H2. To ensure that EJC, and not less conceptually related indicators of climate concern, are responsible for any support I might observe for H3, I also include the scale used to assess concern about the national security implications of climate change (as noted in my pre-analysis plan and employed earlier as a discriminant validation check). This approach allows me to assess whether EJC and environmental justice policy views are correlated with one another; accounting for factors that could jointly influence both.

Joint influence, of course, poses a problem for conventional regression modeling, as it assumes a lack of independence between the outcome and explanatory measures. Consequently, I test H4 using a series of observational mediation models (i.e., for the 1,000 respondents in which I observe EJC) by entering the models described in Table 2 into Hicks and Tingley’s (Reference Hicks and Tingley2011) software for Stata 15, treating EJC as the hypothesized mediator and each set of pre-political values as the observational “treatments.” This allows me to quantify the direct effects of self-transcending (H4a) and self-enhancing (H4b) values on environmental justice policy attitudes, as well as the indirect effects channeled through the hypothesized mediator (EJC).

Table 3 begins this analysis by summarizing the results of both the correlational analyses used to test H3. Consistent with theoretical expectations, I find that EJC is positively and significantly associated with increased support for each policy effort aimed at mitigating the effects of environmental injustice, as well as the summary scale. Again, as all variables were keyed to range 0–1, the results presented in the final column of Table 3 can be interpreted in terms of percentage point change. There, and by way of summarizing the effects of EJC on policy orientations, I find that movement from the minimum to maximum observed levels of EJC is associated with an 11% increase in support across the policy index.

Table 3. The effect of EJC and pre-political values on environmental justice policy support.

* p < .05, two-tailed.

Note. Ordered logistic (columns 1–4) and OLS (column 5) regression parameters presented with standard errors in parentheses. Please refer to Table 1 for a description of all outcome variables.

Table 3 further suggests, however, that self-transcending and self-enhancing values are infrequently associated with policy support. As described earlier, it may be that EJC, which is itself strongly shaped by pre-political value orientations, is acting as a mediating influence. The observational mediation tests outlined in my pre-analysis plan provide strong evidence in favor of this view. I again turn to the summary policy index when calculating these quantities, in pursuit of both analytical simplicity (the mediate package for Stata 15 does not support ordered logistic regression estimation) and parsimony.

The results suggest that self-transcending values (direct effect = 0.20) and, to a lesser degree, self-enhancing values (direct effect = 0.05) are associated with increased policy support in the absence of the EJC. The total effect (i.e., the direct effects plus all indirect effects explained via EJC) of self-transcending (total effect = 0.34) and self-enhancing (total effect = 0.08) values indicate an additional effect of pre-political values channeled through EJC. This implies that 40% of the effects of self-transcending values ( $ \frac{total- direct}{total} $ ; $ \frac{0.14}{0.34} $ ), and 25% of the effects of self-transcending values ( $ \frac{0.02}{0.08} $ ) are explained via EJC. Consistent with H4, these results imply that the effects of self-transcending (H4a) and self-enhancing (H4b) values are at least partially mediated by the EJC index.

Finally, it is important to note that although I intended in my pre-analysis plan to run experimental mediation analyses as an additional test of H4 (see Analytical strategy), I found inconsistent evidence that the ITT thought-listing task increased scores on the EJC scale across treatment versus the control groups (see Validating the EJC scale). Consequently, it makes little sense to test for experimental mediation, as the hypothesized mediator was not, in fact, influenced by the experimental treatment. Correspondingly, I omit these analyses from the presentation of the results.

Discussion

Pre-political attitudes appear to play a powerful role in giving structure to Americans’ attitudes about environmental justice. In this registered report, I showed that, even when accounting for a diverse set of sociopolitical factors that might otherwise influence concern about the unequal impact of climate change on America’s poor, more self-transcendent (other-focused) people are more likely to express environmental justice concern, while more self-enhancing (self-focused) people are less likely to do so. The effect of basic human values extends to views about policy efforts to neutralize the unequal effects of climate change, such that EJC mediates the effects of self-transcending values—and, to a lesser degree, self-enhancing values—on Americans’ climate policy orientations.

This work advances previous research on environmental justice and climate change opinion in at least two ways. First, by both conceptualizing and psychometrically validating the EJC scale, this work provides the field with a short (four-item) measure of Americans’ concerns about climate injustice that can be readily (and, I hope, confidently) exported to other efforts to survey Americans’ views on these issues.

Second, and more generally, these findings contribute to what scholars understand about the nature of environmental justice opinion in the United States. As prior work in this area suggests, I show that concerns about climate injustice map onto existing partisan and sociocultural divides in the United States. Critically, though, I also show that environmental justice attitudes may have pre-political origins, reflecting not only contemporary political disagreements, but fundamental differences in value systems that are thought to give rise to political and social orientations.

The pre-political origins of environmental justice concern and policy orientations have important implications for climate communicators hoping to increase support for policy efforts to neutralize the unequal effects of climate change. Given their centrality in shaping a wide range of socially relevant attitudes and behaviors, basic human values have been shown to be unlikely to change over the life course (Vecchione et al., Reference Vecchione, Schwartz, Davidov, Cieciuch, Alessandri and Marsicano2020). Consequently, efforts to increase concern about the effects of climate change may be better served not by trying to change what it is that Americans value but by instead attempting to create messages that comport with existing value frameworks (see Lupia, Reference Lupia2016; Lunz Trujillo et al., Reference Lunz Trujillo, Motta, Callaghan and Sylvester2021).

For example, having documented that more-self-enhancing people are less likely to express EJC—and subsequently are less likely to support taking policy action to ameliorate the unequal effects of climate change—strategic communicators might craft messaging campaigns that emphasize the personal costs of failing to address environmental injustice. This could take the form of focusing on the negative and far-reaching economic consequences of injustice beyond the most marginalized communities, and/or by drawing attention to the possibility that anyone might one day find themselves “down on their luck” and potentially subject to the unequal impacts of climate change.

Although it is difficult to “micro-target” messages like these on the basis of basic human values alone—because there are no television programs or social network groups that exclusively cater to populations who hold self-focused values—communicators could nevertheless disseminate these messages strategically. Communicators might, for example, focus messaging campaigns on geographic areas thought to have higher (versus lower) concentrations of those who embrace self-focused values (e.g., see Motta & Goren, Reference Motta and Goren2021; Piurko et al., Reference Piurko, Schwartz and Davidov2011), and/or by advertising in political (for example) social net working groups organized around individual liberty considerations. Whatever form these messaging strategies take, they could prove essential in bolstering public concern about environmental injustice, which may create a demand for policy action, and thereby incentivize policymakers to make an effort to address the unequal effects of climate change.

This research, of course, is not without important limitations. Perhaps most obviously, this registered report presents the result of a single opinion survey, conducted at a single point in time, and recruited respondents through an online opt-in sampling service that is not formally representative of the U.S adult population. In the future, scholars ought to consider replicating insights from this study in other opinion survey contexts that vary both with respect to timing, as well as their sampling protocols. For example, it could be the case that the null experimental treatment effects I observe in this study are not necessarily the result of a “weak” experimental manipulation or potential respondent inattentiveness, but are simply due to the possibility that, for most people, EJC, much like the values thought to underlie it, is relatively slow to change over time. In other words, longitudinal analyses could prove useful in determining the temporal stability of EJC attitudes.

Likewise, this study featured just a single set of policy-focused questions related to efforts to address the unequal effects of climate change on marginalized groups. The four policies here represent a small subset of the many ways policymakers might think about addressing climate injustice. As ameliorative policy proposals gain (or lose) traction, scholars should consider revising the items studied here to reflect ongoing policy debates.

Moreover, the basic human values inventory that I study in this piece represents just one set of abstract (and not expressly political) values that researchers might wish to study. For example, Inglehart and Abramson’s (Reference Inglehart and Abramson1999) Materialism/Post-Materialism (MPM) value scale is conceptually similar (with respect to what it intends to measure) as the basic human values inventory but employs a different measurement approach. Correspondingly, MPM has been shown to have less internal validity in cross-cultural settings than the Schwartz values inventory, but may have comparatively higher levels of predictive validity (Datler et al., Reference Datler, Jagodzinski and Schmidt2013). As this research has ties to both postmaterial (i.e., its well-documented link to environmentalism; e.g., Mostafa, Reference Mostafa2013) and material concerns (i.e., systematic unfairness with respect to they ways in which marginalized populations experience the effects of climate change), I encourage researchers to consider replicating this study using other apolitical value inventories to assess both the conceptual extensions and empirical robustness of the results presented in this article.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/pls.2022.7.

Open Scientific Practices Statement

The materials, data, and preregistration that support the findings of this study and the award of the three open science badges are publicly available at https://osf.io/3p9zv.

Footnotes

This article earned Open Materials, Open Data, and Preregistration badges for open scientific practices. For details, see the Open Scientific Practices Statement.

References

Allen, M. R., Babiker, M., Chen, Y., de Coninck, H., Connors, S., van Diemen, R., … & Zickfeld, K. (2018). Summary for policymakers. In Global Warming of 1.5: An IPCC Special Report on the impacts of global warming of 1.5\C above pre-industrial levels and related global greenhouse gas emission pathways, in the context of strengthening the global response to the threat of climate change, sustainable development, and efforts to eradicate poverty. IPCC.Google Scholar
Ansolabehere, S., Rodden, J., & Snyder, J. M. Jr. (2008). The strength of issues: Using multiple measures to gauge preference stability, ideological constraint, and issue voting. American Political Science Review, 102(2), 215232.Google Scholar
Aronow, P. M., Kalla, J., Orr, L., & Ternovski, J. (2020). Evidence of rising rates of inattentiveness on Lucid in 2020. SocArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/8sbe4.Google Scholar
Bauer, MR. (2017). Jacksonville’s Poorest Residents Live in the Worst Flood Zones. Bloomberg. https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2017-11-15/jacksonville-s-poorest-residents-live-in-flood-zonesGoogle Scholar
Bayes, R., Bolsen, T., & Druckman, J. N. (2020). A research agenda for climate change communication and public opinion: The role of scientific consensus messaging and beyond. Environmental Communication. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2020.1805343Google Scholar
Benegal, S. D. (2018). The spillover of race and racial attitudes into public opinion about climate change. Environmental Politics, 27(4), 733756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benegal, S. D., & Holman, M. R. (2021). Racial prejudice, education, and views of climate change. Social Science Quarterly, 102(4), 19071919.Google Scholar
Benegal, S. D., & Scruggs, L. A. (2018). Correcting misinformation about climate change: The impact of partisanship in an experimental setting. Climatic Change, 148(1–2), 6180.Google Scholar
Benegal, S. D. (2018). The spillover of race and racial attitudes into public opinion about climate change. Environmental Politics, 27(4), 733756.Google Scholar
Bolsen, T., Palm, R., & Kingsland, J. T. (2019). The impact of message source on the effectiveness of communications about climate change. Science Communication, 41(4), 464487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting public opinion on climate change: an empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the US, 2002–2010. Climatic Change, 114(2), 169188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmichael, J. T., Brulle, R. J., & Huxster, J. K. (2017). The great divide: Understanding the role of media and other drivers of the partisan divide in public concern over climate change in the USA, 2001–2014. Climatic Change, 141(4), 599612.Google Scholar
Carmines, E. G., & Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and validity assessment. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Coppock, A., & McClellan, O. A. (2019). Validating the demographic, political, psychological, and experimental results obtained from a new source of online survey respondents. Research & Politics, 6(1), 2053168018822174.Google Scholar
Datler, G., Jagodzinski, W., & Schmidt, P. (2013). Two theories on the test bench: Internal and external validity of the theories of Ronald Inglehart and Shalom Schwartz. Social Science Research, 42(3), 906925.Google Scholar
Ehret, P. J., Van Boven, L., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Partisan barriers to bipartisanship: Understanding climate policy polarization. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(3), 308318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, C., & Stimson, J. A. (2012). Ideology in America. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3): 337358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global warming, and the possibility of system-sanctioned change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(3), 326338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fink, A. (2010). Survey research methods. In Phillips, D., Mallinson, V., Wilson, K., Gruber, K. H., & Backhouse, J. K. (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of Education (pp. 152160). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funk, C., & Kennedy, B. (2016, October 4). The politics of climate. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2016/10/04/the-politics-of-climate/Google Scholar
Gauchat, G. (2012). Politicization of science in the public sphere: A study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. American Sociological Review, 77(2), 167187.Google Scholar
Gauchat, G., O’Brien, T., & Mirosa, O. (2017). The legitimacy of environmental scientists in the public sphere. Climatic Change, 143(3–4), 297306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilens, M. (1996). Race coding and white opposition to welfare. American Political Science Review, 90(3), 593604.Google Scholar
Gilens, M. (1999). Why Americans hate welfare: Race, media, and the politics of antipoverty policy. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goren, P. (2013). On voter competence. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goren, P., Motta, M., & Smith, B. (2020). The ideational foundations of symbolic ideology. Political Psychology, 41(S1), 7594.Google Scholar
Goren, P., Smith, B., & Motta, M. (2022). Human values and sophistication interaction theory. Political Behavior, 44, 4973.Google Scholar
Harrison, N. E. (1998). Why science and technology require political guidance to sustain development. Politics and the Life Sciences, 17(2), 179188.Google Scholar
Hicks, R., & Tingley, D. (2011). Causal mediation analysis. The Stata Journal, 11(4), 605619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrman, V. (2017). The United States’ Climate Change Relocation Plan. The Arctic Institute. https://www.thearcticinstitute.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/09/The-United-States-Climate-Change-Relocation-Plan_2017.pdfGoogle Scholar
Holman, L. (2019). Congress tackles flood insurance reform. National Association of Counties. https://www.naco.org/articles/congress-tackles-flood-insurance-reformGoogle Scholar
Huber, G. A., & Paris, C. (2013). Assessing the programmatic equivalence assumption in question wording experiments: Understanding why Americans like assistance to the poor more than welfare. Public Opinion Quarterly, 77(1), 385397.Google Scholar
Inglehart, R., & Abramson, P. R. (1999). Measuring postmaterialism. American Political Science Review, 93(3), 665677.Google Scholar
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, A. C., & Jost, J. T. (2003). Complementary justice: effects of" poor but happy" and" poor but honest" stereotype exemplars on system justification and implicit activation of the justice motive. Journal of personality and social psychology, 85(5), 823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kinder, D. R., & Kalmoe, N. P. (2017). Neither liberal nor conservative: Ideological innocence in the American public. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Krosnick, J. A., & Berent, M. K. (1993). Comparisons of party identification and policy preferences: The impact of survey question format. American Journal of Political Science, 37(3), 941964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ksiazkiewicz, A., Ludeke, S., & Krueger, R. (2016). The role of cognitive style in the link between genes and political ideology. Political Psychology, 37(6): 761776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, L., Kirshen, P. H., Ruth, M., & Douglas, E. M. (2014). Evacuation as a climate adaptation strategy for environmental justice communities. Climatic Change, 127(3), 493504.Google Scholar
Lieberman, B. (2019). Wildfires and climate change: What’s the connection? Yale Climate Connections. https://yaleclimateconnections.org/2019/07/wildfires-and-climate-change-whats-the-connection/Google Scholar
Leiserowitz, A. A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G., & Rosenthal, S. (2018). Climate change in the American mind. University of Washington.Google Scholar
Lunz Trujillo, K., Motta, M., Callaghan, T., & Sylvester, S. (2021). Correcting misperceptions about the MMR Vaccine: Using psychological risk factors to inform targeted communication strategies. Political Research Quarterly, 74(2), 464478.Google Scholar
Lupia, A. (2016). Uninformed: Why people know so little about politics and what we can do about it. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Malka, A., Krosnick, J. A., & Langer, G. (2009). The association of knowledge with concern about global warming: Trusted information sources shape public thinking. Risk Analysis: An International Journal, 29(5), 633647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marquart-Pyatt, S. T., Shwom, R. L., Dietz, T., Dunlap, R. E., Kaplowitz, S. A., McCright, A. M., & Zahran, S. (2011). Understanding public opinion on climate change: a call for research. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development, 53(4), 3842.Google Scholar
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011). The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 20012010. The Sociological Quarterly, 52(2), 155194.Google Scholar
Mendelberg, T. (2001). The race card. In The Race Card. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merkley, E. (2020). Anti-intellectualism, populism, and motivated resistance to expert consensus. Public Opinion Quarterly, 84(1), 2448.Google Scholar
Merkley, E., & Stecula, D. A. (2018). Party elites or manufactured doubt? The informational context of climate change polarization. Science Communication, 40(2), 258274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merkley, E., & Stecula, D. A. (2021). Party cues in the news: Democratic elites, Republican backlash, and the dynamics of climate skepticism. British Journal of Political Science, 51(4), 14391456.Google Scholar
Milfont, T. L., Milojev, P., & Sibley, C. G. (2016). Values stability and change in adulthood: A 3-year longitudinal study of rank-order stability and mean-level differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(5), 572588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosier, S. L. (2019). Policies as species: Viewing and classifying policy from an evolutionary biology perspective. Politics and the Life Sciences, 38(2), 117131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mostafa, M. M. (2013). Wealth, post-materialism and consumers’ pro-environmental intentions: A multilevel analysis across 25 nations. Sustainable Development, 21(6), 385399.Google Scholar
Motta, M. (2018). The dynamics and political implications of anti-intellectualism in the United States. American Politics Research, 46(3), 465498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motta, M., Chapman, D., Stecula, D., & Haglin, K. (2019). An experimental examination of measurement disparities in public climate change beliefs. Climatic Change, 154(1–2), 3747.Google Scholar
Motta, M. (2020). Could concern about climate change increase demand for a Lyme disease vaccine in the US?. Vaccine, 38(40), 61916193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motta, M., & Goren, P. (2021). Basic human values and compliance with government-recommended prosocial health behavior. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion, & Parties, 31(S1), 206217.Google Scholar
Motta, M., Ralston, R., & Spindel, J. (2021). A call to arms for climate change? How military service member concern about climate change can inform effective climate communication. Environmental Communication, 15(1), 8598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliver, J. E., & Rahn, W. M. (2016). Rise of the Trumpenvolk: Populism in the 2016 election. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 667(1), 189206.Google Scholar
Ostro, B., Rauch, S., Green, R., Malig, B., & Basu, R. (2010). The effects of temperature and use of air conditioning on hospitalizations. American journal of epidemiology, 172(9), 10531061.Google ScholarPubMed
Pachauri, R. K., Allen, M. R., Barros, V. R., Broome, J., Cramer, W., Christ, R., … & van Ypserle, J. P. (2014). Climate change 2014: synthesis report . Contribution of Working Groups I, II, and III to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (p. 151). IPCC.Google Scholar
Peri, C., Rosoff, S., & Yager, J. (2017). Population in the U.S. Floodplains. NYU Furman Center Data Brief. https://furmancenter.org/files/Floodplain_PopulationBrief_12DEC2017.pdfGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C. V., & Sexton, K. (1999). Science and policy implications of defining environmental justice. Journal of Exposure Science & Environmental Epidemiology, 9(1), 917.Google ScholarPubMed
Piurko, Y., Schwartz, S. H., & Davidov, E. (2011). Basic personal values and the meaning of left-right political orientations in 20 countries. Political Psychology, 32(4), 537561.Google Scholar
Rathbun, B. C., Kertzer, J. D., Reifler, J., Goren, P., & Scotto, T. J. (2016). Taking foreign policy personally: Personal values and foreign policy attitudes. International Studies Quarterly, 60(1), 124137.Google Scholar
Rasinski, K. A. (1989). The effect of question wording on public support for government spending. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53(3), 388394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sexton, K., & Adgate, J. L. (1999). Looking at environmental justice from an environmental health perspective. Journal of exposure analysis and environmental epidemiology, 9(1), 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, S. H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture-specifics in the content and structure of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(1), 92116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, L., Chu, E., Anguelovski, I., Aylett, A., Debats, J., Goh, K., … & VanDeveer, S. D. (2016). Roadmap towards justice in urban climate adaptation research. Nature Climate Change, 6(2), 131137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siguad, L. (2018). The National Flood Insurance Program Is Subsidizing Millionaires. Politico. https://www.realclearpolicy.com/articles/2018/10/29/the_national_flood_insurance_program_is_subsidizing_millionaires_110881.htmlGoogle Scholar
Smith, K. B., Oxley, D. R., Hibbing, M. V., Alford, J. R., & Hibbing, J. R. (2011). Linking genetics and political attitudes: Reconceptualizing political ideology. Political Psychology, 32(3), 369397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesler, M. (2012). The spillover of racialization into health care: How President Obama polarized public opinion by racial attitudes and race. American Journal of Political Science, 56(3), 690704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treier, S., & Hillygus, D. S. (2009). The nature of political ideology in the contemporary electorate. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(4), 679703.Google Scholar
Vecchione, M., Schwartz, S., Alessandri, G., Dring, A. K., Castellani, V., & Caprara, M. G. (2016). Stability and change of basic personal values in early adulthood: An 8-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 63, 111122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vecchione, M., Schwartz, S. H., Davidov, E., Cieciuch, J., Alessandri, G., & Marsicano, G. (2020). Stability and change of basic personal values in early adolescence: A 2-year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality, 88(3), 447463.Google ScholarPubMed
Williams, A. E. (2011). Media evolution and public understanding of climate science. Politics and the Life Sciences, 30(2), 2030.Google ScholarPubMed
Winter, N. J. (2008). Dangerous. In Dangerous Frames. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wobus, C., Gutmann, E., Jones, R., Rissing, M., Mizukami, N., Lorie, M., … & Martinich, J. (2017). Climate change impacts on flood risk and asset damages within mapped 100-year floodplains of the contiguous United States. Natural Hazards and Earth System Sciences, 17(12), 21992211.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of items used to measure environmental justice concern and policy orientations.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Psychometric validation of the EJC scale. Panels a and b present category characteristic curves and item information functions for individuals expressing the highest levels of concern about environmental injustice across items. Panel c is a skree plot resulting from principal factor analyses (with varimax rotation). Panel d is a histogram summarizing the distribution of the EJC scale extracted from the graded response modeling procedure summarized in Panels a and b. Please refer to the main text for additional information about these analyses, and to Table 1 for the full question wording associated with each constituent item.

Figure 2

Table 2. The effects of sociopolitical factors and pre-political values on EJC.

Figure 3

Table 3. The effect of EJC and pre-political values on environmental justice policy support.

Supplementary material: PDF

Motta supplementary material

Motta supplementary material

Download Motta supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 136.6 KB