Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T20:25:53.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The dynamical landscape: phonological acquisition and the phonology–phonetics link

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2021

Beata Łukaszewicz*
Affiliation:
University of Warsaw
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

During acquisition children internalise adult-based phonological patterns and alternately adopt and discard child-specific patterns reflecting their unskilled production. The child-specific patterns are often assumed to be low-level phonetic effects, and so, in a classical modular feedforward grammar, they should not interfere with the higher-level adult-based phonology. This paper reports an interaction in which the application of a categorical adult-based process (Voice Assimilation) is conditioned by a gradient child-specific process (fricative devoicing). Acoustic analyses of longitudinal data from a Polish-speaking child reveal variable Voice Assimilation effects in target voiced fricative–stop/stop–fricative clusters (voicing and devoicing), correlated with the extent of voicing in fricatives in non-assimilatory contexts. I analyse this phonology–phonetics trade-off by appealing to symbol-like dynamical representations, expressed in the language of non-linear mathematics. Such representations offer a non-derivational link between the qualitative and quantitative aspects of speech. Variability ensues as a natural consequence of grammar change.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

1 Introduction

In the process of acquisition, children internalise phonological patterns on the basis of perceived regularities in the adult input. Also, and to some extent in parallel, they alternately adopt and discard child-specific patterns, reflecting their immature motor skills.Footnote 1

While the acquisition of adult-based phonological patterns has been assumed to be within the realm of phonological competence, child-specific patterns have been viewed as performance-related (cognitively controlled or biomechanical) effects, situated within or outside of the scope of phonological computation depending on the approach (see discussions in Smith Reference Smith1973, Reference Smith2010, Kiparsky & Menn Reference Kiparsky, Menn and Macnamara1977, Hayes Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004, Hale & Reiss Reference Hale and Reiss2008). The phonological vs. phonetic status of these patterns has been a protracted controversy. It suffices to note that, at its extremes, the generativist stance has produced two contradictory views on the status of such patterns: at one end they have been considered to be by-products of the unmarked initial stage, the Universal Grammar (Smolensky Reference Smolensky1996); at the opposite end as performance effects of no linguistic significance (Hale & Reiss Reference Hale and Reiss2008). Under the latter view, child-specific production patterns are regarded as merely physiological acts, not having phonetic representations – analogously to Sapir's (Reference Sapir1925) example of the ‘candle-blowing sound’, which is superficially similar to a voiceless [ʍ], but is not a speech sound (Hale & Reiss Reference Hale and Reiss2008: 66–67).

More commonly, child-specific patterns are viewed as emergent grammaticalised solutions to phonetic problems (e.g. Kiparsky & Menn Reference Kiparsky, Menn and Macnamara1977, Menn Reference Menn, Bell and Hooper1978, Hayes Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004). A cophonological organisation between the adult-based comprehension grammar and the child-specific production grammar has been assumed by some writers (e.g. Kiparsky & Menn Reference Kiparsky, Menn and Macnamara1977, Hayes Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004). The two-component organisation, schematised in Fig. 1, is analogous to standard modular ‘feedforward’ models of phonetic implementation developed for adult speech (e.g. Keating Reference Keating and Newmeyer1988; see also discussion in Pierrehumbert Reference Pierrehumbert, Gussenhoven and Warner2002). This presupposes a strict division of labour and ordering between the two components. In the feedforward model, the adult-based phonological component within the child's system acts independently of child-specific production patterns; child-specific patterns target the output of the adult-based component, i.e. the adult surface form. In Fig. 1, and throughout this paper, target forms (i.e. adult surface representations) are given in / / and the child's pronunciations in [ ]. Adult underlying representations, where relevant, appear in // //.

Figure 1 A feedforward organisation of the phonological system in the child (cf. Kiparsky & Menn 1977: 61).

The constraint-based framework of Optimality Theory (OT; Prince & Smolensky Reference Prince and Smolensky1993, McCarthy & Prince Reference McCarthy, Prince, Beckman, Dickey and Urbanczyk1995) allows for integrating adult-based generalisations and child-specific effects within the child's production grammar. The child-specific component (B → C in Fig. 1), which arises from the child's proprioception and auditory perception of his or her own vocal behaviour as compared with the perceived vocal behaviour of adults, is then hypothesised to operate in terms of the same phonological constraints as his or her comprehension (using the A → B system) (Hayes Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004: 196). This is in accordance with the findings that child-specific patterns in many respects resemble adult phonological phenomena; for example, they can be interrelated, and form ‘conspiracies’ (e.g. Smith Reference Smith1973, Menn Reference Menn, Bell and Hooper1978, Pater & Barlow Reference Pater and Barlow2003, Łukaszewicz Reference Łukaszewicz2007).

The separation of the comprehension grammar (A → B) in Fig. 1 is corroborated by the well-documented developmental perception–production gap. The major transition from a universal categorical-like perception, which is present at birth (Eimas et al. Reference Eimas, Siqueland, Jusczyk and Vigorito1971), towards the language-specific perceptual ability – sensitivity to adult contrasts (Werker & Tees Reference Werker and Tees1984, Kuhl Reference Kuhl1991, Pegg & Werker Reference Pegg and Werker1997) and phonotactics (Jusczyk et al. Reference Jusczyk, Luce and Charles-Luce1994) – emerges in the second half of the first year of life.Footnote 2 Production accuracy develops much later; mastering certain adult distinctions and phonotactics in production extends well into pre-school years (e.g. MacNeilage Reference MacNeilage, Hardcastle and Laver1997). The early acquisition of adult-based phonotactics, contrasts and allophony has been modelled in terms of formal and statistical algorithms (e.g. Tesar & Smolensky Reference Tesar and Smolensky1993, Boersma & Hayes Reference Boersma and Hayes2001, Hayes Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004, Peperkamp et al. Reference Peperkamp, Le Calvez, Nadal and Dupoux2006, Hayes & White Reference Hayes and White2013) solely on the basis of the adult (or adult-like) input (B), without reference to the development of the child's production skills and the child-specific component (B → C). From this perspective, the role of child-specific patterns is purely implementational.

One particularly noteworthy aspect of child-specific patterns, evident from longitudinal studies (e.g. Smith Reference Smith1973, Reference Smith2010, Menn Reference Menn1976, Becker & Tessier Reference Becker and Tessier2011), is that they exhibit only temporary stability. Enhanced variation and, as reported in this paper, gradience along the continuous phonetic dimension are found in the periods when these patterns dissolve. Grammaticalising these patterns in terms of OT constraints obscures the intuition that their gradual disappearance depends largely on the gradual development of the child's production skills. In this paper, I set out to tackle the problem of the interaction between the child's unskilled performance and adult-based phonology in terms of a different formal language, one which naturally links the continuous and discrete aspects of speech, and provides tools for describing mechanisms of change – the non-linear mathematics of the dynamical landscape.

1.1 The contribution of this paper

This paper seeks to deepen our understanding of how child-specific patterns relate to adult phonology by considering the problem in the context of a broader issue, not specific to acquisition but pertaining to human speech in general: the phonological–phonetic (or cognitive–physical) duality of speech structure and the nature of the phonology–phonetics link (for various views on this widely debated topic see Keating Reference Keating and Newmeyer1988, Browman & Goldstein Reference Browman and Goldstein1989, Reference Browman, Goldstein, Port and van Gelder1995, Hayes Reference Hayes, Darnell, Moravcsik, Newmeyer, Noonan and Wheatley1999, Steriade Reference Steriade1999, Pierrehumbert Reference Pierrehumbert, Bybee and Hopper2001, Reference Pierrehumbert, Gussenhoven and Warner2002, Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006). To this end, it investigates the interaction in Polish between child-specific fricative devoicing, exhibiting enhanced gradience along the continuous phonetic dimension, and adult-based Voice Assimilation, which patently reflects categorical phonological organisation. The analysis is conducted on the basis of longitudinal acoustic data from a Polish-speaking child, Jula (/jula/; henceforth J), aged 2;3–2;8. Fricative devoicing is one of the substitution processes reported in child speech worldwide. Voice Assimilation produces uniformly voiceless or voiced obstruent clusters in adult Polish. An important characteristic of the latter process is that it is not only categorical, but also transparent at the utterance level. The phonotactics that underlie this process are learnable at the earliest stages, long before access to the morphological structure of words and the underlying representations of morphemes are available to the child (for details see §2). We can expect that Polish-speaking children use this process effectively in their production as soon as they develop the ability to produce obstruent clusters and contrastive voicing.

The child's data in (1a) reveal stable and target-appropriate application of Voice Assimilation in target voiceless clusters. However, target voiced fricative–stop and stop–fricative clusters are realised variably as voiced or voiceless, as in (b).

  1. (1)

For example, in zdejmij /zdɛjmij/ [ɕtɛjmij] *[ɕdɛjmij] ‘take off (imp)’, the devoicing of the fricative (/z/ → [ɕ]) is accompanied by devoicing of the following prevocalic voiced stop (/d/ → [t]), despite Voice Assimilation being a regressive process in adult Polish. The devoicing of the stop must be interpreted as assimilatory because, unlike fricatives, target voiced stops are produced correctly as voiced in contrastive presonorant non-assimilatory contexts; e.g. dużo /duʐɔ/ [duɕɔ] ‘a lot’ (2;2.30), as in (2). The diacritic in the transcription of the fricative in [duʑ̥ɔ] (2;4.24) in (2) is shorthand for an intermediate stage of the gradient fricative devoicing pattern: it reflects the intermediate median value for the voicing parameter in the five tokens of the word at this stage in comparison with the tokens of the same word recorded at the other two stages.

  1. (2)

The reported influence of a phonetic factor (lack of motor skills) on the application of a phonological process is in conflict with the classical modular feedforward grammar, which is founded on the temporal precedence metaphor of ‘later’ phonetic and ‘earlier’ phonological rules. Voice Assimilation acts as a ‘late repair’, which is unexpected in the modular feedforward approach. As revealed by acoustic analyses, the extent of this effect varies over time, and is correlated with the growth of voicing control in fricatives along the continuous phonetic dimension.

The interaction between phonological and phonetic factors in acquisition is here analysed within the dynamical systems approach, using formal tools of non-linear mathematics proposed in earlier work for the analysis of incomplete neutralisation in adult speech (Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006, Gafos & Benus Reference Gafos and Benus2006). In a nutshell, the dynamical systems approach offers a formal language for integrating the discrete and continuous aspects of speech, while keeping the distinction between them valid. The integration is done in terms of dynamical linkage, not in terms of translating one type of structure (phonological symbols) into another (continuous phonetic parameters). Moreover, enhanced variability and gradience are predicted to be the necessary consequences of grammar change. Building upon this approach, I propose that child-specific patterns arise from performance-related effects (lack of phonetic skills), and can be formalised as consequences of scaling a dynamical system's control parameters. The growth of the phonetic capacity, reflected in the continuous shift in the control parameter, brings about qualitative differences in the dynamical landscape, i.e. the phonological organisation.

The remainder of the paper is organised as follows. §2 presents basic facts about child-specific fricative devoicing and Voice Assimilation in adult Polish, as described in the literature. The effect of Voice Assimilation is further illustrated with acoustic measurement data from adult Polish (among others, the speech of J's caregiver). §2.3 schematises the hypotheses on the distribution of voicing in obstruent clusters in the child's speech on the basis of how the two processes interact in the developing grammar. §3 presents details of the acoustic study of longitudinal data from J, giving voicing parameter measurements in fricatives and stops in singleton non-assimilatory contexts as well as in clusters. §4 outlines the tenets of the dynamical model, interprets the results in terms of this model and discusses some consequences of adopting this model for understanding the link between categoricity and gradience and the underlying mechanisms of change. Conclusions are summarised in §5.

2 The interacting processes: basic generalisations and hypotheses

2.1 Fricative devoicing

Child-specific devoicing neutralises the voiced–voiceless distinction in obstruents; for example, the mispronunciation of zęby /zɛmbɨ/ ‘teeth’ as [sɛmpɨ] (Łobacz Reference Łobacz1996: 180). In standard textbooks on speech development, this is one of numerous processes occurring in normally developing Polish-speaking children in the third year of life (e.g. Kaczmarek Reference Kaczmarek1988). According to Sołtys-Chmielowicz (Reference Sołtys-Chmielowicz1998: 136), an impressionistic cross-sectional study based on a sample of more than 1000 children aged 3–7, fewer than 2% of children still devoice obstruents at the age of 3. The process is gradient: the refinement of the earlier acquired surface contrast continues after the age of 3 (Łobacz Reference Łobacz1996: 180–181).

There is some indication in the literature that the acquisition of the surface voicing contrast in fricatives may be later than in stops. According to Kaczmarek (Reference Kaczmarek1988), the voiceless prepalatal /ɕ/ is the only fricative in the repertoire of 2-year-olds. Its voiced counterpart /ʑ/ emerges at the age of 3. Alveolar and postalveolar (retroflex) fricatives, both voiceless and voiced, are acquired at ages 4 and 5 respectively.Footnote 3 In comparison, the acquisition of voicing in stops is complete for labials and alveolars by the age of 2; it is delayed till the age of 3 only for velars. Bryndal (Reference Bryndal2015), in her study of acquisition of the inventory of Polish consonants in children aged 3–7, based on 200 subjects tested with a standardised production questionnaire (cf. Krajna Reference Krajna2008), reports non-normative pronunciations of the voiced sibilants in 3- and 4-year-olds. At age 3, none of the three consonants – alveolar /z/, prepalatal /ʑ/ and postalveolar /ʐ/ – reaches the criterion of 90% of normative realisations; at age 4, only /ʑ/ does (2015: 100–101).Footnote 4 The acquisition of voicing proceeds faster in stops: for /b/ and /d/ it is complete at age 3; for /g/ at age 4.

Fricative devoicing is also present in J's longitudinal data, which is analysed in the present study. A gradual development towards adult-like surface voicing in fricatives takes place throughout the period of development under investigation; the production of the voicing contrast in stops is predominantly accurate throughout the reported stages. A detailed description of the corpus of the child's speech samples follows in §3.1.

The above asymmetry in the child-specific devoicing pattern is paralleled by cross-linguistic patterns in adult speech. According to Ladefoged & Maddieson (Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996: 176–178), most of the world's languages with voiceless fricatives do not have voiced counterparts. The phonetic grounding for this asymmetry is twofold. Voicing is associated with strong low-frequency energy, which may mask the high-frequency frication noise, which has lower amplitude. The vocal tract settings are such that the impedance of airflow at the glottis increases the difficulty of creating turbulence at the point of articulatory constriction. Although many languages (about 32%) have no voicing contrast in either plosives or fricatives, about 33.4% of languages surveyed in Maddieson (Reference Maddieson, Dryer and Haspelmath2013) have a voicing contrast in plosives but not in fricatives; languages with the opposite pattern, i.e. a voicing contrast in fricatives but not in plosives, are relatively rare – 6.7% of the total.

2.2 Voice Assimilation

The output of the across-the-board process of Voice Assimilation in adult Polish is transparent – uniformly voiceless or voiced obstruent clusters. The process has been widely documented in the standard descriptive sources (e.g. Kowalik et al. Reference Kowalik, Grzegorczykowa, Laskowski and Wróbel1998: 91, Ostaszewska & Tambor Reference Ostaszewska and Tambor2012: 87–89), and extensively analysed within the generative phonological tradition (e.g. Gussmann Reference Gussmann2007, Rubach Reference Rubach2008). The automatic character of the process and its categoricity are widely recognised to be a source of difficulty for Polish learners of English (e.g. Sobkowiak Reference Sobkowiak1996: 50–56). Voice Assimilation effects are also present in loanwords and foreign words (e.g. football /fudbɔl/, Macbeth /magbɛt/; Gussmann Reference Gussmann2007: 291).

Voice Assimilation results in voiced–voiceless alternations both inside words (3a) and across word boundaries (3b). It also has static phonotactic effects (3c). As can be inferred from (a) and (b), Voice Assimilation is regressive: the last obstruent in the cluster determines the surface voicing of the whole cluster. The regressive character of Voice Assimilation is also seen in (3d), where the process interacts with Final Devoicing. Some words, e.g. już in (b), do not have realisations which do not involve Voice Assimilation or Final Devoicing.

  1. (3)

Final Devoicing affects obstruents at the end of words; however, when the word-final obstruent precedes a voiced obstruent at the beginning of the following word, the effects of Final Devoicing are masked by Voice Assimilation; cf. ko/d/ dostępu in (3b). In consequence, Final Devoicing can be inferred only on the basis of voicelessness of obstruents at the end of utterances, or in word-final position when the next word starts with a sonorant.Footnote 5 Although the child's data show distributions corroborating the presence of Final Devoicing in her grammar, I ignore this process in further discussion, because it cannot interact with child-specific fricative devoicing. An OT ranking for this process can be acquired as a part of the ranking established for Voice Assimilation, as sketched below.

From a purely distributional perspective, the contrastive [±voiced] specification is found only in obstruents in presonorant position; elsewhere, it is context-dependent. This simple distribution pattern can be learned at early stages, before children have access to the morphological structure of words and internalise underlying representations of morphemes, which is easily modelled using OT formalism. It can be illustrated using the pure phonotactic learner proposed in Hayes (Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004).Footnote 6 The algorithm is based on a stringent constraint installation procedure which, among other things, favours markedness and specificity. Consistent voice agreement in the input will result in installing Agreeobstr[±vcd] in the top stratum. Because of the surface contrast in voicing in presonorant contexts, the next level in the hierarchy will be occupied by a faithfulness constraint. The ‘favour specificity’ requirement will ensure that it is a positional constraint, Identpreson[±vcd]. Because of the surface contrast in voicing in presonorant contexts, the next level in the hierarchy will be occupied by a faithfulness constraint (assuming the meta-ranking Markedness ⪢ Faithfulness). Note here that standard analyses of regressive Voice Assimilation effects in OT assume either string-based (i.e. presonorant) positional faithfulness or syllable-based (i.e. onset) positional faithfulness (see the discussion in Rubach Reference Rubach2008). Assuming a string-based version of positional faithfulness makes the acquisition of Voice Assimilation independent of prior knowledge of syllable structure. The next two layers will be occupied by the general markedness constraint *Obstr[+vcd] and the general faithfulness constraint Ident[±vcd]; the latter constraint ends up lowest in the hierarchy, as it is favoured neither by markedness nor by specificity. The ranking obtained via this procedure is given in (4). The subhierarchy Identpreson[±vcd] ⪢ *Obstr[+vcd] ⪢ Ident[±vcd] automatically generates Final Devoicing.Footnote 7

  1. (4)

In addition to the patterns described above, Polish also has a process of Progressive Devoicing, which an anonymous reviewer raises as a potential issue. However, its highly restricted scope (Gussmann Reference Gussmann2007: 308), together with various other characteristics, make it inconspicuous from the perspective of a child. The process involves only two fricatives, /ʂ/ and /f/, whose voicelessness is caused by the preceding voiceless obstruent, as evidenced by the alternations exemplified in (5a). To account for the /r/ ~ /ʂ/ alternation, an idiosyncratic /r/ to /ʐ/ change is also needed, as exemplified in (5b), which operates independently of Progressive Devoicing.

  1. (5)

At the surface level, these processes do not apply consistently; consider for example /wɔtrɨ/ łotr-y ‘rascal (nom.pl)’ or /dɔtrɛ/ dotrę ‘I will reach’, in which /r/ is followed respectively by /ɨ/ and /ɛ/, and the examples in (5a.i), in which /ʂ/ occurs in exactly the same contexts. It can further be observed that the /r/ ~ /ʐ/ and /r/ ~ /ʂ/ alternations are often accompanied by /ɛ/ ~ zero alternations inside root morphemes (cf. docierać – dotrze in (5a.i) and brać – bierze in (5b)); likewise, the /ɛ/ ~ zero alternation is seen in szewekszwy in (5a.ii). In such cases, pre-school children have been reported to have difficulty in perceiving the regularity of the connection between different forms of the same morpheme (Łukaszewicz Reference Łukaszewicz2006b: 16–17). Unlike Voice Assimilation, which can be internalised via pure phonotactic learning, Progressive Devoicing requires access to the morphological structure of words. However, words showing these alternations might not be readily available to the child. Not a single instance of the forms łotrzyk, dotrze and krze in (5a.i), which Gussmann (Reference Gussmann2007: 307) adduces as illustration of surface /ʂ/ alternating with /r/, is found in the Polish child-directed speech corpus (Haman et al. Reference Haman, Etenkowski, Łuniewska, Szwabe, Dąbrowska, Szreder and Łaziński2011). (The same problem is found with the form szewek in (5a.ii), and other examples showing the surface /v/ as an alternant of /f/ in Gussmann Reference Gussmann2007.) I conclude that Progressive Devoicing is unlikely to have any relevance at the stages of acquisition reported in this paper.

Another point raised by the anonymous reviewer is the need for acoustic data from adult Polish which would allow us to assess the categoricity of Voice Assimilation and the potential presence of incomplete neutralisation (phonetic gradience of the output reflecting the underlying specification of the target segment; see e.g. Braver Reference Braver2019 for a discussion of the rich literature on the topic). Below I consider acoustic data from adult Polish, which illustrate the effect of Voice Assimilation on the acoustic parameter most reliably connected with the expression of the voicing contrast in adult Polish, voicing percentage (cf. ‘the voicing ratio’ in Strycharczuk Reference Strycharczuk2012: 659).Footnote 8 The measurements were conducted using Praat's Voice Report function (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2017), with pitch ranges adjusted for adult male vs. female speakers (in all other respects, the measurement details were the same as for the child's data in §3.1). The data come from high-quality recordings conducted for the purpose of a previous acoustic study investigating phonetic correlates of word stress in Polish (Łukaszewicz & Rozborski Reference Łukaszewicz and Rozborski2008). In that study, children's speech was compared with that of their parents. Speech samples from three adult speakers (one of whom was J's caregiver) were collected in structured interviews. Because of the high level of acquaintance between the experimenter and the participants, the recorded samples (lasting about 90 minutes in total) reflect casual spontaneous speech in informal settings. This sort of speech can be expected to be least prone to incomplete neutralisation, whose extent is known to vary, depending on the communicative context (Port & Crawford Reference Port and Crawford1989; see also the discussion in Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006: 54–55). The inclusion of the caregiver's data ensures a good approximation of the input to which the child was exposed.

The voicing measurements were conducted for 488 obstruents (244 clusters). The results for each of two obstruents forming a cluster (O1O2) are plotted as coordinates in Fig. 2. The axes represent the voicing percentage; the experimental data points can have values between 0 and 100 on either of the axes. O1 is the target of the process (the horizontal axis), O2 is in presonorant position – it acts as the Voice Assimilation trigger (the vertical axis). S, Z, T and D denote voiceless fricatives, voiced fricatives, voiceless stops and voiced stops respectively. As expected, the data points corresponding to the predicted voiceless Voice Assimilation outputs (145 cluster tokens), e.g. ⫽ZT⫽ and ⫽ST⫽ → /ST/, consistently accumulate in the lower left quarter of the diagram, attracted by the (0,0) point. The predicted voiced Voice Assimilation outputs (99 cluster tokens), e.g. ⫽SD⫽ and ⫽ZD⫽ → /ZD/, consistently accumulate in the upper right quarter; there are 40 data points at exactly (100,100). These cannot be read directly from the diagram, because its axes are continuous. In 70 out of 99 such clusters, O1 (the target of Voice Assimilation) is 100% voiced. The data are divided depending on the underlying specifications of the obstruents in the cluster. The two sets, target voiceless and target voiced, do not overlap.

Figure 2 Voice Assimilation effects (voicing percentages) in adult Polish O1O2 clusters, depending on the underlying voicing of O1 and O2. Voiceless outputs are grey; voiced outputs black. The two sets do not overlap.

There are no incomplete neutralisation effects in target voiceless clusters. In target voiced clusters, the incomplete neutralisation effect on O1 (the Voice Assimilation target) is marginal: there are only four data points in which the underlyingly voiceless O1 has voicing below 80%. These include two clear instances of the suspension of Voice Assimilation, when the underlyingly voiceless O1 retains its voicelessness instead of assimilating to the underlyingly voiced O2 (cf. the two data points near the upper left corner). In general, underlying ⫽ZT⫽, ⫽DS⫽ clusters do not tend to have more voicing in O1 than underlying ⫽ST⫽, ⫽TS⫽ clusters, i.e. they are not shifted towards the lower right quarter of the diagram; underlying ⫽SD⫽, ⫽TZ⫽ clusters do not show less voicing in O1 than underlying ⫽ZD⫽, ⫽DZ⫽ clusters, i.e. they are not shifted towards the upper left quarter of the diagram. For the vast majority of tokens, then, the assimilation effect in O1 is complete. In target voiced clusters, a small ‘gravity’ effect can additionally be observed: O2 (the trigger of assimilation) is sometimes less voiced than the preceding target. We can ascribe this to the biomechanical difficulty of sustaining voicing throughout the entire cluster.Footnote 9

2.3 Hypotheses

The two alternative hypotheses of the interaction between the child's immature production skills and adult phonology can be stated as follows. Will the child-specific fricative devoicing and Voice Assimilation interact in accordance with the ‘feedforward’ scenario, producing an output that is illicit from the point of view of the global adult phonotactics predicted by Voice Assimilation, as in (6a)? Or will it reflect the reversed scenario, with the application of Voice Assimilation conditioned by the child-specific pattern, as in (b)? I assume that the voicing distinction is neutralised only in fricatives; stops in contrastive presonorant contexts are realised in the adult-like manner. As shown earlier in Fig. 2 (§2.2), both ⫽SD⫽ and ⫽ZD⫽ surface as /ZD/ in adult Polish, and both ⫽TZ⫽ and ⫽DZ⫽ surface as /DZ/, so we ignore the differences in underlying representations, which are irrelevant from the point of view of the expected interactions in the child's speech. Also, it is important to note that the hypothesised ‘reversed’ scenario in (6b) is not possible in adult Polish (incomplete neutralisation can potentially affect the target of the neutralising process in adult speech, but never the trigger).

  1. (6)

The diagrams in Fig. 3a schematise the hypothesised position of the child's data points corresponding to target /ZD/ and /DZ/ clusters in the above scenarios. Target voiceless clusters (/ST/, /TS/) are expected to consistently appear in the lower left part of Fig. 3b. In the ‘feedforward’ scenario in Fig. 3a.i, target /ZD/ clusters are expected to occupy the upper left quarter of the diagram, and target /DZ/ clusters the lower right quarter. In the ‘reversed’ scenario in Fig. 3a.ii, realisations of target /ZD/ and /DZ/ are not expected to differ: both are anticipated to appear in the lower left region of the diagram, like the /ST/ and /ST/ clusters in Fig. 3b. The diagrams on the lefthand side of Fig. 3a are based on the assumption that the effects are absolute; those on the righthand side (more plausibly) admit variability of these effects, i.e. some realisations of /DZ/ and /ZD/ may be adult-like. In the latter case, some data points will end up in the upper right quarter, both in Fig. 3a.i and Fig. 3a.ii; cf. the diagram for the adult data in Fig. 2. (For such data, we additionally expect some biomechanically motivated ‘gravity’ effect, such as the one reported for parental speech in §2.2 above.)

Figure 3 (a) Hypothesised position of the target voiced clusters: (i) feedforward; (ii) reversed. (b) Hypothesised position of the target voiceless clusters (adult-like).

3 The acoustic study

3.1 The data and acoustic measurements

The corpus of child data used for acoustic analyses consists of 4 hours 40 minutes of audio recordings of naturalistic speech of a normally developing Polish speaking child, interacting with the experimenter during play sessions. The child (J) is female, aged 2;3–2;8 at the time of the recordings and raised in a monolingual environment in which standard Polish (the Warsaw standard) is spoken. The recordings were divided into seven sessions, conveniently covering the span of 21 weeks during which a shift from a complete neutralisation of voicing in fricatives towards a nearly adult-like surface voicing was observed.

The corpus contains rich audio material of quality sufficient to attempt acoustic analyses.Footnote 10 However, the spontaneous and naturalistic character of the data poses a challenge to acoustic measurement, and requires a rigorous data preselection procedure. The data were rejected for analysis if they were distorted by background sounds (the experimenter's voice, clattering toys, etc.), or if they had low amplitude caused by the child moving away from the microphone. Tokens were also rejected if the obstruent occurred within a larger span of ceased phonation, which sometimes happened towards the end of an utterance.

Some data had to be rejected during segmentation because of interference from certain reduction and substitution patterns. As typical of development between ages 2 and 3, the child's data did not show variability only on the voiced–voiceless dimension. Two phenomena are relevant here: the occasional realisation of fricatives as affricates (e.g. /z/ → [ʥ] in język ‘tongue (nom. sg)’ /jε̃w̃zɨk/ [jɛɲʥik]), and erroneous omission or replacement of sounds. As far as the former issue is concerned, analyses both including and discarding such segments were performed, but this did not have a crucial effect on the outcome (see §3.2 for details). Omission of a sound, resulting in reducing a cluster to a singleton, excluded the token from analysis (e.g. the word gdzie /gʥɛ/ ‘where’ was sometimes rendered as [ʥɛ]). Reduction of obstruent clusters was infrequent at all stages. I also discarded tokens in which an obstruent was replaced by a sonorant or vice versa, e.g. zaśpiewać /zaɕpjɛvaʨ/ [naɕpjɛvaʨ] ‘to sing’, piesek /pjɛsɛk/ [pɕɛsɛk] ‘dog (dim)’ (2;2.30). Such examples occurred only sporadically – with the notable exception of the labiodental /v/, which was replaced by [w] in all contexts. Simultaneously, target /f/ was predominantly rendered as [x], which means that labiodental fricatives were generally absent from J's speech. The focus of the study is thus on clusters containing coronal fricatives. The remaining 1248 obstruent tokens were segmented in Praat; three types of phonetic segments were distinguished: closure, frication and ‘burst’ (positive VOT), as in Fig. 4.

Figure 4 Example of obstruent segmentation: (to) jest drugi (mikrofon) /jεz druɟi/ ‘(it) is another (microphone)’ (J 2;5.10).

As is well-known, phonological contrast in voicing can be expressed in terms of various parameters, depending on the language. The most reliable cue for voicing in adult Polish is the voicing ratio, i.e. glottal pulsing duration divided by closure duration in the case of stops, and by frication duration in the case of fricatives (Strycharczuk Reference Strycharczuk2012: 659). Measurements of a related parameter, voicing percentage (i.e. the percentage of closure/frication during which voicing occurs), were carried out using Praat's Voice Report function, with a pitch range of 200–450 Hz, cross-correlation analysis method and default advanced pitch settings.Footnote 11 The measurements were performed in the sound editor window on the part of the sound corresponding to the earlier annotated closure or frication portion of an obstruent. This allowed for visual inspection of pulses. The outcome of the voicing measurements in fricatives in presonorant position is exemplified in Fig. 5. All three spectrograms represent the target voiced fricative /ʐ/, produced with a different degree of voicing depending on the token: (a) duża /duʐa/ ‘big (fem)’, (b) kucharza /kuxaʐa/ ‘cook (gen.sg)’, (c) dużo /duʐɔ/ ‘a lot’. For stops, I also measured the portion of the speech signal corresponding to the release burst plus any subsequent aspiration, i.e. the positive VOT (see Appendix A3).

Figure 5 Example of voicing measurements in target voiced fricatives, using Praat's Voice Report function: (a) duża /duʐa/ ‘big (fem.nom.sg)’ (J 2;2.30; 23% voicing); (b) kucharza /kuxaʐa/ ‘cook (gen.sg)’ (J 2;5.10; 60% voicing); (c) dużo /duʐɔ/ ‘a lot’ (J 2;7.19; 100% voicing).

Voicing in fricatives and stops was measured independently in singletons in contrastive (presonorant) non-assimilatory contexts and in obstruent clusters (Voice Assimilation contexts); the measurement data are available in the online supplementary materials.Footnote 12 Most target voiced obstruent clusters come from external sandhi contexts, and consist of a sequence of a fricative and a stop, as exemplified in (7a). Among these are tokens containing the word jest ‘is’, in which the final t was omitted (as in casual adult speech). There were also a number of tokens in which the fricative was preceded by a stop (7b), or where either word-internal sandhi (c) or static phonotactics (d) were involved. These were also included, because they were expected to exhibit the same voiced ~ voiceless variation pattern. In the analyses of clusters, I excluded some tokens with fricative–fricative sequences (e) and stop–stop sequences (f), as the lack of clear transitions in such sequences often made their acoustic segmentation infeasible. Both (e) and (f) were rare. Such sequences cannot provide a compelling argument for the ‘feedforward’ vs. ‘reversed’ interaction between child-specific fricative devoicing and Voice Assimilation (although we do expect more target-appropriate voicing in target [+voiced] stop–stop sequences than in target [+voiced] fricative–fricative sequences, because of the presence of fricative devoicing but no stop devoicing in the child's system).

  1. (7)

3.2 Results

The results are presented in Fig. 6, divided into two columns. Column (a) consists of boxplots depicting the percentages of voicing present in voiceless vs. voiced fricatives and stops at each stage, on the basis of measurements in contrasting non-assimilatory positions. (Affricate-like realisations of fricatives were excluded in these analyses.) These results are juxtaposed against the diagrams in column (b). The latter show the percentages of voicing measured in target voiceless/voiced obstruent clusters (assimilatory contexts) at a given stage. Columns (a) and (b) are based on independent measurements (available in the supplementary materials).

Figure 6 Measurement results in non-assimilatory contrastive and assimilatory contexts.

As seen in column (a), the underlying voiceless–voiced distinction is neutralised in fricatives at stage 1. The boxplots for target voiceless and voiced fricatives are more or less the same at this stage. In contrast, the underlying voiceless–voiced distinction in stops is clearly reflected in J's speech throughout the entire period of development under investigation, including stage 1. The shift in the surface expression of the underlying voiced fricative category – from phonetically voiceless towards phonetically voiced segments – is marked by greater variability, starting at stage 2. At stages 3–6, the means for target voiced fricatives fluctuate around 40% voicing. At stage 7, the mean reaches a considerably higher value of about 64%, and the data begin to exhibit a strongly negative skewness (the distribution expected from the point of view of the adult system). In sum, at stages 2–7, child-specific fricative devoicing gradually disappears, yielding a gradient phonetic output for underlying voiced fricatives. The ranges for voicing values are wide, occupying nearly the entire 0–100% space at these stages, and indicating developing cognitive control over the phonetic dimension rather than simply the relaxation of some biomechanical constraints on attainable voicing thresholds. The scatterplot in Fig. 7 shows that there is no clearly discernible bimodal distribution of the data points at these stages. In statistical analyses (using Generalised Linear Models fitted in SPSS, with gamma distributionFootnote 13 and the Log link function) with voicing percentage as the dependent variable, the difference between voiced and voiceless fricatives does not reach the level of significance at stages 1 and 2 (stage 1: χ 2(1) = 0.034, p = 0.853; stage 2: χ 2(1) = 1.861, p = 0.173), but is statistically significant throughout stages 3–7 (stage 3: χ 2(1) = 5.633, p < 0.05; stage 4: χ 2(1) = 5.511, p < 0.05; stage 5: χ 2(1) = 6.048, p < 0.05; stage 6: χ 2(1) = 17.032, p < 0.0001; stage 7: χ 2(1) = 24.643, p < 0.0001). (The p values were obtained via log-likelihood ratio tests comparing the fitted models with intercept-only models.) Voiced stops were significantly different from voiceless stops at all stages (p < 0.0001); see Appendix B for these statistics, the SPSS code used to obtain these statistics and the goodness-of-fit details.

Figure 7 Scatterplots for voicing percentages in target voiced fricatives for each of the seven stages.

In column (b) in Fig. 6, the voicing percentage results for each of two obstruents O1O2 forming a cluster are plotted as coordinates on a diagram. At every stage, there are no data points in the upper left quarter of the diagram. This means that there were no unassimilated clusters of the *[ɕgɔda] type hypothesised as part of the ‘feedforward’ scenario earlier in Fig. 3a.i. Furthermore, most tokens occur in the lower left and the upper right quarters. Thus the data show polarisation similar to the adult system in Fig. 2, suggesting the presence of Voice Assimilation in the child's system. However, the distribution of the target voiced clusters is not exactly adult-like: while all target voiceless obstruents are found in the lower left quarter of the diagram, as expected from the point of view of the adult system, some target voiced clusters also appear in this region; cf. the circled areas in the diagrams in Fig. 6b. The phenomenon is compatible with the hypothesised ‘reversed variable’ scenario in Fig. 3a.ii above. Target /ZD/ and /DZ/ clusters behave similarly. Further, analogously to the adult input in Fig. 2, target voiced O1O2 clusters whose realisations fall outside the circled ‘voiceless’ regions, and which can be tentatively classified as phonetically ‘voiced’ outputs, have on average higher percentage voicing values in O1 than in O2 (the mean difference between the two segments amounts to 20% voicing; t(41) = 4.719, p < 0.0001). The problem of lower percentage voicing values in O2 in such clusters must be connected with the difficulty of sustaining voicing throughout a cluster. Probably, then, it is a biomechanical rather than a cognitive effect.

Finally, as illustrated in Fig. 8, it is of no consequence whether target adult /ZD/ corresponds to the abstract adult underlying representation ⫽SD⫽ or ⫽ZD⫽, and /DZ/ to ⫽TZ⫽ or ⫽DZ⫽. (Ambiguous cases such as the word-final /ʂ/ ~ /ʐ/ alternation in the word już in (3b) above, which always depends on Final Devoicing or Voice Assimilation, were coded as ⫽SD⫽ in this analysis.) Regardless of the underlying specification of O1 (the Voice Assimilation target) as voiced or voiceless in adult Polish, the variable voiced ~ voiceless realisations of such clusters occur in J's speech.

Figure 8 Voicing percentage in target voiced fricative–stop and stop-fricative clusters in J's speech (stages 1–7), depending on the underlying voicing of O1 in adult Polish.

The monotonic disappearance of child-specific fricative devoicing (or, rather, the growth of the voicing contrast in these segments) is illustrated in Fig. 9. Unlike the realisation of target voiced stops, which is stable and largely adult-like throughout the development, the average extent of voicing in target voiced fricatives is to a substantial degree parallel to the extent of the successful avoidance of ‘late repairs’ in target voiced obstruent clusters containing fricatives. The inclusion of affricate-like realisations of fricatives does not have a significant impact on the analysis.

Figure 9 Voicing emerging in target [+voiced] fricatives vs. stops vs. obstruent clusters for each of the seven stages.

As further shown in Fig. 10, the two phenomena are correlated (r(7) = 0.974, p < 0.001). This indicates that progress in one domain (voicing in fricatives in non-assimilatory contrastive contexts) is paralleled by progress in the other domain (target-appropriate voicing in obstruent clusters). (Recall that the scores for voicing in fricatives and voicing in clusters are based on datasets which are entirely independent: although they are expressed in percentages, they do not constitute proportions in the sense of compositional data.) The ‘success score’ for target-appropriate realisations of voiced O1O2 clusters was calculated using SPSS on the basis of k-means classification into ‘voiceless’ and ‘voiced’, run to fit a two-group model; the clusters classified as ‘voiceless’ in these analyses correspond to those that are circled in Fig. 7b. The correlation remains significant (r(7) = 0.961, p < 0.001), with Voice Assimilation scores recalculated using Laplace's rule of succession, which is useful in estimating underlying probabilities when observations are small in number (or when s = {0, n}). (The expectation was calculated with the formula (s + 1)/(n + 2), where s is the number of ‘successes’ and n the number of observations (‘opportunities’).)

Figure 10 Fricative voicing (gradient) vs. Voice Assimilation (categorical) yielding target-appropriate voiced outputs.

Target voiced stops and fricatives also differ in how their variability profiles change over time, as shown in Fig. 11; variability was measured in terms of relative standard deviation (RSD = 100 × SD/Mean). A shift from voicing contrast neutralisation towards target-appropriate surface contrast in fricatives was marked by enhanced variability; cf. the temporarily increased RSDs, in particular at stages 2–4. Although the dispersion remains considerably high throughout stages 2–7, the RSDs become lower, because of the simultaneously increasing means. The category of voiced stops was relatively target-appropriate and stable throughout the stages (their RSDs did not rise). As highlighted in §4, these differences in variability profiles are meaningful in the dynamical systems approach, in which the enhanced gradience is symptomatic of the grammar undergoing a shift from one organisational mode to another. As the mean voicing percentage increases, the system undergoes a qualitative change from a stable contrast neutralisation to a stable contrast realisation, necessarily passing through an intermediate stage of instability.

Figure 11 RSD voicing percentage in target [+voiced] stops vs. fricatives for each of the seven stages.

4 Symbolic-dynamical representations

The acoustic results in §3.2 suggest an interaction in which the application of an adult-based categorical process (Voice Assimilation) is conditioned by a child-specific gradient process (fricative devoicing). This runs against the predictions of the standard feedforward modular grammar. In §4.1, I discuss how the behaviour of the developing phonological system can be understood if we assume symbol-like dynamical representations. In brief, the proposal is that child-specific patterns can be formalised in terms of the consequences of scaling the control parameters of the dynamical system. A continuous shift in the control parameter, reflecting the growth of the phonetic capacity in the child, brings about qualitative differences in the dynamical landscape, which is phonological organisation.

4.1 Phonological grammar as a dynamical landscape: basic notions

Dynamical systems can be viewed as attractor landscapes, formally expressed through non-linear mathematics (for an overview see Thelen & Smith Reference Thelen and Smith1994, especially Chapter 3). Attractors are behavioural modes, which the system develops (via self-organisation) as a function of the interaction of its internal components and their responsiveness to external conditions. Dynamical principles, either stated explicitly in terms of differential equations or only alluded to at the level of metaphor, have been widely applied in natural sciences (for a mathematical introduction, with emphasis on applications, see Strogatz Reference Strogatz1994). A wealth of illustrative examples, ranging from modelling the Belousov–Zhabotinsky oscillatory reaction in chemistry to applications in motor and cognitive development research (for example on the ontogeny of treadmill stepping in infants or the puzzle of the Piagetian A-not-B error), can be found in Thelen & Smith (Reference Thelen and Smith1994). Of interest here is the possibility of conceptualising the relationship between phonological organisation and phonetic parameters using these principles. The symbolic-dynamical representations described below are in terms of first-order differential equations, as formalised in Gafos (Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006). The mathematical formalism of dynamical systems has been used in various ways to model phonological–phonetic phenomena in adult speech: articulatory gestures defined as invariant mathematical laws (Kelso et al. Reference Kelso, Saltzman and Tuller1986, Browman & Goldstein Reference Browman, Goldstein, Port and van Gelder1995), (in)stability of speech perception (Tuller et al. Reference Tuller, Case, Ding and Scott Kelso1994), incomplete neutralisation (Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006, Gafos & Benus Reference Gafos and Benus2006), shifts in phonetic indices dependent on qualitative differences in syllable parses (Gafos et al. Reference Gafos, Charlow, Shaw and Hoole2014) and perception-induced updates in speech production planning (Roon & Gafos Reference Roon and Gafos2016), to mention just a few examples. For a non-mathematical treatment of the shift in phonological organisation from babbling to first words, espousing the dynamical systems approach, see Vihman et al. (Reference Vihman, DePaolis, Keren-Portnoy, Bavin and Naigles2015).

As illustrated in (8), the behaviour of a dynamical system can be pictured as a particle landing in potential wells. The position of the particle is the system's state x at some time t. The wells reflect the system's preferred modes, the so-called ‘attractors’; the relative depth and steepness of the wells reflects the attractors’ relative stability. The peaks are unstable points (repellers): the system is unlikely to reside in these positions.

  1. (8)

Attractors are the states towards which the system tends, and thus correspond to mean values of parameters in experimental data. How close x is with respect to those target values at time t depends on its initial position and its trajectory, determined by a differential equation relating dx/dt (the instantaneous velocity of x) to the so-called force function referring to the attractor's slope (for clarification see below). It also depends on the amount of noise by which the system is perturbed. (Internal fluctuations are a consequence of interaction of multiple subcomponents of a complex system.) A relatively stable attractor is less prone to change; (8c) is an intuitive illustration. When perturbed to a relevant degree by internal noise or external forces, the particle is likely to visit the deeper well of the attractor to the left instead of dropping back into the shallow well of the attractor to the right. Globally, i.e. when starting from different initial positions, the system is more likely to reside in the former attractor than in the latter. The measure of relative variability around a mean state is a powerful tool in assessing the degree of the attractor's stability. Thus, a major progress offered by the dynamical approach is that it allows us to treat a certain kind of variation in experimental data as information, not noise; cf. the RSDs for target [+voiced] fricatives vs. stops in Fig. 11 above, indicative of unstable vs. stable categories.

Characteristically, dynamical systems produce coherent patterns only within a certain range of values of their control parameters. As the control parameter is scaled continuously, it passes through a critical value at which a qualitative transition in the attractor landscape (so-called bifurcation) occurs. Under different conditions, the components of a complex dynamical system are free to reassemble into different stable modes. It is this ‘soft assembling’ property to which dynamical systems owe their enormous flexibility; it is also why they come with the potential to model developmental data in an insightful way (Thelen & Smith Reference Thelen and Smith1994: 60). One of the primary research goals is then to single out factors as potential control parameters that affect the evolution of a dynamical system.

The geometric characterisation of the dynamical landscape in (8) allows us to express the notion of phonological contrast in an intuitively straightforward way. The attractors reflect the macroscopic observable, i.e. the phonological organisation of the system. Given some continuous physical parameter (e.g. the extent of vocal fold vibration), the absence vs. presence of a binary phonological contrast (e.g. a voiced–voiceless distinction) corresponds to a single-attractor vs. two-attractor system, as in (8a) and (b) respectively. The system's state x at time t corresponds to measurable phonetic output. In the dynamical view of the phonology–phonetics link, phonology is not antecedent to phonetics, as no translation from discrete symbolic to physical continuous is necessary. The continuous (phonetic) and discrete (phonological) properties of speech are inseparable, yet distinguishable. The qualitative and quantitative aspects of the system are both expressed with non-linear mathematics (cf. Browman & Goldstein Reference Browman, Goldstein, Port and van Gelder1995, Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006).

The schematic illustrations of different dynamical systems in (8) are based on polynomials of different degrees.Footnote 14 These are potential functions, V(x), defined over some order parameters (e.g. the degree of voicing). Their minima correspond to attractors, i.e. stable equilibrium points at which a particle will eventually land (assuming deterministic dynamical systems), starting from different initial positions. Their maxima are repellers, i.e. unstable equilibrium points. Both attractors and repellers are equilibria in the sense that the rate of change is zero at these points, i.e. dV(x)/dx = 0. The number and depth of attractors depend on the degree of the polynomial as well as on the value of its control parameters, which will be discussed below with reference to grammatical VG(x) and lexical (intentional) potentials VI(x), as defined in Gafos (Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006). Before we turn to exploring the role of these potentials in shaping the attractor landscape in child vs. adult phonologies, let us consider a basic characterisation of stability in mathematical terms.

The essence of symbolic-dynamical representations is that they are assembled in time and are understood as mathematically defined laws. They are defined in terms of differential equations, which have the general form dx/dt = ―dV(x)/dx. (For simplicity, I ignore the noise component which occurs on the righthand side of the equation and introduces indeterminacy in the system's behaviour; see Gafos & Benus Reference Gafos and Benus2006: 908–909.) The lefthand side of the equation is the rate of change of x (with respect to t). The righthand side of the equation corresponds to the force function, which is the negative of the first derivative of the potential function V(x). The mathematical relationship between the potential, V(x), and the force function, F(x) = ―dV(x)/dx, reflects a well-established relationship for oscillating systems in physics. It is the relationship between the potential energy and the restoring force which acts on an oscillating system to bring it back to its equilibrium. The restoring force depends on the degree of displacement – linearly in harmonic, non-linearly in anharmonic systems. Its direction is opposite to the direction of displacement, hence the minus sign in ―dV(x)/dx. First-order dynamical equations, dx/dt = ―dV(x)/dx, express the evolution of the system in time. It is important to note that they have equilibria solutions, not periodic solutions (see Strogatz Reference Strogatz1994: 28). The solution to the equation is a function of x with respect to t, or, to be more precise, an infinite set of functions for an infinitude of different initial conditions (x 0). The solutions are trajectories which converge towards the stable fixed point(s) and diverge from the unstable fixed point(s).

Convergence towards the stable fixed points (attractors) is illustrated in Fig. 12 on the basis of relatively simple single-attractor dynamics: dx/dt =―dV(x)/dx = ―2x and dx/dt = ―dV(x)/dx = ―½x respectively. As we will see later, single-attractor dynamics generalised as dx/dt = ―dV(x)/dx = ―αx, with α > 0, can be considered to be a dynamical equivalent of lexical representations, because they specify the target value of a phonological feature unambiguously, regardless of the shifts in the system's control parameter α. The solutions to these differential equations, which are x(t) = x 0e 2t and x(t) = x 0e ½t respectively, are plotted in the (iii) panels; in both plots the system's initial states are assumed to take the arbitrarily selected values x 0 = {―20, ―15, ―10, ―5, 0, 5, 10, 15, 20}.Footnote 15 We can readily observe how the differences in the attractors’ stability (steepness of the wells) in the (i) panels in Fig. 12 correspond to the differences in slopes (the force functions) in (ii), and ultimately, to the degree of variability in the output data at a given time point in (iii). For example, if we assume that the decision time for assembling representations during the speech planning process is t = 1 (indicated by the arrow), this renders output xt values in Fig. 12a much less variable relative to Fig. 12b.

Figure 12 (a) A more stable attractor; (b) a less stable attractor.

Differences in stability are one of several expected differences between the adult and child phonological systems, making the latter more liable to change. The systems in Fig. 12 are nevertheless qualitatively the same, i.e. their trajectories converge towards the same stable fixed point.

As proposed in Gafos (Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006: 62), a two-way surface phonological contrast (e.g. in voicing) can be represented in terms of the quartic function in Fig. 13, VG(x) = kx − ½x 2 + ¼x 4, where k stands for a control parameter. (The quartic degree is the lowest polynomial degree allowing us to have two minima and one maximum.) The dynamics of phonological grammar are defined in terms of the differential equation: G(x) = dx/dt =―dV(x)/dx = ―k + x − x 3. The G(x) formula is referred to as the ‘tilted anharmonic oscillator’ in Gafos (Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006: 66), and proposed as a first approximation of the grammar dynamics.Footnote 16 (See Tuller et al. Reference Tuller, Case, Ding and Scott Kelso1994: 7 for an earlier application of the same formula in the domain of speech perception.) By scaling the parameter k, we obtain a qualitative shift (bifurcation) between a single-attractor system and a two-attractor system, which corresponds to phonological grammars characterised by the absence vs. presence of binary contrast, as in Fig. 13. The bistable organisation emerges only at some critical (―kc) value of k, and is sustained only within a certain range of k ∈ (―kc, kc). The bistable grammatical potential is symmetrical when k = 0: the chances of a particle landing in either of the two attractors is then equal, as in Fig. 13a. I take this symmetrical potential to represent the adult-like voiced–voiceless distinction.

Figure 13 Grammatical potentials: V G (x) = kx−1/2x 2 + 1/4x 4.

Expanding Gafos’ hypothesis to the domain of acquisition, there can be a twofold difference between the child's system and the corresponding adult system from the point of view of the grammar dynamics. First, there may be only one attractor in the child's system, resulting in absolute contrast neutralisation, as in Fig. 13b; cf. the behaviour of target voiced fricatives at the initial stage reported in this paper, when they are not distinguished from target voiceless fricatives in J's outputs. (The one-attractor system in (b) can also represent contextual voicing neutralisation in adult speech, e.g. the cross-linguistically common patterns of coda devoicing, as described in Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006.) Second, there may be two attractors in the child's system, but their strength may be unequal, as illustrated in (c). With the latter representation, variable contrast neutralisation ensues; cf. the unstable behaviour of target voiced fricatives at some stages of J's development reported in this paper. The depth of the well of the ‘voiced’ attractor gets bigger as the k parameter is scaled up from the critical value ―kc towards 0 (to ultimately match the stability of the ‘voiceless’ attractor at k = 0). Thus, given the three different landscapes in Fig. 13, the predicted order of acquisition is: (b) (initial stage), (c) (intermediate stage), (a) (adult-like stage).

With the grammatical potential supplying two competing attractors for binary contrast (voiced–voiceless in presonorant position), we need a mechanism to ensure that the feature is realised unambiguously in lexical items. In Gafos (Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006), lexical features are expressed in terms of intention dynamics, I(x) = dx/dt = α(xREQ − x), supplying attractors at the required voicing values (xREQ); α is a control parameter and expresses the strength of the intention. Accordingly, in Fig. 14 I assume (arbitrarily) that the values xREQ = ―1 and xREQ = 1 correspond to the categories ‘voiced’ and ‘voiceless’ respectively. The intention parameter in the illustration in Fig. 14 is assumed to take the value of α = 1. The ‘voiced’ vs. ‘voiceless’ intentional dynamics are mutually exclusive.

Figure 14 Intentional (lexical) potentials: V I(x) = ɑ(1/2x 2x REQ × x).

In what follows, I assume that every output can be viewed as a combined effect of the grammar dynamics and the intention dynamics, as in (9). For simplicity, the weighting of the grammar dynamics will be kept constant (wG = 1). This idealisation reflects the expected early acquisition of contrasts and phonotactics (recall the discussion in §1 and §2). The relative weighting will be achieved by allowing the control parameter α = wI of the intention dynamics to assume different values within the range from 0 to 1 (cf. Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006: 70).

  1. (9)

Let us first consider a straightforward case of a fully symmetrical voiced–voiceless contrast in adult speech. In the dynamical interpretation, grammar predicts a voiced–voiceless alternation in presonorant contexts, which needs to be lexically disambiguated. The control parameter in the grammar dynamics is k = 0 (as in (Fig. 13a) above). The values xREQ = {―1, 1} correspond to voiced and voiceless categories respectively. If the weights of both the grammar and intention dynamics are set to wG = wI = 1, we find unambiguously voiced or voiceless outputs, because the resultant force function ―dV(x)/dx = G(x) + I(x) = ―x 3 + xREQ assumes the value of zero only at one point: xREQ = ―1 for the potential in Fig. 15a vs. xREQ = 1 for the potential in (b). Note that this function does not have a linear term, which precludes the possibility of having two attractors in the corresponding potential function.

Figure 15 Adult surface contrast as a consequence of combining Grammar and Intention. The solid black lines represent the combined potentials.

4.2 Contrast emergence in the dynamical landscape

The relative weighting of the grammar and intention dynamics may be different in the child and the adult. Given the order of acquisition, intention may be expected to be weighted lower in children. If the grammar potential is symmetrical (i.e. k = 0), and the weight of intention is as low as 0.3, monostable combined potentials still result, as depicted in Fig. 16. Such a system will produce target-appropriate voiced and voiceless fricatives, although the distinction between the two categories will be much less stable than in the adult system in Fig. 15 above. Nevertheless, the systems in Figs 15 and 16 are qualitatively the same: they are characterised by the same attractors, xREQ = {―1, 1}.

Figure 16 Weaker (less stable) surface contrast due to weaker Intention (w I = ɑ = 0.3).

The mirror images of the voiced and voiceless categories in Fig. 16 are inadequate from the point of view of the asymmetric behaviour of target voiced and voiceless fricatives for most of the stages reported in this paper. The initial bias towards monostable voiceless realisations and the subsequent emergence of bistability (variation) in the target voiced fricatives (and obstruent clusters containing these segments) must follow from changes in the control parameter k. As the parameter gradually shifts from negative values upward towards zero, which reflects the child's growing capacity to produce voicing in fricatives, the landscape changes from asymmetric to symmetric, passing through a critical point where a qualitative shift from a monostable (single-attractor) to a bistable (two-attractor) system occurs. In Fig. 17, I illustrate predicted successive stages of development of the surface voicing contrast in fricatives, assuming k = {―0.5, ―0.3, ―0.1}. The weighting of the intention dynamics is also assumed to shift, reflecting the expected gradual refinement of lexical representations (hypothetically proportional to the development of production skills): wI = {0.15, 0.2, 0.25}; see Appendix C for a summary of these formulas. For simplicity, we model development only at three points: contrast neutralisation (Fig. 17a), variable contrast with weak stability (b) and still variable but more stable contrast (c). The changes in the parameter k crucially influence the modes of stability in target voiced fricatives (the (i) panels), but do not have a major effect on target voiceless fricatives (the (ii) panels). For the latter, the pattern is monostable throughout the reported stages. (The scale is the same in all graphs.)

Figure 17 (a) No surface contrast: target voiced fricatives are realised as voiceless. (b) Loss of stability in the child-specific fricative devoicing pattern: targetappropriate and voiceless renditions of target voiced fricatives concur. (c) Target voiced fricatives are realised in a nearly adult-like manner.

Fig. 17a illustrates the stage at which the voicing contrast in fricatives is neutralised completely in the child's speech. Despite differences in intention (cf. the parabolas drawn as dashed lines), target voiced (i) vs. target voiceless (ii) outputs are not rendered contrastively: both are characterised by a single ‘voiceless’ attractor. The grammatical component (k = ―0.5), which supplies a single ‘voiceless’ attractor, is too strong to be outweighed by the contradictory intention (wI = 0.15) supplying a weaker ‘voiced’ attractor in (a.i). Hence there is no qualitative difference between (a.i) and (a.ii).

Fig. 17b depicts the stage at which target voiced fricatives are produced variably as voiced or voiceless in the child's speech. The parameter k assumes a slightly higher value in comparison with the previous stage (k = ―0.3). The linear combination of the grammar and the ‘voiced’ intention allows a second attractor to emerge in the combined potential (b.i). However, the value of k is still too low to yield a monostable ‘voiced’ pattern. With the same grammar but the opposite, i.e. ‘voiceless’, intention, we get the monostable ‘voiceless’ potential in (b.ii).

In Fig. 17c, a further shift in the parameter k towards 0 (k = ―0.1) produces nearly mirror-image landscape patterns for target voiced (c.i) and voiceless fricatives (c.ii); cf. Fig. 16 above. In (c.i), the voiced attractor is now much more stable; the competing voiceless attractor is almost negligible. Voiceless fricatives, as in previous stages, are represented by a consistently monostable ‘voiceless’ potential.

The dynamical account of gradient contrast neutralisation in child speech relies on a trade-off between grammar and intention specifying contradictory attractors for target voiced segments. The same mechanism has been applied to account for incomplete neutralisation in adult speech (Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006). However, the role of grammar vs. intention is different in these two accounts. In adult speech, the gradient surface effect is caused by an increase in the intention weight (the lexical factor), while grammar is kept constant. In child speech, grammar is affected by the shift in the control parameter k, which reflects development of a phonetic skill.

I now turn to the question of how combining the grammar and intention dynamics predicts Voice Assimilation ‘late repairs’ in target voiced obstruent clusters containing fricatives in the child's system. In regressive Voice Assimilation, the voicing specification of the trigger (O2) is anticipated in the target of assimilation (O1). The simplest way in which this effect can be formalised within the dynamical systems approach is in terms of a linear combination of the grammatical potential for the entire cluster (with the k parameter reflecting the current capacity of producing voicing in fricatives) and the lexical (monostable) potential of the trigger (O2). Assembling voicing for the entire cluster, rather than for each segment separately, mimics the effect of the top-ranked Agreeobstr[±vcd] constraint in OT. On this account, the predicted development of target voiced obstruent clusters fully parallels the development of target voiced fricatives, as illustrated in Fig. 17. This is because the intention dynamics in Fig. 17 correspond to underlying voicing specifications for an obstruent in presonorant position, which is the contrastive context, regardless of whether this obstruent is a singleton or part of a cluster, whether it is a stop or a fricative. For example, in spytać /spɨtaʨ/ ‘to ask (pfv)’ vs. zbadać /zbadaʨ/ ‘to examine (pfv)’ (but also in pytać /pɨtaʨ/ ‘to ask’ vs. badać /badaʨ/ ‘to examine’, as well as in szyć /ʂɨʨ/ ‘to sew’ vs. żyć /ʐɨʨ/ ‘to live’), the voicing contrast in /p/ vs. /b/ (or /ʂ/ vs. /ʐ/) in presonorant position will be the basis for the voiceless vs. voiced intention I(x). Combining it with the grammar dynamics G(x) (whose attractor landscape is influenced by the control parameter k) will eventually determine the surface voicing of the entire cluster. In consequence, the same dynamics pertain to target voiced fricatives in non-assimilatory contrastive contexts and in clusters because in both the intention I(x) is voiced, and the grammar G(x) has the same tilt defined by the current value of the control parameter k (the capacity of combining voicing with frication). (This account ignores the lower voicing values for O2, which were ascribed to biomechanical effects in §2.2 and §3.2 above.)

4.3 Discussion

The dynamical systems approach offers unified accounts of the interaction between categorical and gradient factors in phonological acquisition and of the enhanced gradience and variability accompanying the shift from one stable behavioural mode to another. The continuous shift in the control parameter, reflecting the growth of phonetic capacity, brings about qualitative differences in the dynamical landscape, i.e. phonological organisation.

The dynamical landscape offers a conceptualisation of the phonology–phonetics interface which does not require translating phonological representations into phonetic ones. Crucially, it does so without obliterating the distinction between categorical phonological and gradient phonetic factors. It is worth emphasising here that although symbolic dynamical representations might seem to constitute a major departure from traditional symbolic representations in phonology – as they not only link form and substance, but also structure and process, in this respect hardly resembling phonological primitives such as features – in essence they are no less discrete or symbolic than these standard representations. Like these representations, they comply with ‘the tenet of constancy’ (in the sense of Jackendoff Reference Jackendoff1992: 5), and constitute ‘a form of mental information … an organized combinatorial space of distinctions available to the brain’ (Jackendoff Reference Jackendoff1992: 3).

The idea that speech sounds are not just physiological acts, but have psychological reality, is fundamental to phonology, and appears in the earliest writings in the field (e.g. Baudouin de Courtenay Reference Baudouin de Courtenay1891, Sapir Reference Sapir1925). In Browman & Goldstein's (Reference Browman, Goldstein, Port and van Gelder1995: 177) words, ‘the fundamental insight of phonology … is that the pronunciation of the words in a given language may differ from … one another in only a restricted number of ways: the number of degrees of freedom actually employed in this contrastive behavior is far fewer than the number that is mechanically available’. However, it needs to be underscored that the reduction of the degrees of freedom taking place in the non-linear dynamical landscape goes well beyond grouping physical entities into phonological categories. An essential property of the hypothesised grammatical potential VG(x) is its capacity to evolve into a number of qualitatively different attractor regimes as a consequence of scaling the system's control parameters. Under this view, a speech sound participates in an organised space of distinctions which has a certain well-defined topology. Organisation then entails not only static relations, but also paradigms for change.

The system's evolution predicted by the grammar dynamics, G(x), has systematic components which seem amenable to rigorous study: the control and order parameters of the system. A continuous shift in the control parameter (in the present study, the child's developing motor skill) results in a qualitative change in the system (here contrast emergence). Enhanced variability and gradience in the order parameter (here voicing percentage) accompany this shift. Although engaging meaningfully in the non-trivial task of identifying these parameters is clearly more challenging for cognitive systems than for mechanical systems (Thelen & Smith Reference Thelen and Smith1994), natural candidates for control parameters within the phonology–phonetics domain seem to be speech rate, aerodynamic and motor factors, but also communicative contexts and the lexicon; for example, Gafos (Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006) proposes to model the extent of incomplete neutralisation in adult speech by scaling the dynamical system's intentional (lexical) control parameters. It is noteworthy that studying such systematic drifts in the system's behaviour is not possible in other approaches which postulate an intimate phonology–phonetics link (Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006: 57). Exemplar-based models (e.g. Pierrehumbert Reference Pierrehumbert, Bybee and Hopper2001, Reference Pierrehumbert, Gussenhoven and Warner2002) derive contextual (stylistic, speech rate, etc.) variation in the phonetic output from random variation over the exemplar cloud, but are unable to capture the systematicity of the system's responsiveness to environment as some parameter is varied. A similar problem is encountered within contemporary non-serialist approaches to phonology that model gradient phonetic output (e.g. unified weighted constraints; Flemming Reference Flemming2001), or output variation of a categorical kind (e.g. stochastic OT; Boersma & Hayes Reference Boersma and Hayes2001). As briefly discussed below, although these approaches offer good approximations of some narrow aspects of the developing system's behaviour, they do not shed light on the central issue: the role of the phonology–phonetics interface in linking the developing phonological grammar with the child's phonetic capacity, with increased variability/gradience being the necessary symptoms of a qualitative change.

In the unified approach of Flemming (Reference Flemming2001), weighted constraints can yield intermediate outputs in response to competing demands. In OT, child-specific fricative devoicing can be expressed in terms of a high-ranked *Fric[+vcd]. Assigning progressively less weight to *Fric[+vcd] (in this approach understood as a MinimiseEffort constraint) relative to Ident[+obstr, +vcd] will incrementally diminish the influence of the former constraint on the child's output at subsequent stages of development. (Conversely, assigning more weight to the lexical factor, e.g. via paradigm uniformity, can account for incomplete neutralisation in adult speech; see Braver Reference Braver2019.) The unified approach can thus predict the gradual increase in the mean voicing percentage in fricatives. However, what falls outside the scope of this approach is the extent of gradience, which suggests that what is at stake is the child's developing psychomotor control of voicing, i.e. honing production skills to combine voicing with frication, rather than relaxing a biomechanical threshold on the voicing percentage that the production system can attain. Furthermore, because the distinction between categoricity and gradience is not expressed in this approach, it also loses sight of the non-linear character of the phonology–phonetics link. As a result, adopting unified weighted constraints is not helpful in predicting the categorical voiced–voiceless switches in target [+voiced] obstruent clusters. Instead of the reported polarised patterns recurring across the stages, we would expect an upward movement of a single collection of data points over time. This is not what happens.

Variable interaction between categorical and gradient factors in child speech reported in this paper also falls outside the scope of classical stochastic OT models designed to account only for categorical variation (Boersma & Hayes Reference Boersma and Hayes2001). (Likewise, incomplete neutralisation in adult speech, whose extent varies depending on the communicative context, is not covered by these models; see Gafos Reference Gafos, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006.) Stochastic OT ranks constraints on a scale and adds a random value drawn from a Gaussian distribution during the evaluation process; the presence of evaluation noise results in variable rankings, hence categorical variation in the output. Assuming Stochastic OT, the grammar shift observed in acquisition can be approximated in the following way. *Fric[+vcd] can be ranked on the top of the hierarchy and then gradually shifted downwards relative to Identpreson[±vcd]. (At the same time, the comprehension grammar exhibits the adult-like ranking acquired at the primary stages of acquisition, as outlined in §2.2.) A substantial overlap between the two constraints in terms of their Gaussian distributions will produce variable rankings: *Fric[+vcd] ⪢ Identpreson[±vcd] and Identpreson[±vcd] ⪢ *Fric[+vcd], and, in consequence, categorical variation in the output (both in singleton voiced fricatives and in clusters, given that Agreeobstr[±vcd] also starts top-ranked). The degree of overlap between the two constraints, and hence of categorical variation in the output, will gradually decrease over time, eventually matching the stable ranking of the two constraints in the comprehension grammar. In this account, the gradual maturation of voicing control in the child is irrelevant in attaining the adult-like grammar. Somewhat paradoxically then, the motivation for the separate production grammar component with the top-ranked *Fric[+vcd], in defiance of the adult stimulus, is the child's initial lack of phonetic skills, yet changes in the production grammar are prompted by the adult stimulus, not by development of the child's psychomotor control of voicing.

In the dynamical landscape, even though child-specific patterns are viewed as having their origin in performance-related factors, they do not fall outside the purview of the phonological grammar. Formalising the development of the phonetic skill as a continuous shift in the control parameter of a dynamical system accounts for a qualitative change from one stable mode to another (in this paper, the absence or presence of contrast in voicing) through an intermediate stage of enhanced variation and gradience. Such discontinuities in development seem to find solid support in the widely reported characteristics of child-specific substitution and reduction patterns. The interaction between child-specific patterns and adult-based phonology requires further empirical research, involving both longitudinal observations and controlled experimentation.

5 Conclusions

This paper has provided an argument for the computational relevance of child-specific patterns, based on the interaction between fricative devoicing, a phonetically gradient child-specific pattern, and Voice Assimilation, a categorical process of adult Polish, reported in the longitudinal data from a Polish-speaking child. The interaction is not predicted by the traditional modular feedforward approach: the phonetic character of the child-specific pattern (lack of phonetic skill) did not prevent it from conditioning the application of the adult-based phonological process. The acquisitional data were analysed applying insights from the dynamical systems approach. The dynamical landscape, expressed formally in the universal language of non-linear mathematics, integrates the discrete and continuous aspects of speech, while keeping them distinct. It also offers us tools for the study of the underlying mechanisms of change. The proposal put forward in this paper was to express child-specific patterns, arising out of performance-related effects (lack of production skills) as consequences of shifts in control parameters of a dynamical system (phonological organisation). The symbolic dynamical representations were found to provide a convenient expression of (i) temporary systematicity, (ii) enhanced gradience during transition towards target-appropriate productions and (iii) the ultimate disappearance of child-specific patterns, all of which are widely known characteristics of these patterns. They also predict a reciprocal relationship between child-specific patterns and adult-based phonological organisation in the child, as reported in this study. Such interactions have largely been overlooked in previous research on phonological acquisition, and await further investigation as an interesting source of information about the phonology–phonetics link in developing systems.

Footnotes

I would like to thank three anonymous reviewers, an associate editor and the editors of Phonology for their constructive criticism, which was very helpful in improving both the form and content of the paper. I also wish to express my gratitude to Adamantios Gafos for his very helpful feedback at various stages of the research. Special thanks go to Jula, as well as the adult informants, for their enthusiastic participation in the recording sessions. All errors are solely mine.

Supplementary materials containing appendices and data files are available at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675721000051.

1 The term ‘child-specific’ refers to a pattern that has no parallel in the ambient language, and as such needs to be discarded in the course of development. The focus here is on child-specific patterns typical of phonological organisation of the third year of life. Overall, beyond the first 18 months of age, the child speech output is likely to exhibit segment-like organisation resembling adult speech (cf. the ‘phoneme plane’ of Werker & Curtin Reference Werker and Curtin2005), making it possible to draw correspondences between child and adult forms in terms of phone-sized units; it is also the period when children begin to concatenate words into larger utterances; see the discussion in MacNeilage (Reference MacNeilage, Hardcastle and Laver1997: 326ff).

2 This early sensitivity does not automatically entail full lexical contrast specification at later stages. In 14-month-olds, the capacity of contrast discrimination can be seen in non-lexical tasks, but not in word-learning tasks (Stager & Werker Reference Stager and Werker1997, Pater et al. Reference Pater, Stager and Werker2004), which suggests that pairing sound with meaning is taxing, and not all distinctions are encoded in early lexical representations. The ability to detect contrastive changes in word forms has been reported for 30-month-olds (Tamási et al. Reference Tamási, McKean, Gafos and Höhle2019); at this stage, children's representations can be assumed to be specified for information corresponding to subphonemic features.

3 Ladefoged & Maddieson (Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996: 154–155) describe these sounds as ‘flat postalveolar (retroflex)’; for a comprehensive discussion see Hamann (Reference Hamann2004), and for a study using electromagnetic articulography Lorenc (Reference Lorenc2018).

4 On most accounts, prepalatals are acquired earlier than other coronal obstruents, and can serve as replacements for the latter (Kaczmarek Reference Kaczmarek1988, Sołtys-Chmielowicz Reference Sołtys-Chmielowicz1998, Bryndal Reference Bryndal2015). However, this initial substitution pattern may give way to different patterns in individual children at later stages; cf. the data from a Polish-speaking 4-year-old reported in Łukaszewicz (Reference Łukaszewicz2000, Reference Łukaszewicz, Beata, Bański, Łukaszewicz and Opalińska2006a, Reference Łukaszewicz2007), in which all coronal places reduce to plain alveolars.

5 This description pertains to the so-called Warsaw Polish standard, the variety relevant from the point of view of the child whose data I analyse in this study. I ignore the more intricate voicing patterns of Krakow Polish, as they are irrelevant to the point under discussion.

6 The predominant hypothesis in experimental studies is that acquisition proceeds bottom-up from global properties discernible in the speech signal to smaller phonological domains. However, most formal algorithms aimed at modelling the emergence of sensitivity to ambient contrasts and phonotactics at early stages of acquisition are word-based. Such algorithms can be put to use to account for the acquisition of utterance-level generalisations such as Voice Assimilation in Polish, if they are allowed to take unparsed phrases as their inputs (cf. Hayes Reference Hayes, Kager, Pater and Zonneveld2004: 197).

7 Rubach (Reference Rubach2008) argues that Final Devoicing requires a two-step analysis: a word-level grammar and an utterance-level grammar. The intricacies of this analysis need not concern us here, as they are of no consequence as far as the acquisition of Voice Assimilation alone is concerned.

8 As pointed out by the associate editor, a more complete picture of the realisation of the voicing contrast can be obtained with a combination of electroglottographic, airflow and acoustic data; cf. Smith's (Reference Smith1997) study of the devoicing of /z/ in American English. However, the acoustics of the speech signal seem relevant from the acquisitional perspective, as they constitute the input directly available to the child.

9 For acoustic illustrations of the Voice Assimilation patterns in adult speech, see Appendix A. The measurement data are available as .xlsx files in the online supplementary materials.

10 The corpus was recorded in 2006 for the purpose of an acoustic study of word stress in spontaneous speech in child and adult Polish (Łukaszewicz & Rozborski Reference Łukaszewicz and Rozborski2008). The recordings were made using a DAT (Onkyo Integra R1) recorder. A cardioid condenser microphone (Tonsil MCU-53) was positioned on a stand on the floor, pointing in the direction of the child. The child and the experimenter were sitting on the floor during most of the interaction, which focused on making things out of Play-Doh. All recordings took place in the same room, so the acoustic settings are comparable across the sessions. The research was conducted in agreement with internationally accepted ethical standards.

11 Voicing value percentages were obtained by subtracting the locally unvoiced frame percentages reported by Praat from 100.

12 For simplicity, affricates were grouped with stops. Thus, for affricates I report voicing percentage for closure, but ignore the frication portion in these segments (which was usually voiceless, regardless of the underlying voicing).

13 As pointed out by the associate editor, beta regression would be more appropriate for analysing continuous proportional data.

14 Polynomial functions can have multiple local extrema at which the function can assume different values. We need this property to be able to express the fact that attractors may vary in depth.

15 More often than not, differential equations cannot be solved explicitly and approximation methods need to be used (the simplest of which is Euler's method; see Strogatz Reference Strogatz1994: 32–33 for both a discussion of this method and proposed refinements). The qualitative behaviour of dynamical systems is often visualised using graphic methods. For first-order (one-dimensional) equations these are either a vector field on the real line (so-called ‘phase flow’; Strogatz Reference Strogatz1994: 28–29) or a potential well in which the particle moves (assuming heavy viscous damping; Strogatz Reference Strogatz1994: 30). The latter method will be used in this paper. While differential equations are the legacy of Newton's dynamics, the breakthrough consisting in development of powerful geometric methods to analyse qualitative rather than quantitative questions is due to Poincaré.

16 Unlike the ―dV(x)/dx equations in Fig. 12, this equation contains a non-linear term, x 3. This is because in anharmonic oscillating systems the restoring force is non-linearly proportional to the displacement. This yields the quartic term x 4 in the potential function VG(x) = kx – ½x 2 + ¼x 4. The presence of the quartic term and absence of a cubic term in VG(x) ensures a symmetrical effect on x and ―x when k = 0. The negative vs. positive coefficients of the quadratic and quartic terms are kept fixed, so that the change in the landscape depends only on the parameter k, not on these coefficients. Note that reversing the signs in these coefficients would result in obtaining a one-attractor system which would not be able to shift qualitatively to the two-attractor regime depending on k, and thus would be inadequate from the point of view of the expression of phonological contrast. (The behaviour of a particle moving in a symmetrical double-well potential has been extensively studied both in classical and quantum mechanics; see Strogatz Reference Strogatz1994.)

References

Baudouin de Courtenay, Jan Niecisław (1891). O zadaniach językoznawstwa. Prace Filologiczne 3. 92115.Google Scholar
Becker, Michael & Tessier, Anne-Michelle (2011). Trajectories of faithfulness in child-specific phonology. Phonology 28. 163196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Hayes, Bruce (2001). Empirical tests of the Gradual Learning Algorithm. LI 32. 4586.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David (2017). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 6.0.21. http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Braver, Aaron (2019). Modelling incomplete neutralisation with weighted phonetic constraints. Phonology 36. 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, Catherine P. & Goldstein, Louis (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology 6. 201251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browman, Catherine P. & Goldstein, Louis (1995). Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In Port, Robert F. & van Gelder, Timothy (eds.) Mind as motion: explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 175193.Google Scholar
Bryndal, Małgorzata (2015). Fonologiczna interpretacja procesów doskonalenia wymowy dziecięcej na tle współczesnych teorii fonologicznych. Gliwice: Komlogo.Google Scholar
Eimas, Peter D., Siqueland, Einar R., Jusczyk, Peter & Vigorito, James (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science 171. 303306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flemming, Edward (2001). Scalar and categorical phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and phonology. Phonology 18. 744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gafos, Adamantios I. (2006). Dynamics in grammar: comment on Ladd and Ernestus & Baayen. In Goldstein, Louis, Whalen, D. H. & Best, Catherine T. (eds.) Laboratory phonology 8. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 5179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gafos, Adamantios & Benus, Stefan (2006). Dynamics of phonological cognition. Cognitive Science 30. 905943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gafos, Adamantios I., Charlow, Simon, Shaw, Jason A. & Hoole, Philip (2014). Stochastic time analysis of syllable-referential intervals and simplex onsets. JPh 44. 152166.Google Scholar
Gussmann, Edmund (2007). The phonology of Polish. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, Mark & Reiss, Charles (2008). The phonological enterprise. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haman, Ewa, Etenkowski, Bartłomiej, Łuniewska, Magdalena, Szwabe, Joanna, Dąbrowska, Ewa, Szreder, Marta & Łaziński, Marek (2011). Polish CDS corpus. Available (February 2021) at https://childes.talkbank.org/access/Slavic/Polish/CDS.html.Google Scholar
Hamann, Silke (2004). Retroflex fricatives in Slavic languages. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 34. 5367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1999). Phonetically driven phonology: the role of Optimality Theory and inductive grounding. In Darnell, Michael, Moravcsik, Edith, Newmeyer, Frederick, Noonan, Michael & Wheatley, Kathleen (eds.) Functionalism and formalism in linguistics. Vol. 1: General papers. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 243285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (2004). Phonological acquisition in Optimality Theory: the early stages. In Kager, René, Pater, Joe & Zonneveld, Wim (eds.) Constraints in phonological acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 158203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce & White, James (2013). Phonological naturalness and phonotactic learning. LI 44. 4575.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1992). Languages of the mind: essays on mental representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, Peter W., Luce, Paul A. & Charles-Luce, Jan (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language 33. 630645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaczmarek, Leon (1988). Nasze dziecko uczy się mowy. Lublin: Wydawnictwo Logopedyczne.Google Scholar
Keating, Patricia A. (1988). The phonology–phonetics interface. In Newmeyer, Frederick J. (ed.) Linguistics: the Cambridge survey. Vol. 1: Grammatical theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 281302.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S., Saltzman, E. L. & Tuller, B. (1986a). The dynamical perspective on speech production: data and theory. JPh 14. 2959.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul & Menn, Lise (1977). On the acquisition of phonology. In Macnamara, John (ed.) Language learning and thought. New York: Academic Press. 4778.Google Scholar
Kowalik, Krystyna (1998). Morfonologia. In Grzegorczykowa, Renata, Laskowski, Roman & Wróbel, Henryk (eds.) Gramatyka współczesnego języka polskiego: morfologia. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. 87123.Google Scholar
Krajna, Ewa (2008). 100-wyrazowy test artykulacyjny. Gliwice: Komlogo.Google Scholar
Kuhl, Patricia K. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a ‘perceptual magnet effect’ for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception and Psychophysics 50. 93107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladefoged, Peter & Maddieson, Ian (1996). The sounds of the world's languages. Oxford & Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Łobacz, Piotra (1996). Polska fonologia dziecięca: studia fonetyczno-akustyczne. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Energeia.Google Scholar
Lorenc, Anita (2018). Articulatory characteristics of Polish retroflex sibilants: an analysis using electromagnetic articulography. Logopedia 47. 103121.Google Scholar
Łukaszewicz, Beata (2000). Phonological analysis of reduction and assimilation processes in the acquisition of English and Polish. PhD dissertation, University of Warsaw.Google Scholar
Łukaszewicz, , Beata, (2006a). Acquisition of the ±back contrast in Polish. In Bański, Piotr, Łukaszewicz, Beata & Opalińska, Monika (eds.) Studies in constraint-based phonology. Warsaw: Wydawnictwa Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego. 89109.Google Scholar
Łukaszewicz, Beata (2006b). Extrasyllabicity, transparency and prosodic constituency in the acquisition of Polish. Lingua 116. 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Łukaszewicz, Beata (2007). Reduction in syllable onsets in the acquisition of Polish: deletion, coalescence, metathesis and gemination. Journal of Child Language 34. 5382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Łukaszewicz, Beata & Rozborski, Bogdan (2008). Korelaty akustyczne akcentu wyrazowego w języku polskim dorosłych i dzieci. Prace Filologiczne 54. 265283.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Prince, Alan (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Beckman, Jill N., Dickey, Laura Walsh & Urbanczyk, Suzanne (eds.) Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst: GLSA. 249384.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, Peter F. (1997). Acquisition of speech. In Hardcastle, William J. & Laver, John (eds.) The handbook of phonetic sciences. Oxford & Cambridge, MA: Blackwell. 301332.Google Scholar
Maddieson, Ian (2013). Voicing in plosives and fricatives. In Dryer, Matthew S. & Haspelmath, Martin (eds.) The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info.Google Scholar
Menn, Lise (1976). Pattern, control, and contrast in beginning speech: a case study in the development of word form and word function. PhD dissertation, University of Illinois, Champaign-Urbana.Google Scholar
Menn, Lise (1978). Phonological units in beginning speech. In Bell, Alan & Hooper, Joan Bybee (eds.) Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North Holland. 157171.Google Scholar
Ostaszewska, Danuta & Tambor, Jolanta (2012). Fonetyka i fonologia współczesnego języka polskiego. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.Google Scholar
Pater, Joe & Barlow, Jessica A. (2003). Constraint conflict in cluster reduction. Journal of Child Language 30. 487526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pater, Joe, Stager, Christine L. & Werker, Janet F. (2004). The perceptual acquisition of phonological contrasts. Lg 80. 384402.Google Scholar
Pegg, Judith E. & Werker, Janet F. (1997). Adult and infant perception of two English phones. JASA 102. 37423753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peperkamp, Sharon, Le Calvez, Rozenn, Nadal, Jean-Pierre & Dupoux, Emmanuel (2006). The acquisition of allophonic rules: statistical learning with linguistic constraints. Cognition 101. B31B41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2001). Exemplar dynamics: word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Bybee, Joan & Hopper, Paul (eds.) Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 137157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2002). Word-specific phonetics. In Gussenhoven, Carlos & Warner, Natasha (eds.) Laboratory phonology 7. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 101139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, Robert F. & Crawford, Penny (1989). Incomplete neutralization and pragmatics in German. JPh 17. 257282.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Ms, Rutgers University & University of Colorado, Boulder. Published 2004, Malden, MA & Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Roon, Kevin D. & Gafos, Adamantios I. (2016). Perceiving while producing: modeling the dynamics of phonological planning. Journal of Memory and Language 89. 222243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubach, Jerzy (2008). Prevocalic faithfulness. Phonology 25. 433468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, Edward (1925). Sound patterns in language. Lg 1. 3751.Google Scholar
Smith, Caroline L. (1997). The devoicing of /z/ in American English: effects of local and prosodic context. JPh 25. 471500.Google Scholar
Smith, Neil V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: a case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, Neil V. (2010). Acquiring phonology: a cross-generational case-study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smolensky, Paul (1996). On the comprehension/production dilemma in child language. LI 27. 720731.Google Scholar
Sobkowiak, Włodzimierz (1996). English phonetics for Poles: a resource book for learners and teachers. Poznań: Bene Nati.Google Scholar
Sołtys-Chmielowicz, Anna (1998). Wymowa dzieci przedszkolnych. Lublin: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii Curie-Skłodowskiej.Google Scholar
Stager, Christine L. & Werker, Janet F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature 388. 381382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steriade, Donca (1999). Phonetics in phonology: the case of laryngeal neutralization. UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics: Papers in Phonology 3. 25145.Google Scholar
Strogatz, Steven H. (1994). Nonlinear dynamics and chaos: with applications to physics, biology, chemistry and engineering. Reading, MA: Perseus.Google Scholar
Strycharczuk, Patrycja (2012). Sonorant transparency and the complexity of voicing in Polish. JPh 40. 655671.Google Scholar
Tamási, Katalin, McKean, Cristina, Gafos, Adamantios & Höhle, Barbara (2019). Children's gradient sensitivity to phonological mismatch: considering the dynamics of looking behavior and pupil dilation. Journal of Child Language 46. 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tesar, Bruce & Smolensky, Paul (1993). The learnability of Optimality Theory: an algorithm and some basic complexity results. Ms, University of Colorado, Boulder. Available as ROA-2 from the Rutgers Optimality Archive.Google Scholar
Thelen, Esther & Smith, Linda B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tuller, Betty, Case, Pamela, Ding, Mingzhou & Scott Kelso, J. A. (1994). The nonlinear dynamics of speech categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20. 316.Google ScholarPubMed
Vihman, Marilyn M., DePaolis, Rory A. & Keren-Portnoy, Tamar (2015). Babbling and words: a dynamic systems perspective on phonological development. In Bavin, Edith L. & Naigles, Letitia R. (eds.) The Cambridge handbook of child language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 207229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, Janet F. & Curtin, Suzanne (2005). PRIMIR: a developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development 1. 197234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, Janet F. & Tees, Richard C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 7. 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 A feedforward organisation of the phonological system in the child (cf. Kiparsky & Menn 1977: 61).

Figure 1

Figure 2 Voice Assimilation effects (voicing percentages) in adult Polish O1O2 clusters, depending on the underlying voicing of O1 and O2. Voiceless outputs are grey; voiced outputs black. The two sets do not overlap.

Figure 2

Figure 3 (a) Hypothesised position of the target voiced clusters: (i) feedforward; (ii) reversed. (b) Hypothesised position of the target voiceless clusters (adult-like).

Figure 3

Figure 4 Example of obstruent segmentation: (to) jest drugi (mikrofon) /jεz druɟi/ ‘(it) is another (microphone)’ (J 2;5.10).

Figure 4

Figure 5 Example of voicing measurements in target voiced fricatives, using Praat's Voice Report function: (a) duża /duʐa/ ‘big (fem.nom.sg)’ (J 2;2.30; 23% voicing); (b) kucharza /kuxaʐa/ ‘cook (gen.sg)’ (J 2;5.10; 60% voicing); (c) dużo /duʐɔ/ ‘a lot’ (J 2;7.19; 100% voicing).

Figure 5

Figure 6 Measurement results in non-assimilatory contrastive and assimilatory contexts.

Figure 6

Figure 7 Scatterplots for voicing percentages in target voiced fricatives for each of the seven stages.

Figure 7

Figure 8 Voicing percentage in target voiced fricative–stop and stop-fricative clusters in J's speech (stages 1–7), depending on the underlying voicing of O1 in adult Polish.

Figure 8

Figure 9 Voicing emerging in target [+voiced] fricatives vs. stops vs. obstruent clusters for each of the seven stages.

Figure 9

Figure 10 Fricative voicing (gradient) vs. Voice Assimilation (categorical) yielding target-appropriate voiced outputs.

Figure 10

Figure 11 RSD voicing percentage in target [+voiced] stops vs. fricatives for each of the seven stages.

Figure 11

Figure 12 (a) A more stable attractor; (b) a less stable attractor.

Figure 12

Figure 13 Grammatical potentials: VG (x) = kx−1/2x2 + 1/4x4.

Figure 13

Figure 14 Intentional (lexical) potentials: VI(x) = ɑ(1/2x2xREQ × x).

Figure 14

Figure 15 Adult surface contrast as a consequence of combining Grammar and Intention. The solid black lines represent the combined potentials.

Figure 15

Figure 16 Weaker (less stable) surface contrast due to weaker Intention (wI = ɑ = 0.3).

Figure 16

Figure 17 (a) No surface contrast: target voiced fricatives are realised as voiceless. (b) Loss of stability in the child-specific fricative devoicing pattern: targetappropriate and voiceless renditions of target voiced fricatives concur. (c) Target voiced fricatives are realised in a nearly adult-like manner.

Supplementary material: File

Łukaszewicz supplementary material

Łukaszewicz supplementary material 1

Download Łukaszewicz supplementary material(File)
File 88.1 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Łukaszewicz supplementary material

Łukaszewicz supplementary material 2

Download Łukaszewicz supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.8 MB