Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-dvmhs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-26T12:56:24.292Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hostile Sexism and Abortion Attitudes in Contemporary American Public Opinion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 May 2023

Anne Cizmar*
Affiliation:
Department of Government, Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY, USA
Kerem Ozan Kalkan
Affiliation:
Department of Government, Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, KY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Anne Cizmar; Email: anne.cizmar@eku.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Abortion is a divisive issue in American politics. Studies analyzing attitudes toward abortion have found that abortion attitudes are relatively stable over time compared with attitudes on other issues and that religiosity and partisanship are key factors influencing abortion attitudes. Recent research has also found a role for benevolent sexism in abortion attitudes. This article expands on the literature and examines the role of hostile sexism—dislike toward women who are seen as usurping men’s authority—in attitudes about abortion in the United States. Using data from the 2012, 2016, and 2020 American National Election Studies, we find that hostile sexism is significantly related to abortion attitudes, even after controlling for theoretically relevant covariates such as partisanship, ideology, religiosity, and sociodemographic variables. As hostile sexism increases, people are more likely to express pro-life attitudes rather than pro-choice attitudes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Women, Gender, and Politics Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Abortion is one of the most divisive issues in contemporary American politics (Alvarez and Brehm Reference Alvarez and Brehm1995). It generates intense emotional reactions from the public and garners substantial attention from government officials, activists, and interest groups (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989; Carsey and Layman Reference Carsey and Layman2006; Jelen and Wilcox Reference Jelen and Wilcox2003; Liebertz and Bunch Reference Liebertz and Bunch2021). The landmark 2022 U.S. Supreme Court decision in Dobbs v. Jackson Women’s Health Organization has again made abortion an electorally salient issue. Many scholars have analyzed abortion attitudes in the United States and abroad. Yet relatively little recent work has been done on abortion attitudes in America, and despite developments in the literature, the relationship between sexism and abortion attitudes needs greater attention.

In this article, we update and expand on the literature that analyzes abortion attitudes in the United States by focusing on how sexism is related to people’s abortion attitudes. In particular, we examine the role of hostile sexism—anger directed at women who are believed to be usurping men’s authority—in predicting abortion attitudes. We demonstrate that people who report “hostile sexist” attitudes toward women are more likely to provide pro-life responses to abortion rather than pro-choice responses.

In the first section of the article, we describe abortion attitudes in the United States, including the salience and polarization of this issue. Then, we describe two forms of sexism—benevolent sexism and hostile sexism—and address how hostile sexism in particular predicts abortion attitudes today in the United States. The following section discusses theoretically relevant predictors of abortion attitudes previously identified in the literature. The article proceeds with a description of the data and measures used for our analysis from the 2012, 2016, and 2020 American National Election Studies (ANES). Finally, we demonstrate that hostile sexism is an important predictor of people’s abortion attitudes using a series of multinomial logit models. This finding holds even after controlling for partisanship, religiosity, religious denomination, sexual orientation, marital status, and demographic covariates. Our article concludes by describing the potential implications of our findings, including the criminalization of abortion, the impact on electoral politics, and state abortion laws.

Opinion on Abortion

Although it is just one issue among many in American politics, abortion is frequently debated and highly salient (Arceneaux Reference Arceneaux2002). This issue again made headlines after the U.S. Supreme Court overturned Roe v. Wade in 2022 with its Dobbs v. Jackson Women’s Health Organization decision. In this decision, the Supreme Court upended legal precedent and determined that the U.S. Constitution does not provide the right to abortion. This paved the way for individual states to pass abortion legislation to severely restrict abortion access, and it set off legal wrangling and attempts by health care providers to understand state provisions on conditions that would qualify for exceptions to the abortion bans.

Despite the recent controversy, research demonstrates that public opinion on abortion is relatively crystallized compared with many other political issues (Adams Reference Adams1997; Converse and Markus Reference Converse and Markus1979; Zaller Reference Zaller1992). Most Americans have attitudes toward abortion (Jelen and Wilcox Reference Jelen and Wilcox2003), and these attitudes are more stable and constrained than attitudes on many other political issues. Scholars consider abortion an “easy issue” (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989) or a “morality issue” (Mooney and Schuldt Reference Mooney and Schuldt2008) because conflict centers around values and there is little room for compromise. It is symbolic of broader, deeper debates about worldviews, the role of women in society, and the meaning of parenthood, sexuality, and gender equality (Jelen Reference Jelen1988, 1–9; Luker Reference Luker1985).

Although this issue is divisive, a majority of the public favors abortion access. According to a 2022 Pew Research Center survey, 61% of Americans say that abortion should be legal in all or most cases. Only 37% say that abortion should be illegal in all or most cases. The Pew data demonstrate that women, younger people, and people with a college degree or higher are all more supportive of abortion rights than their counterparts; however, even among men, people over the age of 65, and those with high school or less education, a majority support abortion rights (Pew Research Center 2022).

The two major political parties are polarized on the issue of abortion as well (Carmines, Gerrity, and Wagner Reference Carmines, Gerrity and Wagner2010; Carsey and Layman Reference Carsey and Layman2006; Jelen and Wilcox Reference Jelen and Wilcox2003; Levendusky Reference Levendusky2009; (Osborne et al. Reference Osborne, Huang, Overall, Sutton, Petterson, Douglas, Davies and Sibley2022). The parties have become more differentiated on abortion over time (Sanbonmatsu Reference Sanbonmatsu2002). The Democrats are aligned with a more “pro-choice” stance and Republicans with a more “pro-life” position (Adams Reference Adams1997). Within the electorate, we see that people have increasingly brought their partisan affiliations into congruence with their issue attitudes, either by changing their partisan affiliation to match their beliefs on key issues or by changing their attitudes on policies to match their partisan affiliation (Carsey and Layman Reference Carsey and Layman2006). As a result, attitudes on abortion are now more partisan than in the past.

Even though abortion attitudes have generated a good deal of political and academic attention over the past 50 years, one aspect that has only recently received scholarly attention is the role of sexism in abortion attitudes. Despite the inherent link between biological sex and abortion, as only people with uteruses can become pregnant, little attention has focused on sexism in predicting abortion attitudes.Footnote 1 In recent years, the literature has begun to explore this connection by analyzing different forms of sexism, particularly benevolent sexism. Even though both men and women can have sexist attitudes, we argue that there is a role for “hostile sexism” in people’s abortion stances. In the next section of this article, we describe two complementary forms of sexism—benevolent and hostile—and the expected association with abortion attitudes.

Forms of Sexism

The literature on sexism in the fields of psychology and sociology is long established. Sexism can be broadly defined as “attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors that support unequal status of women and men” (Swim and Campbell Reference Swim, Campbell, Brown and Gaertner2003, 219).Footnote 2 Sexism is not monolithic, and the literature, particularly in the fields of psychology and sociology, distinguishes between different forms of sexism.

The type of sexism that has most recently been connected to abortion attitudes is “benevolent sexism.” Benevolent sexism is “a subjectively favorable, chivalrous ideology that offers protection and affection to women who embrace conventional roles” (Glick and Fiske Reference Glick and Fiske2001, 109). This type of sexism “is comprised of warm but ultimately patronizing attitudes that revere women as deserving of men’s affection and protection, and as more moral, sensitive and sympathetic than men” (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Sutton, Douglas and McClellan2011, 814). It taps into notions that women are the “better half” or the “nobler sex,” so they cannot have roles in politics or business because they are too good, pure, or weak to serve in these roles. As such, men should be protectors and providers for women, who should occupy a different space than men because of their gentler constitutions (Glick and Fiske Reference Glick and Fiske1996). Benevolent sexism views motherhood as a natural role for women, so abortion would not fit with that role.

The other complementary form of sexism identified by Glick and Fiske (Reference Glick and Fiske1996) is “hostile sexism,” which can be defined as “hostility toward women who oppose traditional roles” (Travaglia, Overall, and Sibley Reference Travaglia, Overall and Sibley2009, 599). Traditional roles for women have historically included the roles of wife, mother, and homemaker. “Hostile sexism is a negatively valenced cluster of attitudes toward women that reviles them as manipulative, devious, complaining, and competitive with men” (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Sutton, Douglas and McClellan2011, 814). It taps into feelings that women take pleasure in creating problems for men. It is a “combative ideology that is hostile toward women who challenge traditional patterns and are seen as seeking to control men, either by using their sexuality or feminist ideology” (Thomae and Houston Reference Thomae and Houston2016, 152). Those reporting hostile sexist views believe women tease men regarding sex, they complain in the workplace, and they are critical of men (Glick and Fiske Reference Glick and Fiske1996).

Additionally, other types of sexism are identified in the literature, including overt sexism and covert sexism, modern sexism, and neosexism.Footnote 3 These forms of sexism may be connected to abortion attitudes as well. We focus on the role of hostile sexism in particular, for three reasons. First, hostile sexism is the complement to benevolent sexism in the overarching theory of “ambivalent sexism” identified by Glick and Fiske (Reference Glick and Fiske1996). As Glick and Fiske describe, benevolent sexism is chivalrous or paternalistic behavior toward women who conform to traditional roles; hostile sexism is antipathy toward women who are seen as taking power. We argue that both have a role in attitudes toward abortion.

Second, publicly available data include measures of hostile sexism but not measures of covert sexism, implicit sexism, and so on.Footnote 4 The data for complementary hypothesis testing are not available in other publicly available sources such as the General Social Survey, ANES, and other surveys. Finally, despite attempts to frame abortion restrictions as “pro-woman,” we argue that hostile sexism is still an important predictor of abortion attitudes. Although laws limiting abortion rights are often couched in terms of protecting women, an important component of anti-abortion legislation is controlling behavior. It is the last point on abortion framing versus attitudes that we focus on in the next section.Footnote 5

Sexism and Framing of Abortion

The framing of the abortion debate in recent years demonstrates a strategic movement toward a “pro-woman” (Roberti Reference Roberti2021) or “woman-protective” anti-abortion frame (Donley and Lens Reference Donley and Lens2021; Siegel Reference Siegel2007) among abortion opponents. To advance legislation against abortion, abortion opponents have moved from a pro-fetus or morality focus to a pro-woman frame. The pro-woman frame “situates abortion as inherently harmful for women—both psychologically and physically—and insists that women deserve better than abortion” (Roberti Reference Roberti2021, 207). The laws treat “women as an uninformed group in need of protection from their own ignorance and the practices of abortion providers” (Doan and Schwarz Reference Doan and Schwarz2020, 6). By promoting “regulatory abortion bills that ‘educate’ women about abortion or protect them from potential harm of abortion, anti-abortion lawmakers may claim they are advocates for women” (Roberti Reference Roberti2021, 207). Laws that require ultrasounds, waiting periods, mandatory “counseling” sessions, and “informed consent” for pregnant people before they can obtain an abortion all incorporate this type of woman-protective frame when trying to limit access to abortion services. The laws are “presented as protecting women from coercion and from making poor decisions” (Denbow Reference Denbow2015, 97). We would also mention that these types of frames are consistent with the views of conservative women’s organizations, such as Concerned Women for America and the Independent Women’s Forum (Schreiber Reference Schreiber2002). These organizations, and women who identify as conservative, oppose abortion specifically because they view abortions to be harmful to women (Schreiber Reference Schreiber2002, Reference Schreiber2008).

This “frame extension” has been adopted by those pursuing anti-abortion policies in the United States in “order to attract new adherents to their cause. While the dominant rhetoric of the movement today focuses on the fetus and the immorality of abortion, pro-woman rhetoric has reemerged that leverages both the language of women’s rights as well as science in order to propose that abortion is bad for women” (Rose Reference Rose2011, 1). State laws now regularly incorporate the pro-woman frame (Roberti Reference Roberti2021). As a result of this strategic framing of the abortion issue, it makes sense that benevolent sexism is related to abortion attitudes in the United States. Political activists have worked to create a palatable anti-abortion message that sounds supportive of women rather than hostile toward women. The strategic messaging focuses on women as potential victims of bad influences who need protection and education. This aligns well with gender portrayals under benevolent sexism, which emphasize women’s traditional roles as wives and mothers (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Davies, Sibley and Osborne2016; Osborne et al. Reference Osborne, Huang, Overall, Sutton, Petterson, Douglas, Davies and Sibley2022) and women as the weaker sex who need protection. Under this view, it is not necessarily misogyny that makes people less pro-choice, but rather the beliefs that women are natural caretakers and should embrace their roles as mothers. Sexism plays role in abortion beliefs through attitudes toward motherhood (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Davies, Sibley and Osborne2016). To go against this “natural” role would be harmful to women.

Creating a “pro-woman” frame is a calculated attempt to repackage the abortion debate. “This frame transformation represents a strategic tactic of anti-abortion legislators to soften political behavior and beliefs that are seen as hostile toward women, especially during a time where there is an unprecedented amount of anti-abortion bills introduced in the states” (Roberti Reference Roberti2021, 207). Yet, despite the pro-woman framing efforts in the abortion debate, there is an underlying power conflict surrounding abortion. Who gets to make decisions about abortion—when, where, and under what circumstances one can be obtained—is also about power and control.

Women who can make reproductive choices independently may be seen as challenging traditional power structures and threatening the power of men. This piece connects to hostile sexism, as “hostile sexism is central to the perception that it is ok for men to impose these restrictions” (Petterson and Sutton Reference Petterson and Sutton2018, 244). As Petterson and Sutton (Reference Petterson and Sutton2018, 235) show, “hostile sexism is associated with perceptions that men have the right to constrain women’s reproductive choices.” Hostile sexism, and the ability of men to control women, can be a predictor of abortion attitudes, regardless of pro-woman frames. Since “hostile sexism uncovers antipathy toward women,” Gothreau, Arceneaux and Friesen (Reference Gothreau, Arceneaux and Friesen2022, 2) also find that “hostile sexism predicts less support for abortion and birth control access, as well as funding for Planned Parenthood.” Petterson and Sutton (Reference Petterson and Sutton2018, 236) note that despite the role of hostile sexism in abortion attitudes, those opposed to abortion “seldom describe their position as being motivated to preserve male control over women. More typically, they couch their position as a desire to protect the fetus, and often the pregnant woman herself.” Petterson and Sutton provide important theoretical contributions to the study of sexism’s relationship to abortion attitudes using a sample of undergraduate psychology students in the United Kingdom and Amazon Mechanical Turk workers. Our article expands on this work in the U.S. context, examines a longer timeline, and uses a nationally representative sample.

It would be ideal if the available data also included measures for benevolent sexism so that both could be included in the model. Unfortunately, that is not the case. The available ANES measures tap into hostile sexism. Recent research on other aspects of electoral politics have explored the importance of hostile sexism and gender stereotypes, particularly in vote choice. Although both types of sexism are related to people’s votes in the 2016 U.S. presidential election, it is hostile sexism in particular that “is the basis for polarization, with those high in hostile sexism (men and women both) resisting female leadership” (Winter Reference Winter2022, 22). Cassese and Barnes (Reference Cassese and Barnes2018) find that many White women espouse sexist opinions and that a woman’s level of sexism is a strong predictor of voting for Donald Trump in 2016. Similarly, hostile sexism is related to vote choice in Australia’s 2019 federal election (Beauregard Reference Beauregard2021). Hostile sexism has been shown to create “an attitudinal barrier—especially for conservatives—to supporting equal pay for women” (Connor and Fiske Reference Connor and Fiske2019, 22). It is a predictor of tolerance toward sexual harassment (Russell and Trigg Reference Russell and Trigg2004) and opposition to breastfeeding in public (Huang, Sibley, and Osborne Reference Huang, Sibley and Osborne2020). Other recent work demonstrates the importance of attitudes toward women and the role of sexism and gender stereotypes in contemporary American public opinion (Cassese and Barnes Reference Cassese and Barnes2018; Kam and Archer Reference Kam and Allison2021; McThomas and Tesler Reference McThomas and Tesler2016; Valentino, Wayne, and Oceno Reference Valentino, Wayne and Oceno2018).

Although the framing of the abortion debate might tap into benevolent sexism, we argue that at the attitudinal level, it might manifest as hostile sexism as well. Benevolent sexism is more patronizing and paternalistic—having babies is good for women because it fits with their nature, according to this view. Women should have this explained to them so that they know not to have abortions. Hostile sexism would extend this opposition to abortion because women making reproductive choices independently potentially takes control away from men. Under this view, abortion opponents may not only believe that abortion is bad for women because it goes against their natural roles as mothers, but also oppose abortion because of their antipathy toward women and fear of women seeking power. Opposition to abortion is also related to their dislike of women and ability to take control. Our article attempts to capture the hostile dimension of sexism in its association with abortion attitudes in public opinion while acknowledging the role of benevolent sexism and its framing.

Other Predictors of Abortion Attitudes

This study focuses on the role of hostile sexism and attitudes toward women. However, it is worth noting that research has identified other key predictors of abortion attitudes. One of the focal points of earlier abortion attitudes analysis is religion. Research on abortion attitudes demonstrates that religious considerations are important predictors of abortion beliefs. Both religious tradition and level of religiosity are related to people’s abortion attitudes (Combs and Welch Reference Combs and Welch1982; Jelen Reference Jelen1988). This is a dominant frame when discussing abortion attitudes both within the literature and within public discourse.

We control for level of religiosity in our models. The variable is constructed by combining worship attendance frequency, views of the Bible, and importance of religion (Layman Reference Layman2001) (Cronbach’s α ranges between 0.76 and 0.82). We also separately control for religious tradition by including dichotomous variables for people who are Catholic, evangelical Protestant, or Jewish. Past research has shown that people who identify as Catholic or evangelical are more pro-life in their abortion attitudes than mainline Protestants. According to a recent YouGov analysis, Jewish people are the most pro-choice religious group, after atheists and agnostics (Ballard Reference Ballard2022).

As discussed in the first section of this article, another important predictor of abortion attitudes is party affiliation. Research on the role of party identification on abortion attitudes has demonstrated a clear link, with Democrats identifying predominantly as pro-choice and Republicans identifying predominantly as pro-life (Adams Reference Adams1997). The parties are clearly differentiated in their abortion stances (Carmines, Gerrity, and Wagner Reference Carmines, Gerrity and Wagner2010; Carsey and Layman Reference Carsey and Layman2006; Jelen and Wilcox Reference Jelen and Wilcox2003; Levendusky Reference Levendusky2009; Osborne et al. Reference Osborne, Huang, Overall, Sutton, Petterson, Douglas, Davies and Sibley2022; Sanbonmatsu Reference Sanbonmatsu2002). Over time, “the cumulative effect of abortion attitudes led pro-life Democrats and pro-choice Republicans to switch parties” so that people’s abortion attitudes align with party identification (Killian and Wilcox Reference Killian and Wilcox2008, 561). Our models also control for party identification and ideology.

Previous scholarship finds significant differences in abortion attitudes based on sociodemographic characteristics. Swank (Reference Swank2021) finds that gender, marital status, and social class are related to people’s opinion on abortion. Research also demonstrates that sexual identity or orientation can be linked to abortion attitudes (Worthen Reference Worthen2020), with lesbian, gay, and bisexual people more likely to favor access to abortion (Grollman Reference Grollman2017, Reference Grollman2019).

It is important to note that the research regarding gender’s link to abortion attitudes has presented less unified findings. Some scholars find that gender and attitudes about gender roles are not strong predictors of attitudes on abortion, or at least do not exert a direct influence on abortion attitudes (Jelen and Wilcox Reference Jelen and Wilcox2003). More recently, however, scholars have argued that when religiosity is appropriately controlled for within models, women are more supportive of abortion rights than men (Lizotte Reference Lizotte2015). Religiosity can be a “suppressor variable” that masks the gender difference in abortion attitudes between men and women (Barkan Reference Barkan2014). Social identities can interact with each other to predict abortion attitudes in different ways.

Earlier studies find that White Americans, on average, provide more permissive responses on abortion than Black Americans (Combs and Welch Reference Combs and Welch1982; Hall and Ferree Reference Hall and Ferree1986). However, recent survey findings suggest that Black Americans and Asian Americans are more pro-choice than White Americans (Pew Research Center 2022). Other scholars also find complex interactions between gender, race/ethnicity, and religiosity. For example, among Latino men, evangelicalism and worship attendance are associated with less support for abortion (Holman, Podrazik, and Mohamed Reference Holman, Podrazik and Mohamed2020).

Education has also been demonstrated to be a predictor of abortion attitudes. People who are less educated are more likely to take the pro-life stance (Blake Reference Blake1971; Tedrow and Mahoney Reference Tedrow and Mahoney1979) than those with more education. The reasons people take pro-life stances might vary based on education, too; Jelen (Reference Jelen1988) demonstrates that people who are less educated are more pro-life because they believe that abortion will foster sexual promiscuity, whereas more highly educated people take pro-life stances because of concerns over respect for life.

Because of the established significance of covariates including religiosity, religious tradition, party identification, race, age, education, sexual orientation, and marital status, we include controls for all of these factors in the models presented in the next section, along with the measure of hostile sexism.Footnote 6 All variables are coded to range from 0 to 1, unless otherwise noted.

Data on Hostile Sexism and Abortion Attitudes

We use 2012, 2016, and 2020 ANES data to test our hypothesis. ANES conducted between 4,270 and 15,729 interviews (pre- and post-election) across the years. Table 1 shows that the surveys were conducted both online and face-to-face across these years. The American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR) Response Rate 1 ranges between 36.7% and 50% depending on the year and modality. The AAPOR Response Rate 3 was 49% in 2012 and 40.9% in 2020.

Table 1. American National Election Studies information

Notes: We would like to thank one of the reviewers for encouraging us to report on these essential features of the ANES.

Source: http://www.electionstudies.org.

These publicly available surveys provide an opportunity to test the relationship between sexism and contemporary abortion attitudes. Previously, surveys were inconsistent in including measures tapping into sexist attitudes (Schaffner Reference Schaffner2022). The 2016 ANES has the most comprehensive battery of questions tapping into hostile sexism. These include questions asking respondents whether women interpret innocent remarks as sexist, whether women fail to appreciate what men do for them, whether women seek power to control men, whether women try to keep men on a tight leash, whether women who ask for equality are seeking special favors, and whether women complaining about discrimination create more problems than they solve. These are all components of the hostile sexism index identified by Glick and Fiske (Reference Glick and Fiske1996). The specific question wording can be found in the online appendix. We created an index out these variables to measure hostile sexism in 2016 (Cronbach’s α = 0.79)

The 2020 ANES includes several of these items, including whether women interpret innocent remarks as sexist, whether women seek power to control men, whether women who ask for equality are seeking special favors, and whether women complaining about discrimination create problems. These are all components of the hostile sexism index. We created an index of hostile sexism using these variables in 2020 (Cronbach’s α = 0.72)

The 2012 ANES also includes a few of the same measures for hostile sexism, although there are only two measures available in 2012. Despite this limitation, we also include analysis of the 2012 ANES here. The two items do not assess the same breadth and depth of hostile sexism as the six measures in 2016, but we believe it is important to include all available years to demonstrate that hostile sexism is important for abortion attitudes not just during the Trump presidential campaigns. This is a more conservative test of our hypothesis, in that we analyze using a weaker measure, in a year when gender dynamics were not as pronounced. The 2012 measures are whether women who ask for equality are seeking special favors and whether women complaining about discrimination create more problems than they solve. Similarly, we created an index out of these two variables to measure hostile sexism in 2012 (Cronbach’s α = 0.57).

For all three years of data, the hostile sexism indices are coded so that higher values indicate the more sexist view.Footnote 7 The measures available for hostile sexism are somewhat different across the three years, but findings demonstrate support for our hypothesis that people who espouse more hostile sexist views are less supportive of abortion rights. Please refer to the online appendix for the specific question wording of hostile sexism items across years.Footnote 8

In addition to the measures of hostile sexism, the ANES also includes attitudes toward discrimination faced by women in the United States in all three surveys. As part of the battery about groups that might be discriminated against, the ANES asks how much discrimination women face. Following the modeling strategy of Cassese and Barnes (Reference Cassese and Barnes2018), who also examine the role of hostile sexism using 2012 and 2016 ANES data, we include the discrimination against women variable as a control covariate in our models. We coded the variable so that the higher responses indicate an increasing belief in discrimination against women in the United States. The specific question wording is available in the online appendix.

The abortion attitudes question is asked in 2012, 2016, and 2020 in the format that has been used by the ANES for decades. It is a four-category scale ranging from purely pro-life attitudes to purely pro-choice attitudes—abortion should never be permitted or should always be permitted, regardless of the circumstances surrounding the abortion. The middle two categories acknowledge that abortion should be permissible in a given set of circumstances. One of the middle categories includes allowance for abortion in cases of rape, incest, or danger to the woman’s life. The other middle category, referred to as the “clear need” option, indicates that a woman should be able to obtain an abortion for other reasons, too, so long as a clear need has been established.Footnote 9

We reversed the ordering of the response options in our analyses so that higher response options refer to more pro-life attitudes. The specific question wording and response options are available in the online appendix.

Analysis of Abortion Attitudes

Based on our theoretical framework, we have one main hypothesis: higher values of hostile sexism will be associated with a higher probability of pro-life abortion attitudes as opposed to pro-choice abortion attitudes. Our major focus is the comparison of pure pro-choice and pure pro-life response options in abortion attitudes. We also interpret the findings for other response options. We expect the relationship to hold after controlling for multiple theoretically relevant variables.

Bivariate analyses between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes between 2012 and 2020 provide preliminary support for our hypothesis. We computed pairwise correlation coefficient estimates between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes for each year. The plot of the estimates can be seen in Figure 1. The trend shows a growing association between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes over time. Those who score high on the hostile sexism scale tend to be more pro-life, with the trend increasing between 2012 and 2020. We also computed 95% confidence intervals around these estimates by using bootstrapped standard errors. We find that the strength of association between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes has grown since 2012 at statistically significant levels. After 2016, the association stays around the same level in 2020.Footnote 10

Figure 1. Bivariate relationship between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes over time.

The bivariate analyses show a growing association between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes between 2012 and 2020. However, they do not control for theoretically relevant variables. In the next sections of the article, we explain our modeling rationale and present multivariate findings that show further empirical support for our main hypothesis.

Although the abortion attitudes variable is somewhat ordered in terms of permissiveness toward abortion, we argue that it is a nominal variable. The response options do not monotonically increase or decrease in one direction. There is a clear difference between the pure pro-choice and pure pro-life options—and our hypothesis mainly concerns these responses. However, the ordering might be vague in the case of the clear need and rape/incest/danger options. Hence, we treat the abortion attitudes variable as an unordered covariate and run multinomial logit models for 2012, 2016, and 2020.

It is important to note that a multinomial logit model is essentially “equivalent to a series of pairwise logit models,” and its coefficient estimates are “interpreted in the same way as binary logit model parameters are interpreted, with comparison being with the base category” (Cameron and Trivedi Reference Cameron and Trivedi2022, 910). Although interpretations are identical, running a “series of binary logits is not optimal” due to changes in the sample sizes (Long and Freese Reference Long and Freese2014, 387). Therefore, we run multinomial logit models in which picking the pure pro-choice option served as the comparison category, unless otherwise indicated. The multinomial logit coefficient estimates in each model show the probability of picking a particular response option as opposed to the pure pro-choice option. Our focus is the comparison of the pure pro-choice and pure pro-life response options to test our main hypothesis.

The models use full samples (online and face-to-face) in all years, and the estimates are computed by using complex survey weights.Footnote 11 The multinomial logit model results from 2012, 2016, and 2020 are presented in Figures 2, 3, and 4, respectively.Footnote 12 The comparison category for the top-left and two bottom figures (pure pro-life, clear need, and rape/incest/danger options) is the pure pro-choice option. The comparison category for the top-right figure (pure pro-choice) is the pure pro-life option. The circles refer to the multinomial logit coefficient estimates, and the spikes are the 95% confidence intervals. If the confidence interval spike does not overlap 0, the coefficient estimate is statistically significant.Footnote 13

Figure 2. Abortion attitudes and hostile sexism in 2012.

Figure 3. Abortion attitudes and hostile sexism in 2016.

Figure 4. Abortion attitudes and hostile sexism in 2020.

Findings

Figure 2 shows that hostile sexism predicts abortion attitudes in the 2012 ANES at statistically significant levels for almost all response options. We find strong support for our hypothesis in 2012: people who score higher on hostile sexism are more likely to have a pure pro-life stance on abortion as opposed to a pure pro-choice stance. They are also more likely to pick the rape/incest/danger option as opposed to the pure pro-choice option. We find no statistically significant difference in the probability of picking the clear need option over the pure pro-choice option in 2012. Overall, hostile sexist attitudes are significantly related to attitudes on abortion, with higher levels of hostile sexism related to more support for pro-life attitudes as opposed to pro-choice attitudes. The finding stands even controlling for opinions on discrimination against women, level of religiosity, religious denomination, partisanship, ideology, sexual orientation, marital status, and other variables found to be significant in past research.

Figure 3 presents findings from the 2016 multinomial logit model. Similarly, the results provide a strong empirical support for our main hypothesis. People who score higher on hostile sexism are more likely to be pure pro-life as opposed to be pure pro-choice on abortion attitudes, controlling for other variables. They are also more likely to express support for the two middle categories (clear need and rape/incest/danger) compared with the pure pro-choice category, on average. Hostile sexist attitudes are significantly related to attitudes on abortion in 2016.

The significant findings in the clear need and rape/incest/danger categories may be attributable to two things. First, the measure in 2016 includes six items (instead of two), and it is a more comprehensive measure covering more of the depth and breadth of hostile sexism. Second, the dynamics of the 2016 election highlighted attitudes about women in ways not seen in 2012. Particularly, Hillary Clinton was running as the Democratic nominee, the first woman ever to obtain a major-party nomination for the presidency. Simultaneously, Donald Trump was running as the Republican nominee and faced questions about his attitudes toward and treatment of women for his Access Hollywood tape comments.

Finally, the 2020 model provides more recent empirical support for our hypothesis regarding the relationship between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes. Figure 4 demonstrates that those who score high on hostile sexism are more likely to pick the pure pro-life position as opposed to the pure pro-choice position, controlling for other theoretically relevant variables. Similarly, they are more likely to choose the rape/incest/danger option rather than the pure pro-choice option. As in 2012, the only category that fails to show a statistically significant difference is the clear need clause. This might indicate that respondents have a hard time distinguishing their attitudes between the pure pro-choice and clear need options. Nevertheless, the model findings demonstrate remarkable empirical support for our main hypothesis.

To provide a more substantive interpretation of the multinomial logit results, we computed average marginal effects. The average marginal effect refers to the change in the probability of picking each response option rather than others when there is a one-unit instantaneous change in the hostile sexism variable (for details on the interpretation of average marginal effects, see Cameron and Trivedi Reference Cameron and Trivedi2022; Long and Freese Reference Long and Freese2014). We computed the average marginal effects by holding all other variables at their observed values (Hanmer and Kalkan Reference Hanmer and Kalkan2013). The findings are presented in Figure 5. The hollow circles refer to the change in the probability of picking a particular abortion attitude response option rather than the other options as hostile sexism increases by one unit, on average. The spikes refer to the 95% confidence intervals computed by the delta method. If a spike does not overlap 0, it refers to a statistically significant change.

Figure 5. Average marginal effects of hostile sexism in 2012, 2016, and 2020.

Figure 5 shows that as hostile sexism increases by one unit, on average, the probability of picking the pure pro-choice option as opposed to the other options declines in 2012, 2016, and 2020. The decline is statistically significant in all years. We observe that the probability of picking the pure pro-life response option as opposed to other options increases as hostile sexism also increases by one unit, on average. The change is statistically significant in 2016 and 2020 and barely misses the threshold in 2012. A similar relationship is also found in picking the rape/incest/danger clause as opposed to the others. Growing hostile sexism is associated with an increase in the probability of picking this option rather than the others, and the change is statistically significant in 2012 and 2016. The figure corroborates what the coefficient estimates display in regard to the absence of a systematic link between hostile sexism attitudes and the probability of picking the clear need response option.

It is important to note that there is also a strong relationship between religiosity and people’s attitudes on abortion. People who are more religious are more likely to provide pure pro-life, rape/incest/danger, or clear need responses compared with pure pro-choice responses in 2012, 2016, and 2020. People who are highly religious are less likely to provide pure pro-choice responses than pro-life responses. This fits with the findings in the literature. Religious traditions provide a series of mixed findings, except for Jewish people, who express more pro-choice attitudes compared with other traditions in the model in some years.Footnote 14

We do not find a statistically significant relationship between people’s opinions on the level of discrimination against women and picking the pro-life option rather than the pro-choice option, except for 2020. In all years, Republicans and conservatives are less likely to report pro-choice attitudes than pro-life attitudes, on average. Non-Latinx White Americans do not differ from other respondents in their abortion attitudes in most cases. People who are more highly educated are more likely to provide pure pro-choice attitudes on abortion rather than pure pro-life attitudes. In general, younger people are also more likely to choose the pure pro-choice stance compared with the pure pro-life stance. Neither the marital status nor the sexual orientation variables reach consistent levels of statistical significance in our models predicting pure pro-life attitudes as opposed to pure pro-choice attitudes.

In sum, the findings provide strong empirical support for our main hypothesis. Hostile sexism predicts pure pro-life abortion attitudes at statistically significant levels in 2012, 2016, and 2020. Those who score high on the hostile sexism scale tend to consider themselves more pro-life as opposed to pro-choice. The link stays robust when controlling for theoretically relevant variables. The findings empirically support the expectation that contemporary abortion attitudes in the United States have empirically undeniable sexist roots.

Conclusion

Women and the abortion issue are inextricably linked in the sense that only people with a uterus can bear children for society. Political discourse does focus heavily on the role of babies and the unborn when discussing abortion and educating women to make the “right” choice. However, there is an important role in abortion attitudes about the power to make decisions about when, how, and under what circumstances women can obtain an abortion. American politics research on this topic does not fully consider how attitudes toward women, particularly women who are seen as trying to usurp men’s authority by making reproductive decisions independently, may relate to attitudes toward abortion. Most of the earlier political science literature focuses on the role of partisanship, religion, or religiosity, with a conspicuous disregard for whether and how sexism predicts abortion attitudes. We believe that our article contributes to filling this gap in the literature.

Using the 2012, 2016, and 2020 ANES, we analyze the relationship between hostile sexist attitudes and abortion views and find that hostile sexism is significantly related to picking the pure pro-life option rather than the pure pro-choice option. People who espouse hostile sexist views are less likely to express pro-choice attitudes on abortion. This is empirically supported even when controlling for other relevant covariates identified in the literature, including measures of partisan identification, religion, and religiosity.

There are at least two empirical limitations to our study. First, our analysis uses publicly available data sets with limited availability of different sexism measures. For example, we are unable to longitudinally test the link between different forms of sexism and abortion attitudes. Second, our findings do not make causal claims about the relationship between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes. Observational studies, such as ours, report correlational findings. This is why we paid particular attention to our choice of words and avoided causal language. We believe there is a need for more experimental designs to reveal the nature of the link between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes.

Abortion continues to be a divisive political topic. Based on recent events, such as the Dobbs decision, which provides an opportunity for states to greatly limit abortion access, the issue may be even more salient in elections to come. Our article contributes to the literature both on the importance of sexism in political discussions and on abortion attitudes. The evolving discourse on the abortion issue, toward increased discussion of criminalizing abortion and punishing women who seek abortions, makes it critical to examine the role of hostile sexism in abortion attitudes. While there are many factors that have led to changing discourse surrounding abortion laws, in 2016, when then candidate Trump stated that “there has to be some form of punishment” for women who seek abortions, his statement was met with resistance by leaders from both the pro-life and pro-choice sides, since it was a departure from prior messaging on abortion.

Since the Dobbs decision, though, states are considering previously unprecedented steps to block abortion, including fines and prison time for people who end their pregnancies, classifying abortion as homicide, and laws that ban “abortion trafficking”—taking minors across state lines to obtain an abortion. Providing empirical assessment about the role of hostile sexism in this evolving issue contributes to our understanding of abortion attitudes today.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X23000260.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the editor and anonymous reviewers for their helpful guidance on both the theoretical and methodological aspects of our article. An earlier version of this article was presented at the Department of Government Research Workshop and the 2019 Midwest Political Science Association Annual Conference. We would like to thank Dr. Sara Zeigler for her comments and feedback on an earlier version of the article. Anne Cizmar would like to acknowledge the sabbatical leave she received during the spring 2020 semester, which significantly contributed to the completion of this article.

Footnotes

1. It is important to recognize that gender is not binary. People can become pregnant even if they do not identify as women. Some people who identify as women cannot become pregnant. Based on the historical context of the abortion issue framing and available data measures, we discuss attitudes toward women and their association with abortion attitudes while acknowledging the importance of gender identity/expression.

2. It is important to note that men can also be affected by sexist attitudes or behavior. Historically, women have been more greatly affected by their unequal status in the United States, and most of the literature evaluating sexism examines anti-female sexism. However, this is not the only possible type of sexism.

3. See Campbell, Schellenberg, and Senn (Reference Campbell, Schellenberg and Senn1997) and Swim and Cohen (Reference Swim and Cohen1997) for more information on other forms of sexism.

4. The 2016 ANES includes two components of the Modern Sexism Scale (Swim and Cohen Reference Swim and Cohen1997) among the measures on attitudes toward women. These measures are not available in other years of the ANES. As a robustness check, we estimated the 2016 models including the Modern Sexism measures that are distinct from hostile sexism. These are the questions about how much attention the media should play to discrimination against women and women who complain about discrimination cause more problems than they solve. Even when controlling for these measures of modern sexism, hostile sexism is still significant in predicting abortion attitudes.

5. We would like to thank the editor and reviewers for helpful suggestions on framing that strengthened the article’s theoretical development.

6. See the online appendix for details on the measurement and coding details of these variables.

7. See Schaffner (Reference Schaffner2022) for an excellent assessment of different ways of measuring sexist attitudes in the United States. The study recommends a reduced version of hostile sexism items to measure prejudice and resentment toward women.

8. Unfortunately, the ANES does not include data for 2012–20 on other measures of sexism. Including measures for other forms of sexism, such as benevolent sexism, would be ideal, but this is not possible with the available data.

9. These options for abortion attitudes are limited, particularly when considering a global perspective on abortion views. The ANES data provide four categories for attitudes on abortion, but the ANES does not specifically distinguish between “traumatic” abortions or “elective” abortions in that type of terminology. Research demonstrates that “traumatic” abortion, in which the woman’s health would be compromised or the abortion was the result of rape, is more supported than “elective” abortion, such as cases of financial insecurity or when the woman does not want the child (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Davies, Sibley and Osborne2016). It is not possible to consider this distinction with regard to abortion attitudes in the ANES.

10. We would like to thank one of the reviewers for encouraging us to conduct this bivariate analysis.

11. To test for multicollinearity, we computed variance inflation factors (VIF) by using a linear model. None of the VIFs in our main models reached concerning levels that would indicate multicollinearity. We would like to thank one of the reviewers for giving us the opportunity to report on multicollinearity.

12. Using the complex survey weights does not permit testing for the independence of irrelevant alternatives. We estimated unweighted models, and the Small-Hsiao (Reference Small and Hsiao1985) test shows that all models satisfy the assumption. The Hausman test, on the other hand, provides inconsistent support for the assumption. As a robustness check, we ran separate logit models in which the pure pro-life option is compared with the pure pro-choice option—our main hypothesis. The findings corroborate the multinomial logit findings presented in this article. The results of these logit models are reported in Tables A4, A5, and A6 in the appendix.

13. See Tables A1, A2, and A3 in the appendix for coefficient estimates and standard errors of the multinomial logit models.

14. We also ran a series of interactive models testing the link between religiosity/religious tradition interaction and abortion attitudes. The findings regarding our main hypothesis remain the same. See Tables A7, A8, and A9 in the appendix for the results.

References

Adams, Greg D. 1997. “Abortion: Evidence of Issue Evolution.” American Journal of Political Science 41 (3): 718–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alvarez, R. Michael, and Brehm, John. 1995. “It’s Abortion, Stupid: Policy Voting in the 1992 Presidential Election.” American Journal of Political Science 39 (4): 1055–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, Kevin. 2002. “Direct Democracy and the Link between Public Opinion and State Abortion Policy.” State Politics & Policy Quarterly 2 (4): 372–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, Jamie. 2022. “Which Religious Groups’ Members are Most Likely to Identify as Pro- Choice?” YouGov, May 13. https://today.yougov.com/topics/politics/articles-reports/2022/05/13/which-religious-groups-members-abortion-poll (accessed December 31, 2022).Google Scholar
Barkan, Steven E. 2014. “Gender and Abortion Attitudes: Religiosity as a Suppressor Variable.” Public Opinion Quarterly 78 (4): 940–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauregard, Katrine. 2021. “Sexism and the Australian Voter: How Sexist Attitudes Influenced Vote Choice in the 2019 Federal Election.” Australian Journal of Political Science 56 (3): 298317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, Judith. 1971. “Abortion and Public Opinion: The 1960–1970 Decade.” Science 171 (3971): 540–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, A. Colin, and Trivedi, Pravin K.. 2022. Microeconometrics Using Stata. 2nd ed. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, Bernadette, Schellenberg, E. Glenn, and Senn, Charlene Y.. 1997. “Evaluating Measures of Contemporary Sexism.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 21 (1): 89102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmines, Edward G., Gerrity, Jessica C., and Wagner, Michael W.. 2010. “How Abortion Became a Partisan Issue: Media Coverage of the Interest Group-Political Party Connection.” Politics & Policy 38 (6): 1135–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmines, Edward G., and Stimson, James A.. 1989. Issue Evolution: Race and the Transformation of American Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carsey, Thomas M., and Layman, Geoffrey C.. 2006. “Changing Sides or Changing Minds? Party Identification and Policy Preferences in the American Electorate.” American Journal of Political Science 50 (2): 464–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassese, Erin C., and Barnes, Tiffany D.. 2018. “Reconciling Sexism and Women’s Support for Republican Candidates: A Look at Gender, Class, and Whiteness in the 2012 and 2016 Presidential Races.” Political Behavior 41 (3): 677700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Combs, Michael W., and Welch, Susan. 1982. “Blacks, Whites, and Attitudes toward Abortion.” Public Opinion Quarterly 46 (4): 510–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, Rachel A., and Fiske, Susan T.. 2019. “Not Minding the Gap: How Hostile Sexism Encourages Choice Explanations for the Gender Income Gap.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 43 (1): 2236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Converse, Philip E., and Markus, Gregory B.. 1979. “Plus ça Change: The New CPS Election Study Panel.” American Political Science Review 73 (1): 3249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denbow, Jennifer M. 2015. Governed through Choice: Autonomy, Technology, and the Politics of Reproduction. New York: New York University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doan, Alesha E., and Schwarz, Corinne. 2020. “Father Knows Best: ‘Protecting’ Women through State Surveillance and Social Control in Anti-abortion Policy.” Politics & Policy 48 (1): 637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donley, Greer, and Lens, Jill Wieber. 2021. “Second-Trimester Abortion Dangertalk.” Boston College Law Review 62 (7): 21452208.Google Scholar
Glick, Peter, and Fiske, Susan T.. 1996. “The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating Hostile and Benevolent Sexism.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 (3): 491512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glick, Peter, and Fiske, Susan T.. 2001. “An Ambivalent Alliance: Hostile and Benevolent Sexism as Complementary Justifications for Gender Inequality.” American Psychologist 56 (2): 109–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gothreau, Claire, Arceneaux, Kevin, and Friesen, Amanda. 2022. “Hostile, Benevolent, Implicit: How Different Shades of Sexism Impact Gendered Policy Attitudes.” Frontiers in Political Science 4: 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grollman, Eric Anthony. 2017. “Sexual Orientation Differences in Attitudes about Sexuality, Race, and Gender.” Social Science Research 61: 126–41.Google Scholar
Grollman, Eric Anthony. 2019. “Americans’ Gender Attitudes at the Intersection of Sexual Orientation and Gender.” Journal of Homosexuality 66 (2): 141–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Hall, Elaine J., and Ferree, Myra Marx. 1986. “Race Differences in Abortion Attitudes.” Public Opinion Quarterly 50 (2): 193207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanmer, Michael J., and Kalkan, Kerem Ozan. 2013. “Behind the Curve: Clarifying the Best Approach to Calculating Predicted Probabilities and Marginal Effects from Limited Dependent Variable Models.” American Journal of Political Science 57 (1): 263–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, Mirya, Podrazik, Erica, and Mohamed, Heather Silber. 2020. “Choosing Choice: How Gender and Religiosity Shape Abortion Attitudes among Latinos.” Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics 5 (2): 384411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Yanshu, Davies, Paul G., Sibley, Chris G., and Osborne, Danny. 2016. “Benevolent Sexism, Attitudes toward Motherhood, and Reproductive Rights: A Multi-study Longitudinal Examination of Abortion Attitudes.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 42 (7): 970–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Yanshu, Sibley, Chris G., and Osborne, Danny. 2020. “Breast Is Best, but Where? Hostile Sexism Underlies Men’s Opposition to Breastfeeding in Public.” Journal of Social Issues 76 (2): 219–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jelen, Ted G. 1988. “Changes in the Attitudinal Correlations of Opposition to Abortion, 1977- 1985.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 27 (2): 211–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jelen, Ted G., and Wilcox, Clyde. 2003. “Causes and Consequences of Public Attitudes toward Abortion: A Review and Research Agenda.” Political Research Quarterly 56 (4): 489500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kam, Cindy D., and Allison, M. N. Archer. 2021. “Mobilizing and Demobilizing: Modern Sexism and Turnout in the #MeToo Era.” Public Opinion Quarterly 85 (1): 172–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Killian, Mitchell, and Wilcox, Clyde. 2008. “Do Abortion Attitudes Lead to Party Switching?Political Research Quarterly 61 (4): 561–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layman, Geoffrey C. 2001. The Great Divide: Religious and Cultural Conflict in American Party Politics. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Levendusky, Matthew. 2009. The Partisan Sort: How Liberals Became Democrats and Conservatives Became Republicans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebertz, Scott, and Bunch, Jaclyn. 2021. “Backfiring Frames: Abortion Politics, Religion, and Attitude Resistance.” Politics and Religion 14 (3): 403–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lizotte, Mary-Kate. 2015. “The Abortion Attitudes Paradox: Model Specification and Gender Differences.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 36 (1): 2242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. Scott, and Freese, Jeremy. 2014. Regression Models for Categorical Dependent Variables Using Stata. 3rd ed. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Luker, Kristin. 1985. Abortion and the Politics of Motherhood. Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
McThomas, Mary, and Tesler, Michael. 2016. “The Growing Influence of Gender Attitudes on Public Support for Hillary Clinton, 2008–2012.” Politics & Gender 12 (1): 2849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mooney, Christopher Z., and Schuldt, Richard G.. 2008. “Does Morality Policy Exist? Testing a Basic Assumption.” Policy Studies Journal 36 (2): 199218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, Amy O., Sutton, Robbie M., Douglas, Karen M., and McClellan, Leigh M.. 2011. “Ambivalent Sexism and the ‘Do’s and ‘Don’t’s of Pregnancy: Examining Attitudes toward Proscriptions and the Women who Flout Them.” Personality and Individual Differences 51 (7): 812–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osborne, Danny, Huang, Yanshu, Overall, Nickola C., Sutton, Robbie M., Petterson, Aino, Douglas, Karen M., Davies, Paul G., and Sibley, Chris G.. 2022. “Abortion Attitudes: An Overview of Demographic and Ideological Differences.” Political Psychology 43 (S1): 2976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petterson, Aino, and Sutton, Robbie M.. 2018. “Sexist Ideology and Endorsement of Men’s Control Over Women’s Decisions in Reproductive Health.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 42 (2): 235–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center. 2022. “Public Opinion on Abortion.” May 17. https://www.pewresearch.org/religion/fact-sheet/public-opinion-on-abortion/ (accessed December 28, 2022).Google Scholar
Roberti, Amanda. 2021. “‘Women Deserve Better’: The Use of the Pro-woman Frame in Anti- abortion Policies in U.S. States.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 42 (3): 207–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Melody. 2011. “Pro-life, Pro-woman? Frame Extension in the American Antiabortion Movement.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 32 (1): 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, Brenda L., and Trigg, Kristin Y.. 2004. “Tolerance of Sexual Harassment: An Examination of Gender Differences, Ambivalent Sexism, Social Dominance, and Gender Roles.” Sex Roles 50 (7): 565–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanbonmatsu, Kira. 2002. Democrats, Republicans, and the Politics of Women’s Place. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaffner, Brian F. 2022. “Optimizing the Measurement of Sexism in Political Surveys.” Political Analysis 30 (3): 364–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreiber, Ronnee. 2002. “Injecting a Woman’s Voice: Conservative Women’s Organizations, Gender Consciousness, and the Expression of Women’s Policy Preferences.” Sex Roles 47: 331–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreiber, Ronnee. 2008. Righting Feminism: Conservative Women and American Politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, Reva B. 2007. “The New Politics of Abortion: An Equality Analysis of Woman-Protective Abortion Restrictions.” University of Illinois Law Review 2007 (3): 9911054.Google Scholar
Small, Kenneth A., and Hsiao, Cheng. 1985. “Multinomial Logit Specification Tests.” International Economic Review 26 (3): 619–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swank, Eric. 2021. “The Gender Conservatism of Pro-life Activists.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 42 (2): 124–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swim, Janet K., and Campbell, Bernadette. 2003. “Sexism: Attitudes, Beliefs, and Behaviors.” In Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes, eds. Brown, Rupert and Gaertner, Sam. Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons, 218–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swim, Janet K., and Cohen, Laurie L.. 1997. “Overt, Covert, and Subtle Sexism: A Comparison between the Attitudes toward Women and Modern Sexism Scales.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 21 (1): 103–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tedrow, Lucky M. and Mahoney, E. R.. 1979. “Trends in Attitudes toward Abortion: 1972–1976.” Public Opinion Quarterly 43 (2): 181–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomae, Manuela, and Houston, Diane M.. 2016. “The Impact of Gender Ideologies on Men’s and Women’s Desire for a Traditional or Non-Traditional Partner.” Personality and Individual Differences 95: 152–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travaglia, Lucy K., Overall, Nickola C., and Sibley, Chris G.. 2009. “Benevolent and Hostile Sexism and Preferences for Romantic Partners.” Personality and Individual Differences 47 (6): 599604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, Nicholas A., Wayne, Carly, and Oceno, Marzia. 2018. “Mobilizing Sexism: The Interaction of Emotion and Gender Attitudes in the 2016 US Presidential Election.” Public Opinion Quarterly 82 (S1): 799821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, Nicholas J. G. 2022. “Hostile Sexism, Benevolent Sexism, and American Elections.” Politics & Gender. Published online June 8. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X22000010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Worthen, Meredith G. F. 2020. “‘All the Gays Are Liberal?’ Sexuality and Gender Gaps in Political Perspectives among Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Mostly Heterosexual, and Heterosexual College Students in the Southern USA.” Sexuality Research and Social Policy 17 (1): 2742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaller, John R. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. American National Election Studies information

Figure 1

Figure 1. Bivariate relationship between hostile sexism and abortion attitudes over time.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Abortion attitudes and hostile sexism in 2012.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Abortion attitudes and hostile sexism in 2016.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Abortion attitudes and hostile sexism in 2020.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Average marginal effects of hostile sexism in 2012, 2016, and 2020.

Supplementary material: File

Cizmar and Kalkan supplementary material

Online Appendix
Download Cizmar and Kalkan supplementary material(File)
File 85.2 KB