Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T06:01:30.078Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Explicit memory training leads to improved memory for face–name pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment: Results of a pilot investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2008

BENJAMIN M. HAMPSTEAD*
Affiliation:
Rehabilitation R&D Center of Excellence, Atlanta VAMC, Decatur, Georgia Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia
K. SATHIAN
Affiliation:
Rehabilitation R&D Center of Excellence, Atlanta VAMC, Decatur, Georgia Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia Department of Neurology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia
ANNA BACON MOORE
Affiliation:
Rehabilitation R&D Center of Excellence, Atlanta VAMC, Decatur, Georgia Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia
CARRIE NALISNICK
Affiliation:
Rehabilitation R&D Center of Excellence, Atlanta VAMC, Decatur, Georgia Department of Neurology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia
ANTHONY Y. STRINGER
Affiliation:
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Benjamin M. Hampstead, Emory University, Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, 1441 Clifton Road NE, Room 150, Atlanta, GA 30322. E-mail: bhampst@emory.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Relatively few studies have examined the use of cognitive rehabilitation in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), largely due to the assumption that training will not improve functioning in patients with progressive conditions. Face-name association, an ecologically valid task, is both dependent on the explicit memory system and difficult for MCI patients. During three hour-long sessions, eight patients diagnosed with MCI were trained in the use of explicit memory strategies with 45 face-name pairs. For each pair, they were taught to visually identify a facial feature, link a phonological cue to that feature, and recall the associated name. There was significant improvement in recognition accuracy, along with faster reaction times, for trained face-name pairs. Improved accuracy persisted when tested one month after training. Significant, but less, improvement was also found on untrained stimuli, raising the possibility of generalization of training strategies. Preliminary results suggest strategy-based cognitive rehabilitation may be beneficial in patients with MCI, though these results must be replicated with a control group to rule out practice effects. (JINS, 2008, 14, 883–889.)

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

The benefits of explicit memory training (EMT) are recognized for patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI) (Cicerone et al., Reference Cicerone, Dahlberg, Malec, Langenbahn, Felicetti, Kneipp, Ellmo, Kalmar, Giacino, Harley, Laatsch, Morse and Catanese2005; NIH, 1998); however, little research has investigated EMT in patients with progressive illnesses such as Alzheimer's disease (AD). Despite some evidence that EMT is beneficial in early AD (Acevedo & Loewenstein, Reference Acevedo and Loewenstein2007; De Vreese et al., Reference De Vreese, Neri, Fioravanti, Belloi and Zanetti2001), Clare and Woods (Reference Clare and Woods2004) concluded that such training is not strongly supported in this population, presumably due to their severe explicit memory impairments.

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is often a transitional stage between normal aging and AD (Petersen, Reference Petersen2004). The period of relatively preserved memory functioning after initial diagnosis of MCI but before progression to AD (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pankratz, Negash, Machulda, Petersen, Boeve, Knopman, Lucas, Ferman, Graff-Radford and Ivnik2007), may be due to compensatory mechanisms observed in functional neuroimaging studies (Dickerson et al., Reference Dickerson, Salat, Greve, Chua, Rand-Giovannetti, Rentz, Bertram, Mullin, Tanzi, Blacker, Albert and Sperling2005). This period presents an opportunity to intervene with strategies to maximize memory functioning and possibly delay further decline.

The few studies of EMT in patients with MCI yielded discouraging results. Rapp et al. (Reference Rapp, Brenes and Marsh2002) found no objective evidence of improvement in patients with MCI and Belleville et al. (Reference Belleville, Gilbert, Fontaine, Gagnon, Menard and Gauthier2006) found only limited improvement after training. Similarly, EMT was ineffective in older adults with below average memory functioning (Unverzagt et al., Reference Unverzagt, Kasten, Johnson, Rebok, Marsiske, Koepke, Elias, Morris, Willis, Ball, Rexroth, Smith, Wolinsky and Tennstedt2007). However, these studies taught patients varied mnemonic strategies that made it difficult for them to select and use the appropriate strategy during posttraining memory testing (Rapp et al., Reference Rapp, Brenes and Marsh2002). Thus, training a single mnemonic strategy that can be applied to multiple types of information may be more beneficial than training with multiple strategies (Stringer, Reference Stringer2007).

Face-name association offers an ecologically valid learning and memory task that is difficult for patients with MCI (Petrella et al., Reference Petrella, Krishnan, Slavin, Tran, Murty and Doraiswamy2006) and AD (Sperling et al., Reference Sperling, Bates, Chua, Cocchiarella, Rentz, Rosen, Schacter and Albert2003) and is associated with the explicit memory system (Sperling et al., Reference Sperling, Bates, Cocchiarella, Schacter, Rosen and Albert2001). Importantly, the brain regions within this system are the most severely affected during progression from MCI to AD (Whitwell et al., Reference Whitwell, Przybelski, Weigand, Knopman, Boeve, Petersen and Jack2007). Following EMT, Belleville et al. (Reference Belleville, Gilbert, Fontaine, Gagnon, Menard and Gauthier2006) found significant improvement on a face-name task in MCI patients. Within the AD literature, EMT for face-name associations has ranged from ineffective (Backman et al., Reference Backman, Josephsson, Herlitz, Stigsdotter and Viitanen1991; Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, Reference Metzler-Baddeley and Snowden2005) to significantly beneficial, with improvements persisting at 3- and 6-month follow-up assessments (Acevedo & Loewenstein, Reference Acevedo and Loewenstein2007).

We report here on the use of a novel EMT paradigm in patients with MCI. We used a face-name association task that was based on the Biographical Information Module of the Ecologically Oriented Neurorehabilitation of Memory (EON-Mem) program (Stringer, Reference Stringer2007), which is clinically effective in TBI and stroke patients, but has not previously been applied to MCI. This pilot study was conducted in conjunction with a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, which revealed increased activation in neocortical areas associated with memory (Hampstead et al., Reference Hampstead, Stringer, Stilla, Moore and Sathian2008). As cognitive rehabilitation is not routinely offered to MCI patients, the majority of whom are expected to decline toward AD, and because previous studies of cognitive rehabilitation in MCI have been mostly negative, we believe it is important to alert both clinicians and researchers to novel and potentially efficacious interventions, even at the pilot stage.

METHODS

Participants

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Emory University and the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center (VAMC). Eight Caucasian patients gave informed consent and were recruited from the Emory University Alzheimer's Disease Research Center and the Atlanta VAMC. All were diagnosed with amnestic, multi-domain MCI (Petersen, Reference Petersen2004) during a consensus conference based on neurological, neuropsychological, laboratory, and neuroimaging findings. Activities of daily living (ADLs) and instrumental ADLs were reported as intact at the time of participation by both the patient and a family member, thereby ruling out a diagnosis of dementia. Six of the patients completed the Dementia Rating Scale-2 (DRS-2, Jurica et al., Reference Jurica, Leitten and Mattis2001) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al., 1982–Reference Yesavage, Brink, Rose, Lum, Huang, Adey and Leirer1983) during the first session to briefly reassess their cognitive functioning at the time of study participation. Although two patients performed fully within normal limits on the DRS-2, they demonstrated deficits during the more comprehensive evaluation as noted above, suggesting the DRS-2 was not sufficiently sensitive to detect their impairments. Demographics and test performances for all subjects are given in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographics, average trials needed during training, and memory performance of patients

Note

The number of trials possible during training ranged from 15 (best)–50 (worst). Percent improvement on the trained and untrained lists were calculated by subtracting pre-training memory test accuracy from post-training accuracy. DRS-2 and GDS were collected during session 1; DRS-2 = Dementia Rating Scale-2; AEMSS = Age and education-matched scaled score; I/P = Initiation/Perseveration index; Construct = Construction index; Concept = Conceptualization index; SS = scaled score; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale.

Stimuli

Ninety faces were selected from the Kirwan and Stark (Reference Kirwan and Stark2004) face set, transformed to grayscale images, and divided into two lists of 45, matched for gender, race, and approximate age (by decade). Each face was randomly paired with a gender-appropriate name from one of two lists matched for length (5–6 letters) and popularity by decade.

Procedures

Each patient completed pre- and posttraining assessments and three training sessions over the course of 2 weeks. The posttraining assessment always occurred 2 days after the third training session. During the assessment sessions, patients were exposed to all 90 novel face-name pairs in blocks of five, where each pair was presented for 5 seconds with a 1-second inter-stimulus interval. Patients were instructed to remember the face-name associations; no overt response was required. These active blocks alternated with 20-second rest blocks when patients stared at a fixation cross. The six subjects participating in the fMRI study (Hampstead et al., Reference Hampstead, Stringer, Stilla, Moore and Sathian2008) saw these stimuli during scanning, the other two viewed them on a computer screen in a quiet room. Fifteen minutes after seeing the last face-name pair, patients completed a four-alternative recognition memory test involving all 90 pairs. The four choices used in this test were the target name, a foil from the list used in training, a foil from the untrained list, and a novel foil. This test design reduced chance level performance to 25% and also varied foil familiarity.

Patients underwent three face-name training sessions, with an average of 2.8 days (SD = 0.8) in between. Patients were assigned one of the two lists of 45 pairs, in counterbalanced order. Training used a modified Biographical Information Module from the EON-Mem program (Stringer, Reference Stringer2007). Although this module teaches patients to self-generate cues to facilitate learning and memory, we provided these cues to standardize procedures across subjects. For each face-name pair, patients were directed to a salient facial feature (visual cue) and were given a “nickname” (verbal cue) linking the facial feature to the name. Verbal cues were phonologically similar to, and typically rhymed with the actual name. Examples of the faces and cues can be seen in Figure 1. Patients were instructed to associate the visual and verbal cues in detailed mental images that exaggerated and emphasized the salient facial features. On each subsequent training trial, patients were required to first recall the visual cue, then the verbal cue, and finally the corresponding name. During each training session, patients were trained on 15 face-name pairs, in three successive groups of five. For each pair, they were required to spontaneously recall the name on three consecutive trials, with a maximum of 10 trials to reach this criterion. Following completion of the third group of five pairs, all 15 pairs were reviewed as a single group, with three trials for each pair (same day review). The next training session began with a review of all 15 pairs trained during the previous session, again with three trials for each pair (delayed review).

Fig. 1. Examples of the stimuli and cues used during training.

Six patients returned for a 1-month follow-up, during which they again completed the face-name test and were asked to make memory confidence ratings for each trial using a 4-point scale (1 = not confident at all, 4 = extremely confident). Importantly, they had not been exposed to any of the face-name stimuli during the 1-month interval. Two patients were unable to return at follow-up for logistical reasons but were similar to the remainder of the patients on all variables in Table 1.

RESULTS

Memory Test Performance

Recognition accuracy (Figure 2a) was assessed using a 2 × 2 repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with factors of list (trained, untrained) and time (baseline, posttraining). There was a significant main effect of list [F(1,7) = 59.7; p < .001; partial η2 = .9], indicating significantly better performance on the trained list. The main effect of time was significant [F(1,7) = 54.9; p < .001; partial η2 = .89], as was the list-by-time interaction [F(1,7) = 63.8; p < .001; η2 = .9]. This interaction arose because performance was similar between lists at baseline (t 14 = 1.5; p = .15) but was greater posttraining for the trained compared with the untrained list (t 14 = 5.4; p < .001). There were, however, significant improvements for both trained (t 7 = 8.8; p < .001) and untrained lists (t 7 = 4.0; p = .005) relative to baseline. The correlation between general memory functioning [DRS-2 Memory scaled score (SS)] and improvement after training was nearly significant for the trained list (r = .8; p = .057) and significant for the untrained list (r = .85; p = .03).

Fig. 2. Mean recognition accuracy (a) and median response time (b) on the memory test at each time point. Error bars represent SEM.

Response latencies (Figure 2b) revealed a significant main effect of list [F(1,7) = 7.7; p = .03; partial η2 = .52], indicating that patients made faster responses for trained pairs compared with untrained pairs. There was no main effect of time [F(1,7) = 0.6; p = .48; partial η2 = .07] but there was a significant list-by-time interaction [F(1,7) = 8.7; p = .02; partial η2 = .55], reflecting similar response times at baseline (t 14 = 0.2; p = .84) but faster posttraining responses for the trained compared with the untrained list (t 14 = 2.45; p = .03). General memory functioning (DRS-2 Memory SS) showed no correlation with the change in response latency (trained: r = .05; p = .9; untrained: r = −.31; p = .55).

One-Month Follow-up

Patients demonstrated a slight but nonsignificant decline in accuracy on the trained list compared with posttraining (t 5 = 2.2; p = .08), but performance remained significantly above baseline (t 5 = 5.7; p = .002). Patients were significantly slower to respond to pairs from the trained list compared with both baseline (t 5 = 3.7; p = .01) and posttraining (t 5 = 3.7; p = .01). Recognition accuracy on the untrained list declined significantly from posttraining levels (t 5 = 3.1; p = .03) and was similar to baseline performance (t 5 = 0.7; p = .54). Patients were slower to respond compared with baseline (t 5 = 2.8; p = .04) but this difference was not significant compared with posttraining (t 5 = 1.9; p = .12). Additionally, patients were significantly more confident in selecting the correct names for the trained compared with the untrained list (t 10 = 2.5; p = .03).

Use of Cues During Training

Overall, patients required an average of 24.8 trials (SD = 9.0) to reach criterion during training (possible range: 15–50). There was a significant inverse correlation between the number of training trials needed to reach criterion and the DRS-2 Memory SS (r = −.91; p = .01). There was also a significant inverse correlation between the average number of trials needed to reach criterion and the improvement on the trained list (r = −.81; p = .02) that persisted even when the most severely impaired patient (patient 3) was excluded from analysis (r = −.76; p = .048). There was no correlation between the number of training trials and improved response latency for the trained list (r = −.34; p = .42).

Patients were able to remember both the visual and the verbal cue on the majority of trials during the same day and delayed reviews. A 2 × 2 ANOVA with factors of cue type and time suggested that patients remembered the visual cues better than the verbal cues, although the main effect of cue type fell just short of significance [F(1,7) = 5.3; p = .056; partial η2 = .43]. There was a significant main effect of time, such that patients recalled fewer cues between training sessions [F(1,7) = 6.1; p = .04; partial η2 = .47). The cue-by-time interaction was not significant [F(1,7) = 4.5; p = .07; partial η2 = .39]. Recollection of both visual (r = .81; p < .001) and verbal cues (r = .94; p < .001) was significantly correlated with spontaneous recall of names during the review trials.

DISCUSSION

The current study shows that EMT techniques may be quite effective in patients with MCI. Specifically, our patients demonstrated 27–69% improvement in recognition accuracy for the trained face-name pairs after only three hour-long training sessions. Significant improvement remained evident 1 month after training. From a psychosocial standpoint, the improved accuracy, faster responding, and increased confidence for the trained pairs could help patients avoid uncomfortable social interactions that come with difficulty remembering names. The specificity of the behavioral effects, as well as associated increases in fMRI activation in regions implicated in explicit memory that were unique to the trained stimuli (Hampstead et al., Reference Hampstead, Stringer, Stilla, Moore and Sathian2008), suggests that these changes were induced by the training provided. Additionally, the strong relationship between baseline memory functioning and behavioral improvement suggests that earlier intervention in this population could lead to greater benefit.

Our results support previous studies suggesting that EMT can be effective in MCI and AD (Acevedo & Loewenstein, Reference Acevedo and Loewenstein2007; Belleville et al., Reference Belleville, Gilbert, Fontaine, Gagnon, Menard and Gauthier2006; Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Acevedo, Czaja and Duara2004). Importantly, our patients showed substantial improvement after learning approximately 4 times the number of face-name pairs used in these other studies. This magnitude of improvement may be attributed to the highly focused nature of our intervention, whereas the variety of mnemonic strategies used in a previous study may have been overwhelming and thus counterproductive to patients (Rapp et al., Reference Rapp, Brenes and Marsh2002). Our strategic use of verbal cues that rhymed with the name may have facilitated recognition accuracy since rhyming has been shown to improve memory (e.g., Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Lipinski and Aktunc2005). Future studies could directly contrast single and multiple mnemonic strategy approaches in their ability to improve memory performance.

It is possible that factors such as repeated exposure to the stimuli, or the specific cues provided, could have contributed to the observed improvements. Although we cannot fully rule out such factors at this time, two pieces of evidence suggest that the observed improvements on the trained list are attributable to use of the strategies. First, there was a significant inverse correlation between the number of training trials needed to reach criterion and improved memory for the trained stimuli; a positive correlation would be expected if the amount of exposure to the stimuli determined behavioral improvement. Second, there were significant correlations between spontaneous name recall and recollection of both visual and verbal cues during the review periods. Inclusion of an independent control group in future studies would further address the extent to which improved memory test performance is due to the trained strategies.

We provided patients with the cues for each face-name pair during training to standardize the procedures; however, real-world utility of the strategies depends on the ability to spontaneously generate cues. In this respect, the significant posttraining improvements on the untrained list are encouraging, especially considering that all our patients anecdotally reported attempting to use the strategies during the posttraining exposure. Previous studies in healthy older adults that have used task-specific training have shown generalization to similar tasks (West et al., Reference West, Welch, Yassuda, Hill, Backman and Neely2000). We cannot rule out the possibility that practice effects contributed to improvement on the untrained list, especially considering that performance returned to baseline after 1 month. However, the brief exposure time (5 s), the large number of pairs used, and the 2-week period between baseline and posttraining memory testing suggest that practice effects alone are unlikely to account for the improvements seen on the untrained stimuli. Nonetheless, future studies could address these concerns by including novel stimuli during the posttraining exposure and by comparing the effectiveness of self- versus experimenter-generated cues.

Although our pilot study had a small sample size, memory training must be effective on an individual level: our large effect sizes are evidence of this efficacy. Including a test of free recall may provide a more accurate measure of the ecological validity of training. Our results should be replicated with an independent control group to rule out practice effects. Despite these limitations, our preliminary results provide support for the efficacy of one EMT technique (visual-verbal cueing for face-name associations) in patients with MCI. These findings are especially encouraging given the relative paucity of research investigating memory rehabilitation for patients with MCI and early AD. Future randomized control trials with larger sample sizes are needed to confirm our pilot results, explore the use of other EON-Mem Modules in MCI and AD, and study generalization of treatment effects to real-world situations.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was presented at the 2008 annual meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society. We thank Dr. Felicia Goldstein for her assistance with patient recruitment. This work was supported by grants from the Atlanta VAMC RR&D Center of Excellence (BH), VA RR&D AI B4602H (BH), VA RR&D ARCD B3323K (AM), VA RR&D CDTA B4954N (AM) and NIH K24 EY017332 (KS). Support to KS from the Atlanta VAMC is also gratefully acknowledged. Conflicts of Interest: Dr. Anthony Y. Stringer is author of the Ecologically Oriented Neurorehabilitation of Memory (EON-Mem) program and receives royalties from sales. No other author has any conflict of interest. Author Contributions: Each author provided significant intellectual contribution to warrant authorship. Specifically, B.H., K.S., A.M., & A.S. participated in research design, data analysis and interpretation, and manuscript preparation. B.H. & C.N. participated in stimulus development and data acquisition. Each author declares that he/she has seen and approved the final version of this manuscript. Dr. Benjamin M. Hampstead had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

References

REFERENCES

Acevedo, A. & Loewenstein, D.A. (2007). Nonpharmacological cognitive interventions in aging and dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 20, 239249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Backman, L., Josephsson, S., Herlitz, A., Stigsdotter, A., & Viitanen, M. (1991). The generalizability of training gains in dementia: Effects of an imagery-based mnemonic on face-name retention duration. Psychology of Aging, 6, 489492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belleville, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., Gagnon, L., Menard, E., & Gauthier, S. (2006). Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22, 486499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicerone, K.D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J.F., Langenbahn, D.M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., Ellmo, W., Kalmar, K., Giacino, J.T., Harley, J.P., Laatsch, L., Morse, P.A., & Catanese, J. (2005). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Update review of literature from 1998 through 2002. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86, 16811692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clare, L. & Woods, R.T. (2004). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer's disease: A review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vreese, L.P., Neri, M., Fioravanti, M., Belloi, L., & Zanetti, O. (2001). Memory rehabilitation in Alzheimer's disease: A review of progress. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 794809.Google Scholar
Dickerson, B.C., Salat, D.H., Greve, D.N., Chua, E.F., Rand-Giovannetti, E., Rentz, D.M., Bertram, L., Mullin, K., Tanzi, R.E., Blacker, D., Albert, M.S., & Sperling, R.A. (2005). Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and AD. Neurology, 65, 404411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., & Aktunc, E. (2005). Reexamining the phonological similarity effect in immediate serial recall: The roles of type of similarity, category cuing, and item recall. Memory and Cognition, 33, 10011016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampstead, B.M., Stringer, A.Y., Stilla, R.F., Moore, A.B., & Sathian, K. (2008, February). Functional neuroimaging changes following cognitive rehabilitation in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Wiakoloa, HI: International Neuropsychological Society.Google Scholar
Jurica, P.J., Leitten, C.L., & Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia Rating Scale–2 Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Kirwan, C.B. & Stark, C.E. (2004). Medial temporal lobe activation during encoding and retrieval of novel face-name pairs. Hippocampus, 14, 919930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, D.A., Acevedo, A., Czaja, S.J., & Duara, R. (2004). Cognitive rehabilitation of mildly impaired Alzheimer's disease patients on cholinesterase inhibitors. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12, 395402.Google Scholar
Metzler-Baddeley, C. & Snowden, J.S. (2005). Brief report: Errorless versus errorful learning as a memory rehabilitation approach in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 10701079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institutes of Health. (1998). Rehabilitation of persons with traumatic brain injury. NIH consensus statement, 16:1–41 (October 26–28). Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health.Google Scholar
Petersen, R.C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of International Medicine, 256, 183194.Google Scholar
Petrella, J.R., Krishnan, S., Slavin, M.J., Tran, T.T., Murty, L., & Doraiswamy, P.M. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment: Evaluation with 4-T functional MR imaging. Radiology, 240, 177186.Google Scholar
Rapp, S., Brenes, G., & Marsh, A.P. (2002). Memory enhancement training for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: A preliminary study. Aging & Mental Health, 6, 511.Google Scholar
Smith, G.E., Pankratz, V.S., Negash, S., Machulda, M.M., Petersen, R.C., Boeve, B.F., Knopman, D.S., Lucas, J.A., Ferman, T.J., Graff-Radford, N., & Ivnik, R.J. (2007). A plateau in pre-Alzheimer memory decline: Evidence for compensatory mechanisms? Neurology, 69, 133139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperling, R.A., Bates, J.F., Chua, E.F., Cocchiarella, A.J., Rentz, D.M., Rosen, B.R., Schacter, D.L., & Albert, M.S. (2003). fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 74, 4450.Google Scholar
Sperling, R.A., Bates, J.F., Cocchiarella, A.J., Schacter, D.L., Rosen, B.R., & Albert, M.S. (2001). Encoding novel face-name associations: A functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 14, 129139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stringer, A.Y. (2007). Ecologically-oriented Neurorehabilitation of Memory Therapist Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Unverzagt, F.W., Kasten, L., Johnson, K.E., Rebok, G.W., Marsiske, M., Koepke, K.M., Elias, J.W., Morris, J.N., Willis, S.L., Ball, K., Rexroth, D.F., Smith, D.M., Wolinsky, F.D., & Tennstedt, S.L. (2007). Effect of memory impairment on training outcomes in ACTIVE. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 953960.Google Scholar
West, R.L., Welch, D.C., & Yassuda, M.S. (2000). Innovative approaches to memory training for older adults. In Hill, R.D., Backman, L., & Neely, A.S. (Eds.), Cognitive Rehabilitation in Old Age. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whitwell, J.L., Przybelski, S.A., Weigand, S.D., Knopman, D.S., Boeve, B.F., Petersen, R.C., & Jack, C.R. (2007). 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Brain, 130, 17771786.Google Scholar
Yesavage, J.A., Brink, T.L., Rose, T.L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V.O. (1982–1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17, 3749.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics, average trials needed during training, and memory performance of patients

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Examples of the stimuli and cues used during training.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Mean recognition accuracy (a) and median response time (b) on the memory test at each time point. Error bars represent SEM.