Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-5c569c448b-r8t2r Total loading time: 0.23 Render date: 2022-07-05T02:04:40.865Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "useRatesEcommerce": false, "useNewApi": true } hasContentIssue true

An S-shaped pattern in the perceived passage of time: how social interaction governs temporal experience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2016

MICHAEL G. FLAHERTY*
Affiliation:
Eckerd College
*
Address for correspondence: Michael G. Flaherty, Department of Sociology, Eckerd College, 4200 54th Avenue South, Saint Petersburg, FL, 33711; e-mail: flahermg@eckerd.edu

Abstract

Variation in the passage of time is perceived against the backdrop of standard temporal units. Under certain conditions, we perceive time to be passing slowly. In other settings, our subjective temporal experience is roughly synchronized with the objective time of clocks or calendars. And given different circumstances, we perceive time to have passed quickly. Drawing from 740 narratives that depict distortion in the perceived passage of time, I formulate a theory that accounts for the full range of variation in temporal experience. This theory can be represented by an S-shaped figure. According to this theory, variation in the perceived passage of time reflects variation in the density of experience per standard temporal unit. In turn, the density of experience per standard temporal unit is conditioned by the dynamics of social interaction. In its original form, however, this theory assumes that one’s circumstances shape one’s temporal experience in deterministic fashion. Consequently, based upon interviews with 406 disparate people, I conceptualize time work (or temporal agency) as one’s efforts to control, manipulate, or customize one’s own temporal experience or that of others. I conclude with some directions for future research.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

I thank Vyvyan Evans and Chris Sinha for the opportunity to contribute to this special issue.

References

Ashcraft, M. H. (1989). Human memory and cognition. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (1981). Nam: the Vietnam War in the words of the men and women who fought there. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Bluedorn, A. C., & Standifer, R. L. (2006). Time and the temporal imagination. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 5, 196206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohannan, P. (1953). Concepts of time among the Tiv of Nigeria. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 9, 251262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheek, F. E., & Laucius, J. (1971). The time worlds of three drug-using groups – alcoholics, heroin addicts, and psychedelics. In Yaker, H., Osmond, H., & Cheek, F. (Eds.), The future of time: man’s temporal environment (pp. 330350). New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Dalsgård, A. L., Frederiksen, M. D., Højlund, S., & Meinert, L. (Eds.) (2014). Ethnographies of youth and temporality: time objectified. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, M. S. (1983). Smut: erotic reality/obscene ideology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Denzin, N. K. (1971). The logic of naturalistic inquiry. Social Forces, 50, 166182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1965 [1915]). The elementary forms of the religious life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Flaherty, M. G. (1991). The perception of time and situated engrossment. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, 7685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flaherty, M. G. (1999). A watched pot: how we experience time. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Flaherty, M. G. (2003). Time work: customizing temporal experience. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 1733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flaherty, M. G. (2011). The textures of time: agency and temporal experience. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Flaherty, M. G., Freidin, B., & Sautu, R. (2005). Variation in the perceived passage of time: a cross-national study. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 400410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flaherty, M. G., & Meer, M. D. (1994). How time flies: age, memory, and temporal compression. Sociological Quarterly, 35, 705721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday Anchor.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: essays on face-to-face behavior. Garden City, NY: Doubleday Anchor.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: an essay on the organization of experience. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hogan, H. W. (1978). A theoretical reconciliation of competing views of time perception. American Journal of Psychology, 91, 417428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huxley, A. (1954). The doors of perception. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Ichheiser, G. (1970). Appearances and realities. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, vol. 1. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Koestler, A. (1946). Dialogue with death: the journal of a prisoner of the fascists in the Spanish Civil War. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levine, L. (1999). Chic to chic. InStyle, February, 229.
Lowitt, B. (1994). Reggie! Reggie! St. Petersburg Times, Thursday, June 2, 1C.
Malkin, E. (2015). Ready to travel the world together. New York Times, Sunday, February 15, 22.
Mann, T. (1968). The magic mountain. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Menaghan, E. G. (1995). Social psychology: interpreting the data. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 321325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, M. P. (1920). Primitive time-reckoning. Lund: C. W. K. Gleerup.Google Scholar
Ornstein, R. E. (1969). On the experience of time. Baltimore, MD: Penguin.Google Scholar
Schutz, A. (1962). Collected papers. Vol. 1: the problem of social reality. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Sorokin, P. A., & Merton, R. K. (1937). Social time: a methodological and functional analysis. American Journal of Sociology, 42, 615629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Times Wires (2011). Harmless toss, tragic result. St. Petersburg Times, Saturday, July 9, 1C, 5C.
Ward, G. C. (1996). The west: an illustrated history. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., Sechrest, L., & Grove, J. B. (1981). Nonreactive measures in the social sciences. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wittmann, M., Otten, S., Schötz, E., Sarikaya, A., Lehnen, H., Jo, H., Kohls, N., Schmidt, S., & Meissner, K. (2015). Subjective expansion of extended time-spans in experienced meditators. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelditch, M. Jr. (1962). Some methodological problems of field studies. American Journal of Sociology, 67, 566576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zerubavel, E. (1981). Hidden rhythms: schedules and calendars in social life. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Zerubavel, E. (1982). The standardization of time: a sociohistorical perspective. American Journal of Sociology, 88, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zerubavel, E. (1985). The seven day circle: the history and meaning of the week. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
2
Cited by

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

An S-shaped pattern in the perceived passage of time: how social interaction governs temporal experience
Available formats
×

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

An S-shaped pattern in the perceived passage of time: how social interaction governs temporal experience
Available formats
×

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

An S-shaped pattern in the perceived passage of time: how social interaction governs temporal experience
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *