Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-5mhkq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T22:47:27.400Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning biases in proper nouns

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 April 2024

Yu Tanaka*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Culture and Information Science, Doshisha University, Kyoto, Japan.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

It has been proposed that there are cognitive biases in language learning that favour certain patterns over others. This study examines the effects of such bias factors on the learning of the phonology of proper nouns. I take up the phenomenon of compound voicing in Japanese surnames. The results of two judgment experiments show that, while Japanese speakers replicate various kinds of statistical regularities in existing names, they tend to extend only phonologically motivated patterns to novel names. This suggests that phonological naturalness plays a role even in the learning of a highly faithful category of words, namely proper nouns, and provides evidence for the relevance of learning biases in synchronic grammar.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Some sound patterns are more commonly attested than others in languages of the world. One of the factors that has been proposed to account for such cross-linguistic tendencies is the notion of bias. Some researchers argue that language learners are biased for certain phonological properties. For example, patterns are easier to learn if they are structurally simple (e.g., Pycha et al. Reference Pycha, Nowak, Shin and Shosted2003; Moreton Reference Moreton2008, Reference Moreton2012; see Moreton & Pater Reference Moreton and Pater2012a for a review) or grounded in phonetic principles such as ease of articulation and perception (e.g., Wilson Reference Wilson2006; Myers & Padgett Reference Myers and Padgett2014; Martin & Peperkamp Reference Martin and Peperkamp2020; see Moreton & Pater Reference Moreton and Pater2012b for a review; Donegan & Stampe Reference Donegan, Stampe and Dinnsen1979; Hayes Reference Hayes, Darnell, Moravcsik, Noonan, Newmeyer and Wheatley1999 for general discussion). This kind of cognitive bias with respect to language learning can be referred to as ‘analytic bias’. Others argue that bias lies rather in the way language sounds are transmitted. Speech signals can be systematically misperceived due to factors such as coarticulation on the part of the speaker and compensation for it on the part of the hearer. This can eventually lead to changes in phonological representations, often in the direction of phonetic naturalness (Ohala Reference Ohala and Jones1993; Blevins Reference Blevins2004; see Hansson Reference Hansson2008; Garrett & Johnson Reference Garrett, Johnson and Yu2013 for reviews). This kind of bias that may cause transmission errors can be called ‘channel bias’.

Note that most bias-based accounts, whether the biasing factors are assumed to be in speech transmission or in language learning, do not make an extreme argument that all sound patterns should be natural. An account based on channel bias, for example, may attribute the emergence of unnaturalness to historical quirks. An unnatural pattern may be created by a sequence of natural sound changes (Kenstowicz & Kisseberth Reference Kenstowicz and Kisseberth1977: 64–65; Beguš Reference Beguš2018) or external factors such as language contact (Blevins Reference Blevins, Bowern, Horn and Zanuttini2017). An account based on analytic bias may also allow for the existence of unnaturalness. Natural patterns are favoured in language learning; yet those that do not fit the description are still learnable if there are sufficient input data (e.g., Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Zuraw, Siptár and Londe2009; White Reference White2014; see Hayes Reference Hayes, Darnell, Moravcsik, Noonan, Newmeyer and Wheatley1999 for discussion).

Although the two approaches are not mutually exclusive (Moreton Reference Moreton2008; Beguš Reference Beguš2018), they do make different predictions as to how certain types of sound patterns are learned. Suppose that there is a phonological pattern that is structurally complex and has no phonetic motivation. Suppose also that there is another pattern that is structurally simpler and phonetically better motivated. An account based on analytic bias predicts that a learner will find the former more difficult to learn than the latter. An account based on channel bias, on the other hand, does not necessarily predict that there will be a difference in the learning of those patterns; the two patterns can be learned equally well as long as there is no difference in their phonetic precursors, which would lead to transmission errors between the speaker and the hearer (see Moreton Reference Moreton2008 for relevant discussion).

Along these lines, some previous studies have tried to tease apart the two approaches via artificial language learning experiments (see Moreton & Pater Reference Moreton and Pater2012a,Reference Moreton and Paterb for reviews). Others have addressed the same question by conducting so-called ‘surfeit-of-the-stimulus’ experiments (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Ketrez and Nevins2011), which use data based on real language phonology. As stated above, a language may contain natural and unnatural patterns, and speakers of the language are exposed to both of them as statistical regularities. Researchers can investigate whether and if so how they generalise those patterns to novel items. If it turns out that speakers show preferences for natural patterns over unnatural ones in nonce word tasks, despite the same amount of experience with both kinds in real words, this would suggest that their grammar is biased for naturalness. A number of experimental studies have shown that patterns that ‘phonologically make sense’ are more readily reproduced or judged more acceptable than arbitrary patterns, even though their participants must have received data for both of them in their ambient languages (e.g., Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Zuraw, Siptár and Londe2009; Zhang & Lai Reference Zhang and Lai2010; Becker et al. Reference Becker, Ketrez and Nevins2011, Reference Becker, Nevins and Levine2012; Hayes & White Reference Hayes and White2013).

The current study aims to address the issues of learning biases through surfeit-of-the-stimulus experiments, making use of proper nouns as primary data. Names in general often show aberrant sound patterns that differ from those of other words (see Broad et al. Reference Broad, Prickett, Moreton, Pertsova and Smith2015; Moreton et al. Reference Moreton, Smith, Pertsova, Broad and Prickett2017; Jaber & Omari Reference Jaber and Omari2018 and references therein). They also tend to be highly lexicalised, and retain archaic characteristics that are no longer seen elsewhere in the language. Theoretically, these facts can be explained in terms of category-specific privilege effects (Smith Reference Smith, Oostendorp, Ewen, Hume and Rice2011). Proper nouns constitute an independent category, and faithfulness constraints indexed to that category are inherently ranked high in grammar (Moreton et al. Reference Moreton, Smith, Pertsova, Broad and Prickett2017; Jaber & Omari Reference Jaber and Omari2018). This does not mean, however, that proper nouns are simply exempt from all sorts of grammatical operations. Phonological processes may still apply productively in newly coined names (e.g., Moreton et al. Reference Moreton, Smith, Pertsova, Broad and Prickett2017). One can then ask how the sound patterns of names are learned and extended to novel names. Studying the phonology of a highly faithful category of words and investigating what processes are learned, or underlearned, may provide a new window on the role of learning biases in phonology.

Compound voicing, known as rendaku, in Japanese surnames is a good case study subject in this respect. Rendaku in compound surnames exhibits peculiar patterns compared to its application in common noun compounds. Some of these patterns are difficult to define in terms of phonological features and lack clear phonetic motivations. Most of these peculiarities turn out to have roots in the sound patterns of Old Japanese, suggesting that surnames may simply retain archaic traits of the language. However, there is also evidence that rendaku in surnames is productive; the voicing alternation occurs in novel names. This suggests that Japanese speakers are exposed to surnames, somehow learn the rendaku application patterns in them, and extend those patterns to surnames they have not seen before. Questions to be addressed here are (i) what kinds of phonological patterns are found in existing names, and (ii) which among those are well learned and generalised to novel names. If phonologically natural patterns are preferred over unnatural ones even in the learning of proper nouns, which are generally tolerant of idiosyncrasy, it will serve as yet another kind of evidence for the effects of analytic biases.

The article is organised as follows. §2 gives descriptions of the rendaku patterns in Japanese surnames. §3 proposes possible accounts of the phenomenon and discusses how they relate to the issues of learning biases. In §§4 and 5, I report the results of rendaku judgment experiments using real and nonce name stimuli. §6 gives a general discussion, and §7 concludes the article.

2. Rendaku in surnames

2.1 Background: Rendaku

Rendaku is a process in Japanese whereby the initial obstruent of the second member of a morphologically complex word becomes voiced, as shown in (1).Footnote 1

As can be seen, if the second element (henceforth E2) of a compound starts with a voiceless obstruent such as , , or , the consonant becomes voiced as a result of compounding. Note that alternates with for historical reasons (Ueda Reference Ueda1898; Miyake Reference Miyake2003: 66–77).

Rendaku is intrinsically variable, and its applicability is affected by a number of phonological and non-phonological factors (see Vance Reference Vance and Kubozono2015a; Kawahara & Zamma Reference Kawahara and Zamma2016 for overviews). One such factor is lexical idiosyncrasy. Rosen (Reference Rosen2001) shows that some morphemes very often undergo voicing when they appear as E2 of a compound, hence he calls them ‘rendaku lovers’; there are other morphemes dubbed ‘rendaku haters’ that typically do not voice, and ‘rendaku immune morphemes’ that in fact never voice (Rosen Reference Rosen2001; Irwin Reference Irwin2016a; see also Rosen Reference Rosen2016 for additional observations and analysis.)

Not only E2 but compound words themselves may show idiosyncratic behaviours. There are cases where the exact same E2, whether a lover or hater, undergoes rendaku in one compound but not another, as shown in (2).

Rendaku is also seen in proper nouns. Many Japanese surnames are compounds; about 96% of the 10,019 most common surnames are composed of multiple morphemes (see Shirooka & Murayama Reference Shirooka and Murayama2011). A compound surname qualifies for rendaku application if its E2 starts with a voiceless obstruent. As in regular compounds, rendaku in surnames is not an iron-clad rule. Certain names typically show voicing while others do not, and there are also some that oscillate, as shown in (3).Footnote 2

It is worth noting here that rendaku in surnames is usually not reflected in orthography. By convention, surnames are written in Chinese characters, or kanji, which do not indicate voicing resulting from rendaku. Notice that in (3), the E2 morpheme ‘paddy’ is always written with the same kanji , whether it is realised as or . Thus, when speakers encounter a surname in written form, especially an unknown one, they need to make a judgment on whether it should be pronounced with or without voicing.

The literature shows that rendaku in surnames exhibits some peculiarities. In what follows, I describe the patterns of voicing in compound surnames, highlighting their differences from those in regular compounds.

2.2 Strong Lyman’s Law

Previous studies have shown that rendaku application in surnames, though inherently variable, is predictable to some extent. Sugito (Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965) points out that voicing is conditioned by the onset consonant of the final syllable of the first element (henceforth E1). As shown in (4a), if the consonant in question (underlined) is , , , or , rendaku is commonly observed. Contrariwise, if it is a voiced obstruent, rendaku never applies, as in (4b).

Although Sugito’s (Reference Sugito1965) focus was limited to surnames with ‘paddy’ as E2, subsequent studies (Kubozono Reference Kubozono2005; Zamma Reference Zamma2005; Tanaka Reference Tanaka2017; Zamma & Asai Reference Zamma and Asai2017, among others) have further shown that the generalisation holds mostly true for many other names.

This blocking of rendaku by a voiced obstruent in E1 may, at first glance, seem to be the effect of oft-cited Lyman’s Law (Lyman Reference Lyman1894; also see Motoori Reference Motoori1790–1822), namely a ban on multiple voiced obstruents. The law is understood as a general morpheme structure constraint against the co-occurrence of voiced obstruents (see Morita Reference Morita1977; Ito & Mester Reference Ito and Mester1986; Yamaguchi Reference Yamaguchi1988). One may thus think that if there is already a voiced obstruent in E1, rendaku should be blocked, as its application would create two voiced obstruents within the name.

Although the description itself may be on the right track, it raises the issue of the domain of Lyman’s Law. Under the most widely accepted definition, the law stipulates that there may be no multiple voiced obstruents within a single stem of Yamato (native) Japanese origin. In other words, the law is bounded by the stem and not by the prosodic word (Ito & Mester Reference Ito and Mester2003). In regular compounds, rendaku is categorically blocked if there is already a voiced obstruent in E2, as shown in (5a). A voiced obstruent in E1, on the other hand, does not necessarily inhibit rendaku, as in (5b).

Even though examples like (5b) are found, it has been suggested that voiced obstruents in E1 still inhibit compound voicing (e.g., Kindaichi et al. Reference Kindaichi, Hayashi and Shibata1988: 264; Sato Reference Sato1989; Labrune Reference Labrune2012: 120–121). This alleged restriction on multiple voiced obstruents at the word level is often referred to as ‘Strong Lyman’s Law’, as opposed to the stem-bounded version of the law.

The validity of Strong Lyman’s Law has been the subject of controversy, however, and the evidence supporting it is mixed. Irwin (Reference Irwin2014, Reference Irwin2016a) and Sano (Reference Sano2015) investigated different corpora of regular compounds, and both argue that its effect, if any, is negligible. Ohta’s (Reference Ohta2015) corpus study employed the same data source as Irwin (Reference Irwin2014) but different analytical methods. He suggests that Strong Lyman’s Law is partially active, reporting that a subset of voiced obstruents in E1 do lower rendaku applicability. Asai (Reference Asai2014) also found some weak effects of the word-bounded law in his original data of compounds compiled from magazines. It should be noted, however, that his corpus included both regular words and proper nouns (person and place names), and the results may not be suitable for our purposes. Kawahara & Sano (Reference Kawahara, Sano, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs2014b) tested the psychological reality of Strong Lyman’s Law experimentally, and found null results. In fact, when Lyman (Reference Lyman1894) first described what would be later known as Lyman’s Law, he also made it clear that a voiced obstruent in the final syllable of E1 does not affect rendaku, based on his own observation of words in a Japanese-English dictionary.

Part of the problem here is that Strong Lyman’s Law has never been formally defined, and that there seem to be different views on what counts as an active constraint. Given what has been reported in the literature, a plausible interpretation would be that Lyman’s Law, which is usually stem-bounded, also shows some gradient or probabilistic effects on the word level. It is known that phonotactic restrictions that have a categorical effect within a smaller domain (e.g., stem) may also have a weaker effect in, or ‘leak into’, a bigger domain (e.g., word or sentence) in languages (Martin Reference Martin2011; Breiss & Hayes Reference Breiss and Hayes2020). Strong Lyman’s Law might then be another case of leakage of stem-internal phonotactics.

The question still arises as to why the law’s effects differ in surnames and normal words. As stated above, Strong Lyman’s Law is very much in force in compound surnames. In regular compounds, it is not strictly enforced, with only weaker effects at best.

2.3 Identity and Similarity Avoidance

Previous studies have revealed another factor affecting rendaku in surnames. Tanaka (Reference Tanaka2017) and Zamma & Asai (Reference Zamma and Asai2017) show that avoidance of consonantal identity or similarity promotes voicing. That is, rendaku is more likely to occur when a compound surname underlyingly has a sequence of homorganic voiceless obstruents in E1 and E2. In such cases, application of compound voicing can yield the dissimilation of identical consonants (e.g., ), which I call Identity Avoidance (Yip Reference Yip, LaPointe, Brentari and Farrell1998), or the dissimilation of similar consonants more broadly (e.g., ), which I call Similarity Avoidance (see Frisch Reference Frisch2004; Frisch et al. Reference Frisch, Pierrehumbert and Broe2004). Examples are given in (6).

Research to date has not fully confirmed that Identity Avoidance and Similarity Avoidance play any active roles as rendaku triggers in regular compounds. Most corpus studies of rendaku do not devote particular attention to the issue (Irwin Reference Irwin2014, Reference Irwin2016a; Ohta Reference Ohta2015; Sano Reference Sano2015; cf. Asai Reference Asai2014). Other studies only discuss identity in a narrower sense. Toda (Reference Toda1988) investigates compound words in Early Modern Japanese, and states that rendaku is more likely to occur if there are ‘identical moras’ across the E1–E2 boundary (e.g., $\rightarrow $ ‘red-paper’). Kawahara & Sano (Reference Kawahara, Sano, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs2014b) conduct a nonce-word judgment experiment, and show that voicing change is more acceptable if it helps dissimilate ‘identical moras’ (e.g., $\rightarrow $ ‘squid-nonce’). These studies, however, do not specifically test the question of whether Identity Avoidance and Similarity Avoidance at the ‘consonantal level’ promote rendaku in regular compounds.Footnote 3

2.4 Other patterns: Sonorants in E1

Rendaku in surnames shows further complications. Kubozono (Reference Kubozono2005) observes that, in Sugito’s (Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965) data of surnames with as E2, the presence of in the final syllable of E1 tends to block rendaku. Many surnames with E1 indeed resist rendaku, as shown in (7a), with just a few exceptions, as in (7b).

Later studies have confirmed that this rendaku-inhibiting effect of E1 is also found in surnames with other E2 morphemes (Asai Reference Asai2014; Zamma & Asai Reference Zamma and Asai2017).

There has been a claim that in the final syllable of E1 acts as a rendaku blocker in regular compounds as well (Hirano Reference Hirano2013 cited by Irwin Reference Irwin2016a: 97; also see Asai Reference Asai2014; Vance & Asai Reference Vance and Asai2016; Asai & Vance Reference Asai and Vance2017). Irwin (Reference Irwin2016a) calls this into question, however, noting that the rendaku rates of words with E1 are only slightly lower than expected in his large corpus data. Toda (Reference Toda1988) also states impressionistically that E1 dampens rendaku somewhat, but not considerably, in Early Modern Japanese. Lastly, Asai (Reference Asai2014) reports that in E1 does inhibit rendaku in his magazine-based corpus, noting that the effect is weak but statistically significant. As stated above, however, his data include proper nouns, and thus the result should be taken with a grain of salt.

The nasals and also pose puzzles. It has been argued that onset nasals in E1-final syllables promote rendaku in surnames. As shown in (4) above, Sugito (Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965) classifies them as consonants that trigger voicing along with voiceless obstruents. Zamma (Reference Zamma2005) and Zamma & Asai (Reference Zamma and Asai2017) report that the rendaku-promoting effects of nasals are also found in surnames with some E2 morphemes other than ‘paddy’, even though they are not as robust. They further state that the two nasal segments show different behaviours; triggers rendaku more than . Putting these observations together, they conclude that nasals in E1 promote rendaku only in surnames with certain E2 morphemes, and that the effect of is weaker than that of .

Do we find the same or similar patterns in regular compounds? The literature does not offer a definitive answer. Irwin’s (2016a) corpus study reports the average rendaku rates of all compounds (0.769), as well as those of compounds with E1 (0.765) and E1 (0.726), but no statistical test is conducted to specifically test their differences. Ohta (Reference Ohta2015) shows that some specific cases of in E2 make rendaku less likely (see §3.2 for details), but does not include nor in E1 in his list of the factors that significantly affect rendaku, much less mention the difference between the two.

These facts about sonorants again highlight the peculiarities of rendaku in surnames. Several studies report that the type of the last sonorant in E1 affects rendaku applicability in compound surnames, but the literature has not reached a consensus as to whether the same effects are found in regular compounds.

2.5 Summary of rendaku in surnames

Table 1 summarises the characteristics of rendaku in surnames based on the findings of previous studies. In the next section, I lay out two possible accounts of these patterns: one based on naturalness and the other based on diachrony. I then discuss how they can be relevant to the issues of learning biases.

Table 1 Effects on rendaku in surnames.

3. Naturalness or diachrony?

3.1 A naturalness account

One possible way of accounting for rendaku in Japanese surnames is to attribute all of the patterns to principles based on phonological naturalness. Here, I use the term ‘naturalness’ in a broad sense, defining it from two perspectives: phonetic substance and structural complexity. A sound pattern is said to be natural in terms of phonetic substance, if it is motivated by the ease of articulatory or perceptual difficulties (Donegan & Stampe Reference Donegan, Stampe and Dinnsen1979; Hayes Reference Hayes, Darnell, Moravcsik, Noonan, Newmeyer and Wheatley1999; also see Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Kirchner and Steriade2004 and papers therein). A pattern is also considered natural from the structural point of view, if it can be easily defined without dependencies of distinctive features across multiple dimensions (see Moreton Reference Moreton2008, Reference Moreton2012; Moreton & Pater Reference Moreton and Pater2012a). I discuss below whether each of the rendaku patterns in Japanese surnames deserves such a naturalness-based explanation.

Strong Lyman’s Law should be considered natural in terms of structural complexity. Formally, it is a segmental version of the Obligatory Contour Principle (OCP; McCarthy Reference McCarthy1986, Reference McCarthy1988); it simply bans multiple cases of [ $-$ sonorant, $+$ voice] segments within a word (Ito & Mester Reference Ito and Mester1986, Reference Ito and Mester2003), without involving inter-dimensional dependencies (Moreton Reference Moreton2012). Meanwhile, the law is unnatural in its phonetic grounding (Kawahara Reference Kawahara2008). According to Ohala (Reference Ohala, Masek, Hendrick and Miller1981), dissimilation stems from perceptual errors of some kind; phonetic features that spread out across segments can cause perceptual confusions, which can in turn lead to a dissimilatory sound change due to hypercorrection by listeners. Ohala (Reference Ohala, Masek, Hendrick and Miller1981, Reference Ohala and Jones1993) specifically claims that a voicing feature, whose main phonetic correlates do not typically stretch out, should not be subject to dissimilation, and that cases of synchronic voicing dissimilation must have originated from co-occurrence restrictions against features other than voicing (see Kawahara Reference Kawahara2008; Vance et al. Reference Vance, Kawahara and Miyashita2021; §3.2 for the origin of Lyman’s Law). Taken together, Strong Lyman’s Law is structurally simple but phonetically unmotivated.

The patterns of Identity Avoidance and Similarity Avoidance discussed in this study are natural on the grounds of both formal structure and phonetic substance. They are instances of dissimilation that are particularly concerned with place and manner, and can be formalised as versions of OCP targeting a sequence of total or partial identity (McCarthy Reference McCarthy1986; Yip Reference Yip, LaPointe, Brentari and Farrell1998; Frisch et al. Reference Frisch, Pierrehumbert and Broe2004), without involving complex feature dependencies. A number of studies have also claimed that the principles have a functional motivation. Repetition of consonants that share place and manner features tends to induce errors in production and perception, which may stem from difficulties in language processing (see Frisch Reference Frisch2004; Alderete & Frisch Reference Alderete, Frisch and Lacy2007 and references therein). Avoidance of consonantal identity or similarity is thus well motivated in that it reduces those difficulties. The effects are seen as both static and dynamic patterns in many languages (see Suzuki Reference Suzuki1998; Alderete & Frisch Reference Alderete, Frisch and Lacy2007 for overviews), including Japanese (Kawahara et al. Reference Kawahara, Ono and Sudo2006).

In contrast, the behaviour of does not seem very straightforward for a naturalness account. Recall that in E1 prevents rendaku in surnames. To formulate a constraint specifically banning a sequence of and a voiced obstruent, which I tentatively dub *r…D for short, one would have to refer to not only the feature [voice] but also other features with disagreeing values. Such a constraint would not count as a normal OCP-type constraint, and it would involve relatively complex feature dependencies (see Moreton Reference Moreton2008, Reference Moreton2012). Some studies of rendaku in surnames have entertained the idea of treating as a voiced obstruent (see Kubozono Reference Kubozono2005; Zamma Reference Zamma2005), but the proposal seems ad hoc unless there is supporting evidence outside this particular phenomenon. Furthermore, the constraint would look unnatural from the phonetic point of view; a sequence of and a voiced obstruent does not seem to pose any particular difficulty in production or perception.

That said, one could possibly still appeal to dissimilation as the phonetic basis of *r…D. Japanese is most typically realised as an alveolar tap or flap, or some other close variant, depending on the context (Tsuzuki & Lee Reference Tsuzuki and Lee1992; Vance Reference Vance2008: 89; Labrune Reference Labrune2014; Katz et al. Reference Katz, Mehta and Wood2018). These variants must be similar to the phonetic realisations of the coronal voiced obstruents and . Indeed, in some dialects of Japanese, , and are often misperceived as one another, leading to near phonemic mergers (see Sugito Reference Sugito, Iitoyo, Hino and Sato1982 and work cited there). It could then be that a surname with E1 resists rendaku so as not to create a sequence of phonetically similar consonants, such as and . Note, however, that this explanation makes a particular prediction: in E1 should block rendaku when the initial consonant of E2 is or , which would become or through voicing, but not when it is or . Since previous studies have not considered this particular hypothesis, the possible phonetic grounding of ’s behaviour remains to be tested.

The effects of nasals are also hard to interpret in terms of naturalness. Again, an onset nasal in the final syllable of E1 is argued to cause voicing of the initial obstruent of E2 (e.g., $\rightarrow $ ‘gold-paddy’). The pattern is not to be confused with so-called post-nasal voicing, where a coda nasal voices the immediately following obstruent. Post-nasal voicing is cross-linguistically common and arguably has a phonetic motivation (e.g., Pater Reference Pater, Kager, Hulst and Zonneveld1999; Hayes & Stivers Reference Hayes and Stivers2000; see also Ito & Mester Reference Ito and Mester1986 for its effect in Japanese). However, non-local voicing by an onset nasal seems rare and unmotivated.Footnote 4 Nasality itself is indeed a feature that may spread across segments, but it is unclear how it directly affects the voicing of a non-adjacent obstruent (see Ohala Reference Ohala, Masek, Hendrick and Miller1981, Reference Ohala and Jones1993). The fact that triggers rendaku less than also seems to have no phonetic basis. Lastly, formalisation of non-local post-nasal voicing would involve complex feature dependencies, as in the case of , suggesting that the pattern is also unnatural in structural terms.

We have seen so far that rendaku in surnames poses challenges to a naturalness account. The discussion is summarised in Table 2, which shows whether each pattern can be considered natural () or unnatural () from the viewpoints of formal structure and phonetic substance. A question mark means that it is still unclear from the evidence at hand.Footnote 5

Table 2 Formal and phonetic naturalness () or unnaturalness () of rendaku effects in surnames.

3.2 A historical account

One can also seek a different kind of explanation and claim that the whole phenomenon of rendaku in surnames should be understood as a diachronic problem. Historical studies suggest that the sound patterns of surnames we see today actually have a number of similarities with patterns seen in earlier stages of the Japanese language.

Recall that Lyman’s Law is active on the word level in current surnames. Historically, the domain of the law was in fact the prosodic word even in regular compounds (see Unger Reference Unger1977). In Old Japanese (early C7 to late C8), rendaku was blocked if either E1 or E2 contained , , or , as it would create multiple voiced obstruents within the ‘whole word’ (e.g., + $\rightarrow $ , * ‘water-bird’; Vance Reference Vance, Mufwene, Francis and Wheeler2005; the transcription is his). Vance (Reference Vance, Mufwene, Francis and Wheeler2005) and Vance & Irwin (Reference Vance and Irwin2013) confirm the validity of this generalisation by scrutinising headwords in a comprehensive dictionary of Old Japanese (Jodaigo Jiten Henshu Iinkai 1967). Unger (Reference Unger1977) refers to this ban on multiple voiced obstruents within the whole compound as the ‘strong version of Lyman’s Law’, and later studies simply call it ‘Strong Lyman’s Law’, the term we have already seen. (For more details, also see Miyake Reference Miyake1932; Jodaigo Jiten Henshu Iinkai 1967: 31; Ramsey & Unger Reference Ramsey and Unger1972; Vance Reference Vance, Mufwene, Francis and Wheeler2005; Vance et al. Reference Vance, Kawahara and Miyashita2021.)

It is worth mentioning here that the phonetic realisations of voiced obstruents have also changed historically. Word-medial voiced obstruents were prenasalised in Old Japanese (see Hamada Reference Hamada1952; Miyake Reference Miyake2003: 74; Frellesvig Reference Frellesvig2010: 34–36). This may explain the origin of (Strong) Lyman’s Law. Prenasalisation is a feature that often spreads out across segments, and multiple instances of prenasalised segments can cause perceptual confusion, which may further lead to dissimilation through phonologisation (Ohala Reference Ohala, Masek, Hendrick and Miller1981, Reference Ohala and Jones1993). Kawahara (Reference Kawahara2008) suggests that Lyman’s Law was originally a co-occurrence restriction against prenasalisation. Vance et al. (Reference Vance, Kawahara and Miyashita2021) further claim that it was a constraint banning prenasalised segments in consecutive syllables, without regard to morphological boundaries. Diachronic facts thus also explain the original phonetic motivation of the law, which has now been obscured by sound change and morphological bounding.

The properties of Lyman’s Law are not the only similarities that current surnames share with Old Japanese compounds. Vance & Irwin (Reference Vance and Irwin2013) report that in E1 affected rendaku application in Old Japanese. In their dictionary data, compounds with in the final syllable of E1 have a lower rendaku application rate (26%; e.g., + $\rightarrow $ , * ‘white-jewel’) than the overall average (41%). Although they do not conduct a statistical test, nor do they give an explanation as to why this pattern exists at all, they draw the conclusion that E1 acted as a rendaku-inhibiting segment in Old Japanese (see Vance & Asai Reference Vance and Asai2016 for related discussion). Again, we see the same pattern in Japanese surnames today.

The descriptive statistics in Vance & Irwin (Reference Vance and Irwin2013) further point to interesting facts about nasals in E1. Old Japanese compounds with E1 show a lower rendaku rate (around 30%) than those with E1 (around 50%). This apparent rendaku-inhibiting behaviour of may be attributed to its similarity to . Historically, was often interchangeable with (Martin Reference Martin1987: 31–32; Unger Reference Unger2004: 331–332), which was prenasalised word-medially as (see above). Previous studies claim that this has some influence on the way rendaku applies in present-day Japanese (Nakagawa Reference Nakagawa1966; Irwin Reference Irwin2014, Reference Irwin2016a; Ohta Reference Ohta2015; Vance & Asai Reference Vance and Asai2016): E2 morphemes containing that developed from (e.g., > ‘smoke’) tend to resist rendaku (e.g., + $\rightarrow $ , * ‘sand-smoke’), as if Lyman’s Law were in force. It is conceivable that in E1 also inhibited rendaku in Old Japanese due to its similarity to , as if Strong Lyman’s Law would apply. Then, the difference between E1 and E1 in current surnames could also be a historical relic.

In sum, many of the rendaku patterns in Old Japanese discussed here look parallel to those in current surnames summarised in Table 2. Does this simply mean that surnames are old and have retained archaic sound patterns that once existed in Old Japanese but are no longer attested in regular words in present-day Japanese? The scenario seems compatible with the idea that proper nouns are inherently privileged in the faithfulness hierarchy (Smith Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs2014; Broad et al. Reference Broad, Prickett, Moreton, Pertsova and Smith2015; Moreton et al. Reference Moreton, Smith, Pertsova, Broad and Prickett2017). After some phonological processes lose their original motivations for markedness reduction and go extinct in regular words, they still survive as lexicalised patterns in proper nouns due to greater faithfulness requirements. If this is actually the case with Japanese surnames, it may even be that their quirky rendaku patterns are just historical vestiges and not actively produced by synchronic phonology.Footnote 6

3.3 Motivation for experimentation

I have presented a naturalness account and a historical account of rendaku in Japanese surnames. The two accounts are in fact not mutually exclusive. It is possible that the patterns of voicing have their roots in diachrony but are still regulated by synchronic grammar, and naturalness also plays a role. It is thus of interest here to investigate whether and if so how present-day Japanese speakers generalise and reproduce rendaku patterns in surnames. To this end, I conduct two sets of judgment experiments: one experiment with real surnames and the other with nonce surnames as their stimuli.

First, we must examine the voicing patterns of real surnames in greater detail, since the descriptions reported in the literature are not adequate for our purposes. Rendaku is affected by variability and idiosyncrasy. Previous studies (Sugito, Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965; Kubozono Reference Kubozono2005; Zamma Reference Zamma2005; Tanaka Reference Tanaka2017; Zamma & Asai Reference Zamma and Asai2017) have not fully taken these factors into account. Their data are based on judgments made by a limited number of speakers, sometimes including the authors themselves, or a web-based corpus with potential noise. Some just focus on certain frequent E2 morphemes. Also, the jury is still out on the naturalness status of one of the factors conditioning rendaku, namely E1 . Running a systematic judgment experiment with a large number of speakers as participants and a large number of existing surnames as stimuli will allow us to better describe the general rendaku patterns of Japanese surnames.

Second, conducting a wug-test-style experiment using nonce words is an established way to test the productivity of a morphophonological process (Berko Reference Berko1958). There is already some evidence that rendaku in Japanese surnames is productive. As stated above, rendaku is not usually shown in writing, and speakers occasionally need to judge whether there is voicing or not in names they have never seen. If it turns out that speakers systematically apply rendaku in novel surnames in experimental settings, it will corroborate the argument that rendaku in proper nouns is a productive process. Diachrony may give a good account of the peculiar behaviours of surnames. However, as long as they are productively replicated in present-day Japanese, they should not be simply dismissed as fossilised patterns, but should be treated as a synchronic issue.

Lastly, once the patterns of rendaku in real surnames have been established and the productivity of the process has been confirmed with nonce surnames, we can compare the results of the two experiments and see how the patterns are learned. The questions to be addressed here are: when Japanese speakers are presented with novel names, do they faithfully reproduce all the voicing patterns of real names, natural and unnatural patterns alike, or do they generalise certain patterns better than others? This is a kind of surfeit-of-the-stimulus experiment (Becker et al. Reference Becker, Ketrez and Nevins2011) with a highly faithful category of words, namely proper nouns (Smith Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs2014; Moreton et al. Reference Moreton, Smith, Pertsova, Broad and Prickett2017), serving as stimuli. If it turns out that natural patterns are learned and reproduced more readily than unnatural ones, even with respect to proper nouns that are inherently tolerant of idiosyncratic, phonologically unmotivated patterns, this will suggest that analytic biases for naturalness play a role in language learning.

4. Experiment 1: Rendaku in real surnames

4.1 Method

4.1.1 Stimuli

I created an original list of Japanese surnames by combining data from two existing databases. The main data come from the Database of Japanese Surnames and their Rankings (Shirooka & Murayama Reference Shirooka and Murayama2011), which lists the 25,000 most common surnames taken from telephone directories. Since the database is entirely based on kanji and no annotations for pronunciation are given, data from another online database (Suzaki Reference Suzaki1999), which provides logically possible readings of surnames, were combined with it. I then extracted surnames that are potential rendaku-undergoers (i.e., E2 starts with an underlying voiceless obstruent), are written with two characters (i.e., bimorphemic), and are relatively frequent with more than 1,500 registered households.

Additional notes on the stimulus list are in order. A surname may have multiple possible readings for the kanji in E1. For instance, the kanji ‘small’ has readings such as and , and the surname ‘small-river’ can be or with as E1, but could also be or with as E1. In this study, I consider each of these pairs to be underlyingly different, assuming and , respectively, even though they are orthographically the same. Surnames may also be morphologically the same but orthographically different. The kanji for ‘island’ has two variant forms and . Surnames with these variants, such as ‘central-island’ and ‘central-island’ are treated as different surnames, following the original kanji-based databases.

Certain surnames have genitive , or between E1 and E2, which is not reflected in the orthography (e.g., ‘tree-gen.-bottom’); they were excluded since genitive particles and rendaku usually do not cooccur (see Lyman Reference Lyman1894; Vance Reference Vance, Frellesvig, Shibatani and Smith2007).Footnote 7 There are also other factors for rendaku (see Vance Reference Vance and Kubozono2015a) that are potentially at play in surnames but are not directly relevant to this study. I did not strictly control for all those factors, in order to keep the list of surnames as comprehensive as possible. These additional factors are to be taken into consideration in statistical analysis.

The resulting list contained 1,176 surnames with 122 distinct E2 morphemes. Additionally, 12 common surnames composed of three elements (e.g., ‘small-mountain-paddy’) were included as stimuli for practice. A full list of the surnames is provided as the Supplementary Material.

4.1.2 Procedure

The experiment was designed and run on the Internet-based experiment platform Experigen (Becker & Levine Reference Becker and Levine2013). Participants were asked to go to the experiment website by clicking a link posted on a recruitment page. The first page showed a consent form. After agreeing to take the experiment, participants were directed to a general instruction page. They were told that they would be answering questions about the readings of Japanese surnames written in kanji. They were then asked to complete a practice session, which had three randomly selected three-element surnames. After the practice, they moved on to the main session, where they completed 120 trials with randomly selected two-element surnames.

At each trial, participants were presented with a surname written in kanji along with the honorific suffix -san ‘Mr./Ms.’ in a frame sentence, as in ‘There is a person called -san’. They were given two numbered options for the reading of the surname written in hiragana, one with rendaku voicing (e.g., nakada) and the other without (e.g., nakata). (The presentation order of the two types of readings was shuffled for each trial.) They were asked to read the surname out loud using both of the reading options, and to judge which one would sound more natural. To make their response, they clicked on a button marked ‘1’ or ‘2’ according to the number of their selection. Once the response was made, a proceed button would appear. On clicking on the button, they were taken to the next trial.

At the end of the experiment, participants were asked to fill out a questionnaire about personal information, such as their age and home prefecture. They were also asked whether they knew what the term ‘Lyman’s Law’ means, which would indicate their knowledge of linguistics.

4.1.3 Participants

In all, 500 native speakers of Japanese were recruited on the crowdsourcing platform CrowdWorks and participated in the experiment. They received 200 Japanese yen as a reward. The data of 26 participants were excluded as their CrowdWorks ID indicated that they had also participated in Experiment 2 reported in the next section.Footnote 8 The data of eight participants were also excluded as they reported that they knew the meaning of Lyman’s Law. Some of the responses from three participants were not recorded properly on the data server’s database, possibly due to connection issues, and their entire data were discarded. The data of 463 participants, aged 18–71 (mean: 39.03; SD: 10.67), were thus entered in the final analysis.

No recruitment criteria were set with respect to dialects, and speakers of any Japanese dialect were allowed to take part.Footnote 9 Although there may be regional differences in the rendaku patterns of surnames (e.g., Sugito Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965; Morioka Reference Morioka2011: 23; Iwasaki Reference Iwasaki2013: 42; but see Takemura et al. Reference Takemura, Pellard, Hwang and Vance2019 who report no clear regional effects on rendaku in place names), speakers of all dialects were included in the participant pool because the surname databases on which this study is largely based (Suzaki Reference Suzaki1999; Shirooka & Murayama Reference Shirooka and Murayama2011) gathered data from all over Japan, without taking regional differences into consideration. The results here are thus meant to be a sample of the rendaku judgments of Japanese speakers as a whole. Since the task was orthography-based and no audio stimuli were used, the experimental design did not present biases for any particular dialect or any particular accent patterns that could affect participants’ judgments (see Sugito Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965; Zamma Reference Zamma2005; Zamma & Asai Reference Zamma and Asai2017).

4.2 Results

To first present the descriptive statistics of the results, I plot in Figure 1 the average rendaku rates of surnames with obstruents (top) and sonorants (bottom) in E1-final syllables, each broken down by E2-initial consonant. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Following the descriptions of previous studies (see Table 1), I represent the conditions that are expected to promote rendaku with white and light grey bars, and those expected to inhibit rendaku with dark grey bars. All other conditions are shown in medium grey.

Figure 1 Average rendaku rates: Existing surnames.

It can be seen that surnames containing voiced obstruents E1-finally, or the conditions labelled ‘D’, generally show lower rates of rendaku application, indicating that Strong Lyman’s Law is in effect. Meanwhile, Identity Avoidance and Similarity Avoidance promote rendaku, as the rates of conditions such as ‘E1-s & E2-s’ are relatively high. Turning to sonorants, E1 seems to act as a rendaku inhibitor. Notice that the trend is observed not only for E2 or E2 , where rendaku would yield a similar sequence such as or [r…d], but also in the case of E2 . This suggests that the blocking of rendaku by E1 is a general effect, rather than being motivated by avoidance of particular sequences. As for the nasals, E1 shows higher rendaku rates than E1 for the most part.

These results should not be taken at face value, however. The experiment has used existing surnames as stimuli without much experimental control. For example, each condition contains different numbers of E2 morphemes, including rendaku lovers and haters (Rosen Reference Rosen2001), which might possibly skew the results. Individual participants may also have different trends for rendaku responses. In order to assess the effects of the phonological factors in question while considering other factors such as the idiosyncratic properties of lexical items and participants, I ran a mixed-effects logistic regression analysis. The model was constructed using the glmer function of the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova et al. Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017) built on lme4 (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) in R (R Core Team 2021).Footnote 10

The model’s response variable was participants’ responses for rendaku application (rendaku or not). Predictors related to obstruents included the presence of a voiced obstruent in the final syllable of E1 (‘E1-D’), as well as an underlying identical consonant sequence (‘IdentC’) and a similar consonant sequence (‘SimilarC’) across the E1-E2 boundary. Those related to sonorants included the presence of (‘E1-r’), (‘E1-n’) and (‘E1-m’) in the final syllable of E1. The obstruency of the consonant in the E1-final syllable (‘E1-Obs’) was also included in the model in order to capture the potential baseline difference between obstruents and sonorants.

Table 3 Logistic regression model: Existing surnames.

Though not directly relevant to the discussion here, other factors that could affect rendaku application according to the literature were also included in the model: the presence of a so-called special mora in the E1-final position, such as a coda nasal (‘E1-CodaN’) and a non-nasal consonant (‘E1-CodaQ’), the presence of a voiced obstruent in E2 (‘E2-D’), and the length of E1 or E2 being equal to or greater than three moras (‘LongE1/E2’). (See below for more details on these factors.) For the model’s random structure, I set random intercepts for experimental participants, as well as intercepts for both E1 and E2 items, following the observation that each morpheme may show an idiosyncratic behaviour with respect to rendaku (Rosen Reference Rosen2016).Footnote 11

A table of the coefficients is given in Table 3. Note that the model presented here is a hypothesis-driven model that includes all the predictors introduced above, and is not necessarily the one that best fits the data with the fewest possible predictors. This is intended to make it easy to examine the effects of the phonological factors of interest in a single model, and also to compare the results with those of the nonce-name experiment presented later. The baseline intercept here refers to the condition where the last consonant of E1 is a sonorant and none of the factors in question is present (roughly corresponding to the ‘E1-jw’ bars in Figure 1).

The model predicts that both IdentC and SimilarC positively affect rendaku ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 2.104$ ; $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.298$ ). That is, as expected, Identity Avoidance and Similarity Avoidance make compound voicing more likely to occur. The effect of E1-D, namely Strong Lyman’s Law, is also significant in that it rather makes rendaku less likely ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -2.627$ ). E1-n and E1-m raise the probability of rendaku application numerically ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 1.292$ ; $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.351$ ), but the effect of the latter is weaker and also not statistically significant ( $p = 0.4088$ ). This seems more or less compatible with the literature: onset nasals in the final syllable of E1 tend to facilitate rendaku voicing, but the tendency is more clearly seen with than . E1-r has a significantly negative effect ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -2.048$ ), meaning that in the final syllable of E1 generally inhibits rendaku. E1-Obs is not significant ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.202$ , $p = 0.4695$ ), suggesting that there is no clear baseline difference between obstruents and sonorants (but compare the results of Experiment 2 in §5.2).

Additionally, the following predictors have turned out to be significant. A coda nasal in E1 (E1-CodaN) raises rendaku applicability (e.g., /hon-ta/ $\rightarrow $ ‘original-paddy’ ), indicating that post-nasal voicing is operative (Zamma & Asai Reference Zamma and Asai2017; cf. Irwin Reference Irwin2016a; Vance & Asai Reference Vance and Asai2016 about regular compounds). Rendaku is less likely to occur if E1 ends in a non-nasal in the coda (E1-CodaQ); in such a case, the consonant must turn into the first half of a geminate (conventionally represented as ‘Q’ in Japanese linguistics) through regressive assimilation, and rendaku would further create a voiced geminate (e.g., /hor-ta/ $\rightarrow $ , *? ‘digging-paddy’ ), which is disfavoured in the language (Ito & Mester Reference Ito and Mester1986; Nishimura Reference Nishimura2003; Kawahara Reference Kawahara2006). A voiced obstruent in E2 (E2-D) also inhibits rendaku (e.g., $\rightarrow $ , *? ‘short-cedar’ ), conforming to the stem-bounded version of Lyman’s Law (Lyman Reference Lyman1894). Three-mora or longer E1 and E2 (LongE1/E2) promote rendaku, as is also observed in regular compounds (Rosen Reference Rosen2001, Reference Rosen2003; Vance Reference Vance2015b; Irwin Reference Irwin2016a,Reference Irwinb; cf. Tamaoka et al. Reference Tamaoka, Ihara, Murata and Lim2009; Kawahara & Sano Reference Kawahara and Sano2014c; Tanaka Reference Tanaka2020 for complications).

To summarise the results overall, most of the generalisations proposed by previous research have proven to be valid. That is, rendaku in surnames contains statistical regularities that are both natural and unnatural. Of importance here is that these patterns are reflected in Japanese speakers’ judgments on existing surnames. In order to further examine whether such generalisations are actually internalised in speakers’ phonological grammars, I conduct another judgment experiment with nonce surnames.

5. Experiment 2: Rendaku in nonce surnames

5.1 Method

5.1.1 Stimuli

Non-existing surnames composed of nonce E1 and real E2 were used as stimuli. For E1, I created items looking like monomorphemic native stems. They were all two moras in length and of the (C)VCV configuration. See Table 4 for some examples of E1 items, organised by the type of the last consonant. (A full list of the words is given as the Supplementary Material.)

Table 4 Nonce E1 items by last consonant.

Table 5 Real E2 items.

I also made word definitions and example phrases which would be presented along with nonce items in the trials. For example, for some participants, the word would be presented as a type of plant, with the example phrase ‘Leaves of hesa are colouring’.

For E2, I used 35 native morphemes with an initial voiceless obstruent that appeared as E2 more than five times in the 1,176 surnames used in Experiment 1. Table 5 shows the E2 items with their kanji, meanings and raw frequencies in the real surname data.Footnote 12

E1 items and E2 items were then combined to create non-existing surnames. For example, with E1 and E2 , a surname hesa-island’ was created. The created surnames can be classified into conditions based on the last consonant of E1 and its combination with the initial consonant of E2. For example, a surname with a voiceless obstruent in the E1-final syllable may have an identical consonant sequence (e.g., ) or a non-identical sequence (e.g., ) underlyingly. One with an E1-final voiced obstruent can be characterised as a potential violator of Strong Lyman’s Law (e.g., $\rightarrow $ ).

The main session of the experiment had 128 judgment trials. It was designed so that the presented stimuli would be balanced based on the initial consonant of E2. That is, a participant would see 32 surnames with each of , , and in E2-initial position. Additionally, some E2 morphemes were set to appear more often than others, so that their frequency would roughly match the actual frequency. Those found more than 10 times in the real surname data, or ‘frequent E2s’, would appear twice as often as other ‘infrequent E2s’. For example, for -initial E2, frequent , , , and would be presented 24 times, while infrequent , , and would be presented 12 times in total. The stimulus set was also balanced based on the last consonant of E1. A participant would receive 16 surnames with each of the three voiceless obstruents (, and ), 16 with a voiced obstruent (, , or ), 16 with each of the nasals ( and ), 16 with (), and another 16 with an approximant ( or ).

5.1.2 Procedure

The experiment was implemented on Experigen (Becker & Levine Reference Becker and Levine2013). The basic procedure was the same as that of Experiment 1, except that participants were also presented with nonce E1 items before making rendaku judgments. In the instruction session, they were told that they would be presented with some obsolete words or words from some regional dialects of Japanese that might sound unfamiliar to them. They were also told that they would be answering questions about the readings of uncommon Japanese surnames. They completed a practice session with two judgment trials. After the practice, they moved on to the main session, where they completed 128 trials.

An image of the task is shown in Figure 2. (The text is translated from Japanese into English.) At each trial, participants were first presented with an E1 morpheme written in the phonographic hiragana script along with its definition and an example sentence, and were asked to read them out loud. On clicking on the Proceed button, new text would appear. A surname composed of the previously presented morpheme as E1 and an existing morpheme as E2 was presented with the honorific suffix attached. E1 was written in hiragana (e.g., ) and E2 was written in kanji (e.g., ‘paddy’). Participants were given the rendaku form (e.g., hozeda) and the non-rendaku form (e.g., hozeta) in hiragana, and were asked to judge which would sound more natural as the reading of the surname. They made their response by clicking on one of the buttons according to the number of their selection. The order of the stimuli was randomised for each participant, and the order of the response options (rendaku or no rendaku) was shuffled for each trial.

Figure 2 An image of the experimental task.

5.1.3 Participants

A total of 150 native Japanese speakers were recruited through CrowdWorks, and received 360 Japanese yen for participating. The data of two participants were excluded as they reported that they knew what Lyman’s Law means, or did not answer the question. The data of three participants were also excluded as some of their responses were not recorded properly for some unknown reason. As a result, the data of 145 participants, aged 19–65 (mean: 39.84; SD: 10.24), were included in analysis. As in Experiment 1, the participants were from various regions of Japan.Footnote 13

5.2 Results

Figure 3 graphs the average rendaku response rates of nonce surnames with E1 obstruents (top) and E1 sonorants (bottom) by the initial consonant of E2. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals calculated based on participant means. Would-be rendaku promoters are shown in white and light grey, rendaku inhibitors in dark grey and all the others in medium grey.

Figure 3 Average rendaku rates: Nonce surnames.

Surnames with E1-final voiced obstruents (D) appear to have lower rates than those with voiceless obstruents. This suggests that, by and large, Strong Lyman’s Law is operative in rendaku in non-existing surnames as well. Identity Avoidance and Similarity Avoidance also seem to be at work, since surnames with s-s, t-t, k-k and t-s show relatively high rates. In contrast, the rates of surnames with E1 sonorants appear to be neither very high nor low, hovering around 50%. Note that, compared to the results of real surnames, E1 ’s rendaku-inhibiting effects and the differences, if any, between E1 and E1 appear small and inconsistent.

A mixed-effects logistic regression model was fitted to the data in the same manner as in Experiment 1 with the same fixed predictors and random structure, except that the predictors did not include controlled factors such as the presence of an E1-final special mora and the length of E1 and E2.Footnote 14 The results are shown in Table 6. Again, the model’s baseline intercept corresponds to the condition where the last consonant of E1 is a sonorant and none of the other factors is present (i.e., roughly the ‘E1-jw’ bars in Figure 3).

Table 6 Logistic regression model: Nonce surnames.

As it shows, IdentC and SimilarC raise rendaku applicability ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.705$ ; $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.208$ ), while E1-D lowers it ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -0.812$ ). These results suggest that the patterns of voicing driven by Identity Avoidance, Similarity Avoidance and Strong Lyman’s Law are all productively reproduced in nonce surnames. In addition, E1-Obs has a positive effect ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.219$ ), indicating that the rendaku rates of E1 obstruents are generally higher than those of E1 sonorants. Although this trend is not found in real surnames, it may also be interpreted as being phonologically motivated. When there is a sequence of voiceless obstruents in general (whether homorganic or not), rendaku applies, achieving dissimilation in voicing (e.g., $\rightarrow $ ).

On the other hand, the effects of E1 sonorants are less clear. According to the model, E1-n does not particularly increase rendaku applicability ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -0.026$ ), nor does E1-m ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -0.043$ ). E1-r is predicted to make rendaku less likely, but the effect is still relatively weak ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -0.144$ ) and is also not statistically significant at the conventional alpha level of 0.05 ( $p = 0.0825$ ). These results suggest that the patterns of voicing involving E1 sonorants are not robustly extended to novel surnames.

The effects of the predictors were also assessed by means of model comparison. Simpler models were constructed by removing each predictor and were compared to the full model in terms of goodness of fit. Based on the results of likelihood ratio tests, all the predictors related to obstruents were confirmed to contribute to the full model’s better fit. In contrast, the fit does not improve in a statistically significant manner by including E1-n ( $\unicode{x3c7} ^2(1) = 0.099$ , $p = 0.7529$ ) or E1-m ( $\unicode{x3c7} ^2(1) = 0.270$ , $p = 0.6036$ ). The same is true for E1-r ( $\unicode{x3c7} ^2(1) = 2.949$ , $p = 0.0859$ ) with the alpha level set at 0.05.

The non-significant results for E1 sonorants, especially the effect of E1-r, are not easily interpreted by themselves, but they should be contrasted with the results for E1 obstruents. In the very same experiment, factors such as IdentC and E1-D were found to be more sound and significant. The whole results can also be compared to those of Experiment 1. Recall that in real surnames, most of the effects on rendaku under discussion, including E1-n and E1-r, were in fact valid. The discrepancies suggest that the rendaku patterns with E1 sonorants are not strongly internalised in Japanese speakers’ minds.

6. Discussion

6.1 Alternatives: Robustness and scope

The two experiments have shown that the rendaku patterns found in real surnames do not have the same degree of productivity in novel surnames. Given the differing naturalness of these patterns (Table 2), this can be taken as evidence that speakers are biased towards learning natural patterns. Before claiming that it is actually so, I consider alternative explanations.

An anonymous reviewer points out that the results could instead be explained in terms of the robustness of the patterns in question. According to the estimated coefficients of the regression model for real surnames (Table 7), the magnitude of rendaku promotion effect is larger for IdentC ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 2.104$ , $z = 29.638$ ) than for E1-n ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 1.292$ , $z = 3.050$ ), and that of inhibition effect is larger for E1-D ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -2.627$ , $z = -6.553$ ) than for E1-r ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -2.048$ , $z = -5.135$ ). It is then possible that the results of Experiment 2 merely reflect these differences in robustness. That is, the patterns that I have described as natural are just stronger in the data of real surnames, and it is easier for learners to generalise them.

Table 7 Logistic regression model: Regular compounds.

Although it is difficult to completely rule out this possibility, especially regarding the effects discussed above, it seems that robustness is not a panacea either. Notice that the promotion effect of SimilarC in real surnames is relatively weak ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.298$ , $z = 3.628$ ), but still shows up in nonce surnames ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.208$ , $z = 2.485$ ) on top of the E1-Obs effect. This can be compared to the inhibition effect of E1-r, which is at least stronger in real surnames ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -2.048$ , $z = -5.135$ ) but is greatly reduced in nonce surnames ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -0.144$ , $z = -1.736$ ). This may not be a fair comparison, given that one is promotion and the other is inhibition, but it still suggests that not every robust pattern shows up as is, and that the difference remains to be explained.

The reviewer also suggests another possible confounding factor: the scope of the patterns in question. There remains uncertainty in the literature as to whether these voicing patterns are truly unique to surnames. This means that Japanese speakers might actually be exposed to the reported statistical regularities in other contexts of rendaku, and also possibly to just a subset of them in those contexts. Crucially, if it is the case that the patterns deemed to be natural are more prominent in a broader range of data, it should be easier for learners to generalise them.

In order to address this issue, I reexamined the patterns of rendaku in regular compounds. I used the latest version of the Rendaku Database made by Irwin et al. (Reference Irwin, Miyashita, Russell and Tanaka2020), which contains compounds taken mainly from two large dictionaries of Japanese (see Irwin Reference Irwin2016a for details). I converted the $+$ and $-$ signs, which indicate ‘rendaku’ and ‘no rendaku’, respectively, into ones and zeros.Footnote 15 I restricted my data to compounds with purely nominal E2 morphemes, excluding those with (de)verbal and (de)adjectival ones, so that the same analysis as for surnames could be applied.Footnote 16 I also excluded words if they were annotated as being used only as proper nouns. After the procedure, 20,476 words remained. I conducted a mixed-effects logistic regression analysis on them in the same manner as in Experiment 1, except that some of the main predictors (E1-Q and E2-D) and random intercepts for participants were not included, as there were no relevant data.

The results are shown in Table 7. Note that they are quite similar to those of Experiment 1 (Table 3). Most of the main predictors have significant effects in the expected directions. This is perhaps a little surprising, given that I have described these patterns as peculiar characteristics of surnames. A closer look suggests that the tendencies are generally weaker than in existing surnames, but are still there in the large-scale data. It is possible that, given that the database includes not a few obsolete words (see Irwin Reference Irwin2016a), old traits such as Strong Lyman’s Law and E1-r are made more visible. It might be that these effects would be weaker if other predictors were also considered (Ohta Reference Ohta2015), or even non-significant if they were tested with spontaneous speech data (Sano Reference Sano2015) or in experimental settings (Kawahara & Sano Reference Kawahara and Sano2016). I leave these issues for future research.

To return to the main point, the scope-based explanation discussed above seems no longer tenable. Both natural and unnatural patterns are present also in regular compounds, albeit somewhat weakly in relative terms. If these data affect the learning of rendaku in surnames in any way, the learning of both kinds of patterns should be boosted. One may still argue that robustness plays a role here, too. In regular words, IdentC and E1-D appear to have greater effects than E1-n and E1-r. Although that is true, the ‘rendaku-promotion’ effect of E1-n in nonce surnames looks much weakened ( $\unicode{x3b2} = -0.026$ , $z = -0.315$ , $p = 0.7524$ ), considering what would be expected simply from its robustness in regular words ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.576$ , $z = 3.440$ , $p = 0.0006$ ). Furthermore, the rendaku-promotion effect of SimilarC is not even significant in regular words ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.022$ , $z = 0.123$ , $p = 0.9021$ ), but still shows up in nonce surnames ( $\unicode{x3b2} = 0.208$ , $z = 2.485$ , $p = 0.0129$ ). In sum, the results of Experiment 2 cannot be fully explained by scope or robustness.

6.2 Naturalness biases in names

I now argue that the results should be explained in terms of learning biases for phonological naturalness. Again, speakers of present-day Japanese are exposed to both natural and unnatural patterns with respect to rendaku in real surnames, and also in regular words. Nevertheless, they replicate the former more productively in novel surnames. This is very much compatible with the idea that speakers are biased towards only generalising natural patterns.

It should be highlighted here that this study is concerned with rendaku in the domain of proper nouns in particular. Names tend to exhibit peculiar patterns, and this has been explained by a strong faithfulness requirement specific to this word category (Smith Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs2014; Broad et al. Reference Broad, Prickett, Moreton, Pertsova and Smith2015; Moreton et al. Reference Moreton, Smith, Pertsova, Broad and Prickett2017). Put differently, learners are generally willing to accept idiosyncratic patterns, or perhaps any patterns, in proper nouns, attributing them to input specifications. Rendaku is also known for its irregularity, posing challenges to linguists and possibly to learners. This has led some to argue that the phenomenon should just be treated as lexicalised (see Vance Reference Vance, Kabata and Ono2014; Kawahara Reference Kawahara2015 for discussion). The patterns of rendaku in surnames as a whole could then well be learned as lexicalised patterns. Even so, speakers have still extracted phonological regularities that are well motivated and extended them to novel surnames. Meanwhile, they have underlearned unmotivated ones. I take this to indicate that they have failed to see them as real phonological regularities, and treat them as the lexicalised properties of individual surnames with name-specific faithfulness. Overall, these facts corroborate the general claim that a set of analytic biases for phonological naturalness exists; their effects appear even in the most faithful word category.

6.3 Structure or substance

As has been discussed in §3.1, analytic biases for phonological naturalness may be divided into two kinds: one based on formal structure and the other on phonetic substance. The literature suggests that the former exerts stronger effects on language learning than the latter (e.g., Pycha et al. Reference Pycha, Nowak, Shin and Shosted2003; see Moreton & Pater Reference Moreton and Pater2012b). Does the case of Japanese surnames offer any insight into this issue? Table 8 presents a summary of the patterns of rendaku in surnames with their naturalness and productivity statuses indicated.

Table 8 Rendaku in surnames viewed from naturalness and productivity.

Once again, Identity and Similarity Avoidance, which are considered natural both in terms of structure and substance, are productive. Strong Lyman’s Law, which is formally simple but not phonetically motivated, has also shown to be effective. In contrast, the patterns related to sonorants, which are deemed unnatural from both perspectives, have not proven to be fully productive. Since the E1- effect has shown no phonetic motivation, there is no condition under which the phonetic substance could be specifically tested, with structure-based factors controlled. Thus, the results here do not provide a clear-cut answer to the question of which kind of bias is stronger. Given the productivity of Strong Lyman’s Law, it can be said that the effect of formal structure is at least strong enough to manifest itself. The effect of phonetic substance alone as it relates to the phonology of proper nouns remains to be tested.

6.4 Other remaining issues

There are several remaining issues. First, the effect of E1 in nonce surnames was not statistically significant, but its weak trend should probably not be neglected. It could be, of course, that the design of the experiment was not sensitive enough to detect such a feeble effect, which would suggest that unnatural patterns are not completely unlearnable (e.g., Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Zuraw, Siptár and Londe2009; White Reference White2014), even though they are at least harder to learn.

One reviewer asks what would happen if the nonce items used in Experiment 2 were presented as common nouns. Now that the natural and unnatural rendaku patterns have also been found as weaker trends in regular words, we could actually address the same question about learning biases with them. There is one experimental study (Kawahara & Sano Reference Kawahara, Sano, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs2014b) that has found no evidence for Strong Lyman’s Law in regular compounds. It would indeed be interesting to investigate the effects of other factors and compare the results between proper nouns and common nouns.

Lastly, I have abstracted away the prosodic patterns of surnames, as I recruited experimental participants speaking various dialects of Japanese having different accent patterns. It is reported that, in Tokyo Japanese, rendaku often cooccurs with unnaccentedness (e.g., Sugito Reference Smith, Kingston, Moore-Cantwell, Pater and Staubs1965; Zamma Reference Zamma2005; Zamma & Asai Reference Zamma and Asai2017).Footnote 17 Examining whether this holds in nonce surnames would be another research topic worth pursuing.

7. Conclusion

The current study has offered evidence for learning biases from a slightly new perspective. By focusing on the phonology of proper nouns, I have shown that speakers are biased towards generalising natural patterns even in a highly faithful word category. In generative linguistics, segmental alternations in names have received relatively little attention. As I have shown, however, alternations in proper nouns can be productive, and studying their patterns closely can also help clarify the effects of learning biases. Further investigations of the phonology of proper nouns with the aim of addressing learnability and other theoretical issues are awaited.

Supplementary material

Complete lists of the surnames used in Experiment 1 and the nonce E1 items used in Experiment 2 are provided in the Supplementary Material https//doi.org/10.1017/S0952675724000046.

Acknowledgements

I am particularly grateful to Bruce Hayes, Junko Ito, Megha Sundara, Kie Zuraw and P-side members of the UCLA Linguistics Department for discussion at earlier stages of this research project. I also thank three anonymous reviewers and the editorial team at Phonology for their comments and suggestions, which have greatly helped improve this article.

Funding statement

The work has been supported by Kakenhi grants from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Grant Nos. 20K13019 and 22K13106).

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Footnotes

1 Throughout the article, I use broad transcriptions largely based on the kunrei romanisation system in Japanese, except that I transcribe the palatal approximant with and vowel length with as in IPA.

2 ‘Oscillation’ here does not mean that one single person’s surname can be pronounced either with or without rendaku at each utterance, but rather that we find some people who have the rendaku reading and others who have the non-rendaku reading, even though their surnames contain the same morphemes and are written with the same Chinese characters.

3 It has also been proposed that avoidance of consonantal identity may function as a ‘rendaku blocker’ when combined with Strong Lyman’s Law (e.g., Sato Reference Sato1989; Takayama Reference Takayama1992). See Irwin (Reference Irwin2014, Reference Irwin2016a) and Kawahara & Sano (Reference Kawahara and Sano2014a, Reference Kawahara and Sano2016) for discussion and complications.

4 One possibly related phenomenon is found in Japanese. Kindaichi ([1976] Reference Kindaichi2005) reports that the adjectival and adverbial composite suffixes and undergo voicing after a non-local nasal (e.g., ‘tremendous’). However, there are many exceptions to this generalisation, and to the best of my knowledge, no other suffixes show such patterns.

5 These are merely simplified representations. It may be more accurate to state the naturalness of phonological patterns in relative rather than absolute terms. Also, a more quantitative evaluation of each effect will be shown by means of a statistical estimate in the analysis of experimental results in §4.2.

6 Although not all of the current surnames are as old as Old Japanese, it is still conceivable that they have generally retained obsolete phonological traits. Not a few of them can actually be traced back to the names of clans in Classical Japan (C6 to late C12), or the family names of aristocrats and warriors in Medieval Japan (late C12 to C16) (see Toyoda [1971] Reference Toyoda2012; Sakata Reference Sakata2006 for the history of Japanese surnames). There is a common belief that commoners had no surnames historically and created new ones in the Meiji era (1868–1912). However, records suggest that peasants and merchants did have unofficial surnames before then (Hora Reference Hora1952; Sakata Reference Sakata2006: 42–60). These surnames were probably created in or before medieval times and were modeled after already existing surnames and place names, which presumably had old sound patterns.

7 The fifth most common surname ‘crossing-gen.-edge’ appears to be an exception to this statement as it usually shows voicing despite having genitive , as in . Its E2 was in fact historically a different morpheme ‘vocational group’ with an underlyingly voiced segment, and was later confused with the voiced form of ‘edge’ (Toyoda [1971] Reference Toyoda2012: 48). Most present-day speakers are not aware of the etymology, but the surname was excluded from the stimulus list for consistency, as it contained .

8 Experiment 2 was conducted before Experiment 1. Participants were asked not to participate in both experiments at the time of recruitment, but some still did. I excluded their data from the results of Experiment 1 to be conservative.

9 The distributions of the 463 participants’ home regions are as follows: Hokkaido: 22; Tohoku: 33; Tokyo/Kanto: 141; Tokai-Tosan: 57; Hokuriku: 12; Kinki/Kansai: 95; Chugoku: 30; Shikoku: 12; Kyushu: 50; Okinawa: 4; Other/No Answer: 7.

10 The model was run with bound optimisation by quadratic approximation with the bobyqa optimiser (Powell Reference Powell2009), with the number of iterations increased to 200,000 in order to avoid convergence issues.

11 Further including random slopes for participants in the model causes a singular fit (overfitting) problem. I thus report here the model with random intercepts, but the predictions are essentially similar with or without random slopes.

12 Here, E2 morphemes with different kanji variants (e.g., $\sim $ ‘mountain stream’; $\sim $ ‘island’; $\sim $ ‘river’) are counted as the same morpheme. In the experiment, the more frequent variant was used for presentation.

13 Their home regions are as follows: Hokkaido: 11; Tohoku: 7; Tokyo/Kanto: 44; Tokai-Tosan: 20; Hokuriku: 2; Kinki/Kansai: 26; Chugoku: 11; Shikoku: 7; Kyushu: 13; Okinawa: 1; No Answer: 3.

14 The nonce E1 items all have two moras. There are two E2 items that have three moras (Table 5), but their potential idiosyncratic behaviours are already captured as random effects.

15 Words that show variation are marked $+$ / $-$ . If the two source dictionaries both showed $+$ / $-$ for a given word, I discarded that word. If one gave a $+$ / $-$ but the other gave either $+$ or $-$ , I adopted the description of the latter and assigned the word 1 or 0 accordingly.

16 Verbs and adjectives tend to have specific phonological shapes, which would skew the results. Rendaku in (de)verbal compounds is also affected by morphological and semantic factors (see Vance Reference Vance and Kubozono2015a and references therein).

17 It has also been pointed out during the review process that the prosodic patterns of given names in Japanese show very regular patterns; most of them have antepenultimate accent or are unnaccented (Tanaka & Kubozono Reference Tanaka and Kubozono1999; Tanaka & Sugawara Reference Tanaka and Sugawara2018). This is true of surnames as well. Answering the question of why regularities and irregularities coexist in proper nouns is beyond the scope of this study. I leave it for future work.

References

Alderete, John D. & Frisch, Stefan A. (2007). Dissimilation in grammar and the lexicon. In de Lacy, Paul (ed.) The Cambridge handbook of phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 379398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asai, Atsushi (2014). Rendaku seiki no keikō to teichaku-ka [The tendency of rendaku application and its stabilization]. NINJAL Research Papers 7, 2744.Google Scholar
Asai, Atsushi & Vance, Timothy J. (2017). Kobetsu onso to rendaku [Individual phonemes and rendaku]. In Vance et al. (2017), 4768.Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Maechler, Martin, Bolker, Ben & Walker, Steve (2015). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using ‘Eigen’ and S4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, Michael, Ketrez, Nihan & Nevins, Andrew (2011). The surfeit of the stimulus: analytic biases filter lexical statistics in Turkish laryngeal alternations. Lg 87, 84125.Google Scholar
Becker, Michael & Levine, Jonathan (2013). Experigen: an online experiment platform. Available at https://becker.phonologist.org/experigen/.Google Scholar
Becker, Michael, Nevins, Andrew & Levine, Jonathan (2012). Asymmetries in generalizing alternations to and from initial syllables. Lg 88, 232268.Google Scholar
Beguš, Gašper (2018). Unnatural phonology: a synchrony-diachrony interface approach. PhD dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Berko, Jean (1958). The child’s learning of English phonology. Word 14, 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2004). Evolutionary phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2017). Between natural and unnatural phonology: the case of cluster splitting epenthesis. In Bowern, Claire, Horn, Laurence & Zanuttini, Raffaella (eds.) On looking into words (and beyond). Berlin: Language Science Press, 316.Google Scholar
Breiss, Canaan & Hayes, Bruce (2020). Phonological markedness effects in sentence formation. Lg 96, 338370.Google Scholar
Broad, Rachel, Prickett, Brandon, Moreton, Elliott, Pertsova, Katya & Smith, Jennifer L. (2015). Emergent faithfulness to proper nouns in novel English blends. WCCFL 33, 7778.Google Scholar
Donegan, Patricia J. & Stampe, David (1979). The study of Natural Phonology. In Dinnsen, Daniel A. (ed.) Current approaches to phonological theory. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club, 126173.Google Scholar
Frellesvig, Bjarke (2010). A history of the Japanese language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, Stefan A. (2004). Language processing and segmental OCP. In Hayes et al. (2004), 346371.Google Scholar
Frisch, Stefan A., Pierrehumbert, Janet B. & Broe, Michael B. (2004). Similarity avoidance and the OCP. NLLT 22, 179228.Google Scholar
Garrett, Andrew & Johnson, Keith (2013). Phonetic bias in sound change. In Yu, Alan C. L. (ed.) Origins of sound change: approaches to phonologization. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 5197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamada, Atsushi (1952). Hatsuon to dakuon to no sōkan-sei no mondai [The issues in the relation between moraic nasals and voiced obstruents]. Kokugo Kokubun 21, 1832.Google Scholar
Hansson, Gunnar (2008). Diachronic explanations of sound patterns. Language and Linguistics Compass 2, 859893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1999). Phonetically driven phonology: the role of Optimality Theory and inductive grounding. In Darnell, Michael, Moravcsik, Edith, Noonan, Michael, Newmeyer, Frederick & Wheatley, Kathleen (eds.) Functionalism and formalism in linguistics, volume 1. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 243285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce, Kirchner, Robert & Steriade, Donca (eds.) (2004). Phonetically based phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce & Stivers, Tanya (2000). The phonetics of post-nasal voicing. Ms, University of California, Los Angeles. Available at https://linguistics.ucla.edu/people/hayes/Phonet/NCPhonet.pdf.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce & White, James (2013). Phonological naturalness and phonotactic learning. LI 44, 4575.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce, Zuraw, Kie, Siptár, Péter & Londe, Zsuzsa (2009). Natural and unnatural constraints in Hungarian vowel harmony. Lg 85, 822863.Google Scholar
Hirano, Takanori (2013). A rule application approach to rendaku. Paper presented at the International Conference on Phonetics and Phonology 2013, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Hora, Tomio (1952). Edo jidai no ippan shomin wa hatashite myōji o motanakatta ka [Is it really the case that commoners in the Edo period did not have surnames?]. Nihon Rekishi 50, 27.Google Scholar
Irwin, Mark (2014). Rendaku across duplicate moras. NINJAL Research Papers 7, 93109.Google Scholar
Irwin, Mark (2016a). The rendaku database. In Vance & Irwin (2016), 79106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, Mark (2016b). Rosen’s Rule. In Vance & Irwin (2016), 107117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, Mark, Miyashita, Mizuki, Russell, Kerri & Tanaka, Yu (2020). The rendaku database. Version 4.0. Available at http://www-h.yamagata-u.ac.jp/$\sim$irwin/site/Rendaku\_Database.html.Google Scholar
Ito, Junko & Mester, Armin (1986). The phonology of voicing in Japanese: theoretical consequences for morphological accessibility. LI 17, 4973.Google Scholar
Ito, Junko & Mester, Armin (2003). Japanese morphophonemics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwasaki, Shoichi (2013). Japanese: revised edition, number 17 in London Oriental and African Language Library. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaber, Aziz & Omari, Osama (2018). Proper name subcategory: a prominent position. Language Sciences 69, 113124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jodaigo Jiten Henshu Iinkai (ed.) (1967). Jidaibetsu kokugo daijiten Jōdaihen [The unabridged dictionary of the national language by age: Old Japanese]. Tokyo: Sanseido.Google Scholar
Katz, William F., Mehta, Sonya & Wood, Matthew (2018). Effects of syllable position and vowel context on Japanese /r/: kinematic and perceptual data. Acoustical Science and Technology 39, 130137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto (2006). A faithfulness ranking projected from a perceptibility scale: the case of voicing in Japanese. Lg 82, 536574.Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto (2008). Phonetic naturalness and unnaturalness in Japanese loanword phonology. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 18, 317330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto (2015). Can we use rendaku for phonological argumentation? Linguistics Vanguard 1, 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto, Ono, Hajime & Sudo, Kiyoshi (2006). Consonant co-occurrence restrictions in Yamato Japanese. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 14, 2738.Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Sano, Shin-ichiro (2014a). Identity Avoidance and Lyman’s Law. Lingua 150, 7177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Sano, Shin-ichiro (2014b). Identity Avoidance and rendaku. In Kingston, John, Moore-Cantwell, Claire, Pater, Joe & Staubs, Robert (eds.) Proceedings of the 2013 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America, 10 pp.Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Sano, Shin-ichiro (2014c). Testing Rosen’s Rule and Strong Lyman’s Law. NINJAL Research Papers 7, 111120.Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Sano, Shin-ichiro (2016). Rendaku and Identity Avoidance: consonantal identity and moraic identity. In Vance & Irwin (2016), 4755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto & Zamma, Hideki (2016). Generative treatments of rendaku and related issues. In Vance & Irwin (2016), 1334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael J. & Kisseberth, Charles W. (1977). Topics in phonological theory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kindaichi, Haruhiko ([1976] 2005). Rendaku no kai [An account of rendaku]. In Kindaichi Haruhiko chosakushū [Haruhiko Kindaichi collection], volume 6. Tokyo: Tamagawa Daigaku Shuppanbu, 583614 [Originally published in 1976 in Sophia Linguistica 2:1–22].Google Scholar
Kindaichi, Haruhiko, Hayashi, Oki & Shibata, Takeshi (eds.) (1988). Nihongo hyakka daijiten [An encyclopaedia of the Japanese language]. Tokyo: Taishūkan.Google Scholar
Kubozono, Haruo (2005). Rendaku: its domain and linguistic conditions. In van de Weijer et al. (2005), 524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Brockhoff, Per Bruun & Christensen, Rune Haubo Bojesen (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labrune, Laurence (2012). The phonology of Japanese, The Phonology of the World’s Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labrune, Laurence (2014). The phonology of Japanese /r/: a panchronic account. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 23, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyman, Benjamin (1894). The change from surd to sonant in Japanese compounds. In The Oriental Club in Philadelphia (ed.) Oriental studies: a selection of the papers read before the Oriental Club in Philadelphia 1888–1894. Boston: Ginn & Company, 160176.Google Scholar
Martin, Alexander & Peperkamp, Sharon (2020). Phonetically natural rules benefit from a learning bias: a re-examination of vowel harmony and disharmony. Phonology 37, 6590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, Andrew (2011). Grammars leak: modeling how phonotactic generalizations interact within the grammar. Lg 87, 751770.Google Scholar
Martin, Samuel E. (1987). The Japanese language through time. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John (1986). OCP effects: gemination and antigemination. LI 17, 207263.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John (1988). Feature geometry and dependency: a review. Phonetica 43, 84108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, Hideo Marc (2003). Old Japanese: a phonetic reconstruction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Miyake, Takeo (1932). Dakuonkō [An examination of voiced obstruents]. Onsei no Kenkyū 5, 135190.Google Scholar
Moreton, Elliot (2012). Inter- and intra-dimensional dependencies in implicit phonotactic learning. Journal of Memory and Language 67, 165183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott. (2008). Analytic bias and phonological typology. Phonology 25, 83127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott & Pater, Joe (2012a). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning. Part I: Structure. Language and Linguistics Compass 6, 686701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott & Pater, Joe (2012b). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning. Part II: Substance. Language and Linguistics Compass 6, 702718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott, Smith, Jennifer L., Pertsova, Katya, Broad, Rachel & Prickett, Brandon (2017). Emergent positional privilege in novel English blends. Lg 93, 347380.Google Scholar
Morioka, Hiroshi (2011). Myōji no nazo [The mysteries of surnames]. Tokyo: Chikuma Shobo.Google Scholar
Morita, Takeshi (1977). Nippo-jisho ni mieru goon-ketsugō-jō no ichi-keikō [A combinatorial tendency of sounds in Vocabulario da Lingoa de Iapam]. Kokugogaku 108, 2029.Google Scholar
Motoori, Norinaga (1790–1822). Kojiki-den [Commentaries on the Kojiki, Records of Ancient Matters]. Nagoya: Eirakuya.Google Scholar
Myers, Scott & Padgett, Jaye (2014). Domain generalisation in artificial language learning. Phonology 31, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakagawa, Yoshio (1966). Rendaku, rensei (kashō) no keifu [A genealogy of sequential voicing and sequential unvoicing (working label)]. Kokugo Kokubun 35, 302314.Google Scholar
Nishimura, Kohei (2003). Lyman’s Law in loanwords. Master’s thesis, Nagoya University.Google Scholar
Ohala, J. John (1981). The listener as a source of sound change. In Masek, Carrie S., Hendrick, Roberta A. & Miller, Mary Frances (eds.) Papers from the parasession on language and behavior: Chicago Linguistic Society, May 1–2, 1981. Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society, 178203.Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. (1993). The phonetics of sound change. In Jones, Charles (ed.) Historical linguistics: problems and perspectives. London: Longman, 237278.Google Scholar
Ohta, Shinri (2015). Onin-teki/imi-teki yōin ga rendaku ni ataeru eikyō: rendaku dētabēsu to rojisutikku kaiki bunseki o riyō shita kenkyū [The effects of phonological and semantic factors on rendaku: a study using a rendaku database and logistic regression analyses]. On’in Kenkyū 18, 8592.Google Scholar
Pater, Joe (1999). Austronesian nasal substitution and other NC effects. In Kager, René, van der Hulst, Harry & Zonneveld, Wim (eds.) The prosody–morphology interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 310343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, Michael J. D. (2009). The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. Technical Report NA06, Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics, University of Cambridge. Available at https://www.damtp.cam.ac.uk/user/na/NA\_papers/NA2009\_06.pdf.Google Scholar
Pycha, Anne, Nowak, Pawel, Shin, Eurie & Shosted, Ryan (2003). Phonological rule-learning and its implications for a theory of vowel harmony. WCCFL 22, 101114.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Version 4.1.2. Available at https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Ramsey, Robert & Unger, Marshall (1972). Evidence for a consonant shift in 7th century Japanese. Papers in Japanese Linguistics 1, 279295.Google Scholar
Rosen, Eric (2001). Phonological processes interacting with the lexicon: variable and non-regular effects in Japanese phonology. PhD dissertation, University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
Rosen, Eric (2003). Systematic irregularity in Japanese rendaku: how the grammar mediates patterned lexical exceptions. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 48, 137.Google Scholar
Rosen, Eric (2016). Predicting the unpredictable: capturing the apparent semi-regularity of rendaku voicing in Japanese through Gradient Symbolic Computation. BLS 42, 235249.Google Scholar
Sakata, Satoshi (2006). Myōji to namae no rekishi [History of surnames and given names], Rekishi Bunka Library. Tokyo: Yoshikawa Kobunkan.Google Scholar
Sano, Shin-ichiro (2015). Universal markedness reflected in the patterns of voicing process. NELS 45, 4958.Google Scholar
Sato, Hirokazu (1989). Fukugō-go ni okeru akusento kisoku to rendaku kisoku [Accent rules and rendaku rules in compounds]. In Kōza nihongo to nihongo kyōiku 2: Nihongo no onsei, on’in (jō) [Japanese and Japanese teaching 2: Japanese phonetics, phonology 1]. Tokyo: Meiji Shoin, 233265.Google Scholar
Shirooka, Keiji & Murayama, Tadashige (2011). A database of Japanese surnames and their rankings. Available at http://hdl.handle.net/10297/00025667.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer (2011). Category-specific effects. In van Oostendorp, Marc, Ewen, Colin J., Hume, Elizabeth & Rice, Keren (eds.) The Blackwell companion to phonology, volume 4. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 24392463.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer L. (2014). Prototypical predicates have unmarked phonology. In Kingston, John, Moore-Cantwell, Claire, Pater, Joe & Staubs, Robert (eds.) Proceedings of the 2013 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America, 8 pp.Google Scholar
Sugito, Miyoko (1965). Shibata-san to Imada-san: tango no chōkakuteki benbetsu ni tsuite no ichi kōsatsu [Shibata-san and Imada-san: an examination of auditory distinction of words]. Gengo Seikatsu 165, 6472.Google Scholar
Sugito, Miyoko (1982). Kinki hōgen-ni okeru za-gyōon, da-gyōon, ra-gyōon no kondō ni tsuite [Perceptual confusions of /z/, /d/, and /r/ in Kinki dialects]. In Iitoyo, Kiichi, Hino, Sugezumi & Sato, Ryoichi (eds.) Kōza hōgengaku [Courses in dialectology], volume 7. Tokyo: Tosho Kankokai, 299325.Google Scholar
Suzaki, Haruo (1999). A private on-line database of 120,000 Japanese family names. Available at https://suzaki.skr.jp/index40.html.Google Scholar
Suzuki, Keiichiro (1998). A typological investigation of dissimilation. PhD dissertation, University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Takayama, Michiaki (1992). Rendaku to renjōdaku [On sequential voicing and sequential post-nasal voicing]. Kuntengo to Kunten Shiryō 88, 115124.Google Scholar
Takemura, Akiko, Pellard, Thomas, Hwang, Hyun Kyung & Vance, Timothy J. (2019). Rendaku in place names across Japanese dialects. Reports of the Keio Institute of Cultural and Linguistic Studies 50, 7989.Google Scholar
Tamaoka, Katsuo, Ihara, Mutsuko, Murata, Tadao & Lim, Hyunjung (2009). Effects of first-element phonological-length and etymological-type features on sequential voicing (rendaku) of second elements. Journal of Japanese Linguistics 25, 1738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanaka, Shin’ichi & Kubozono, Haruo (1999). Nihon-go no hatsuon kyōshitsu: riron to renshū [A course in Japaense pronunciation: theory and practice]. Tokyo: Kurosio Publishers.Google Scholar
Tanaka, Yu (2017). Phonotactically-driven rendaku in surnames: a linguistic study using social media. WCCFL 34, 519528.Google Scholar
Tanaka, Yu (2020). Testing Rosen’s Rule yet again: an experimental study. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 27, 355364.Google Scholar
Tanaka, Yu & Sugawara, Ayaka (2018). Revisiting accent in Japanese given names: stem-like accent with foot faithfulness. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 88, 217228.Google Scholar
Toda, Ayako (1988). Wago no hi-rendaku kisoku to rendaku keikō: Nippo Jisho to Waei Gorin Shūsei kara [The rendaku-inhibiting rules and tendencies for rendaku application in native Japanese words: an examination of the Vocabulario da Lingoa de Iapam and Hepburn’s Japanese-English and English-Japanese Dictionary]. Dōshisha Kokubungaku 30, 8098.Google Scholar
Toyoda, Takeshi ([1971] 2012). Myōji no rekishi [The history of surnames] [Originally published in 1971 by Chuo Koronsha]. Tokyo: Yoshikawa Kobunkan.Google Scholar
Tsuzuki, Masaki & Lee, Hyun-Bok (1992). A phonetic study of the Korean and Japanese lateral, flap and nasal. In The Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.) Proceedings of the 1992 Seoul International Conference on Linguistics. Seoul: Koryo University, 761780.Google Scholar
Ueda, Kazutoshi (1898). Gogaku sōken: p-on-kō [A new linguistic perspective: on the sound /p/]. Teikoku Bungaku 4, 4146.Google Scholar
Unger, J. Marshall (2004). Alternations of m and b in Early Middle Japanese: the deeper significance of the sound-symbolic stratum. Japanese Language and Literature 38, 323337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unger, Marshall (1977). Studies in Early Japanese morphophonemics. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
van de Weijer, Jeroen, Nanjo, Kensuke & Nishihara, Tetsuo (eds.) (2005). Voicing in Japanese. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (2005). Sequential voicing and Lyman’s Law in Old Japanese. In Mufwene, Salikoko S., Francis, Elaine J. & Wheeler, Rebecca S. (eds.) Polymorphous linguistics: Jim McCawley’s legacy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2743.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (2007). Have we learned anything about rendaku that Lyman didn’t already know? In Frellesvig, Bjarke, Shibatani, Masayoshi & Smith, John Charles (eds.) Current issues in the history and structure of Japanese. Tokyo: Kurosio Publishers, 153170.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (2008). The sounds of Japanese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (2014). If rendaku isn’t a rule, what in the world is it? In Kabata, Kaori & Ono, Tsuyoshi (eds.) Usage-based approaches to Japanese grammar: towards the understanding of human language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 137152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (2015a). Rendaku. In Kubozono, Haruo (ed.) The handbook of Japanese phonetics and phonology, number 2 in Handbooks of Japanese Language and Linguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 397441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (2015b). Rendaku no fukisokusei to Rōzen no hōsoku [Rendaku’s irregularities and Rosen’s Rule]. NINJAL Research Papers 9, 207214.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. & Asai, Atsushi (2016). Rendaku and individual segments. In Vance & Irwin (2016), 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. & Irwin, Mark (2013). A rendaku database for Old Japanese. Paper presented at the 21st International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Oslo.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. & Irwin, Mark (eds.) (2016). Sequential voicing in Japanese: papers from the NINJAL Rendaku Project. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vance, Timothy J., Kaneko, Emiko & Watanabe, Seiji (eds.) (2017). Rendaku no kenkyū [Research on rendaku]. Tokyo: Kaitakusha.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J., Kawahara, Shigeto & Miyashita, Mizuki (2021). The diachronic origins of Lyman’s Law: evidence from phonetics, dialectology and philology. Phonology 38, 479511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, James (2014). Evidence for a learning bias against saltatory phonological alternations. Cognition 130, 96115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, Colin (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: an experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science 30, 945982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi, Yoshinori (1988). Kodai-go no fukugō-go ni kansuru ichi-kōsatsu: rendaku o megutte [A study of compounds in Old Japanese: on rendaku]. Nihongogaku 7, 412.Google Scholar
Yip, Moira (1998). Identity avoidance in phonology and morphology. In LaPointe, Steven G., Brentari, Diane K. & Farrell, Patrick M. (eds.) Morphology and its relation to phonology and syntax. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications, 216246.Google Scholar
Zamma, Hideki (2005). The correlation between accentuation and rendaku in Japanese surnames: a morphological account. In van de Weijer et al. (2005), 157176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, Hideki & Asai, Atsushi (2017). Sei ni mirareru Sugitō no hōsoku to kakuchō-ban Raiman no hōsoku ni kansuru keitai-teki/onin-teki kōsatsu [Morphological and phonological analyses of Sugito’s Law and Strong Lyman’s Law in surnames]. In Vance et al. (2017), 147179.Google Scholar
Zhang, Jie & Lai, Yuwen (2010). Testing the role of phonetic knowledge in Mandarin tone sandhi. Phonology 27, 153201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Effects on rendaku in surnames.

Figure 1

Table 2 Formal and phonetic naturalness () or unnaturalness () of rendaku effects in surnames.

Figure 2

Figure 1 Average rendaku rates: Existing surnames.

Figure 3

Table 3 Logistic regression model: Existing surnames.

Figure 4

Table 4 Nonce E1 items by last consonant.

Figure 5

Table 5 Real E2 items.

Figure 6

Figure 2 An image of the experimental task.

Figure 7

Figure 3 Average rendaku rates: Nonce surnames.

Figure 8

Table 6 Logistic regression model: Nonce surnames.

Figure 9

Table 7 Logistic regression model: Regular compounds.

Figure 10

Table 8 Rendaku in surnames viewed from naturalness and productivity.

Supplementary material: File

Tanaka supplementary material

Tanaka supplementary material
Download Tanaka supplementary material(File)
File 149.8 KB