Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-568f69f84b-ftpnm Total loading time: 0.173 Render date: 2021-09-19T15:16:06.864Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "metricsAbstractViews": false, "figures": true, "newCiteModal": false, "newCitedByModal": true, "newEcommerce": true, "newUsageEvents": true }

Spontaneous confabulation, temporal context confusion and reality monitoring: A study of three patients with anterior communicating artery aneurysms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2010

MARTHA S. TURNER*
Affiliation:
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
LISA CIPOLOTTI
Affiliation:
National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery and Institute of Neurology, London, United Kingdom
TIM SHALLICE
Affiliation:
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom Cognitive Neuroscience Sector, Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati, Trieste, Italy
*Corresponding
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Martha Turner, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London, WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom. E-mail: martha.turner@ucl.ac.uk

Abstract

Spontaneous confabulation involves the production of false or distorted memories, and is commonly associated with ventromedial prefrontal damage. One influential theory proposes that the critical deficit is a failure to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces that intrude into ongoing thinking (Schnider & Ptak, 1999). In this study, we report experimental investigations with three spontaneously confabulating patients aimed at exploring this account. Using Schnider and Ptak’s (1999) continuous recognition paradigm, we replicated their experimental results with our patients. However, our data suggest that the critical impairment might be more generalized than a failure to suppress currently irrelevant memories. First, a temporal source monitoring task failed to show that previous memory traces intrude into the present. Second, a reality monitoring task revealed that confabulating patients had a tendency to misidentify imagined events as real, a result that cannot be explained in terms of temporal confusion. This error was specific to confabulating patients and was not shared by non-confabulating ACoA patients. Our data therefore suggest a more generalized impairment in source monitoring, not only on the basis of temporality or current relevance, but across a range of contextual domains, including information used to distinguish real memories from imaginings. (JINS, 2010, 16, 984–994.)

Type
Symposia
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aleman, A., Bocker, K.B., Hijman, R., de Haan, E.H., & Kahn, R.S. (2003). Cognitive basis of hallucinations in schizophrenia: Role of top-down information processing. Schizophrenia Research, 64, 175185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonhoeffer, K. (1901). Die akuten giesteskrankheiten des gewohnheitstrinkers. Eine klinische studie. Jena: Gustav Fischer.Google Scholar
Burgess, P.W., & Shallice, T. (1996). Confabulation and the control of recollection. Memory, 4, 359411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciaramelli, E., & Ghetti, S. (2007). What are confabulators’ memories made of? A study of subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulation. Neuropsychologia, 45, 14891500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciaramelli, E., & Spaniol, J. (2009). Ventromedial prefrontal damage and memory for context: Perceptual versus semantic features. Neuropsychology, 23, 649657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J.R., & Garthwaite, P.H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 11961208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalla Barba, G. (2002). Memory, consciousness and temporality. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Barba, G., Capelletti, J.Y., Signorini, M., & Denes, G. (1997). Confabulation: Remembering “another” past, planning “another” future. Neurocase, 3, 425436.Google Scholar
Delbecq-Derouesne, J., Beauvois, M.F., & Shallice, T. (1990). Preserved recall versus impaired recognition: A case study. Brain, 113, 10451074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A., Conway, M.A., & Solms, M. (2007). Confabulation: Motivated reality monitoring. Neuropsychologia, 45, 21802190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa, A., Alain, C., Stuss, D.T., Melo, B., Miller, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2006). Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: A strategic retrieval account. Brain, 129, 13991414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa, A., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of confabulation: A review and a model. In Baddeley, A.D., Kopelman, M.D., & Wilson, B.A. (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders (2nd ed., pp. 315342). London: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Hasselmo, M.E., Wyble, B.P., & Wallenstein, G.V. (1996). Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: Role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 6, 693708.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M.K. (1997). Source monitoring and memory distortion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 352, 17331745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M.K., Hayes, S.M., D’Esposito, M., & Raye, C.L. (2000). Confabulation. In Boller, F., Grafman, J. (Series Eds.), & Cermak, L.S. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of neuropsychology: Vol. 2. Memory and its disorders (2nd ed., pp. 383407).Google Scholar
Johnson, M.K., O’Connor, M., & Cantor, J. (1997). Confabulation, memory deficits, and frontal dysfunction. Brain and Cognition, 34, 189206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korsakoff, S.S. (1889). Etude medico-psychologique sur une forme des maladies de la memoire. Revue Philosophique, 20, 501530.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1886). Ueber Erinnerungsfalschungen (1/3). Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 17, 830843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manns, J.R., & Squire, L.R. (1999). Impaired recognition memory on the Doors and People Test after damage limited to the hippocampal region. Hippocampus, 9, 495499.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercer, B., Wapner, W., Gardner, H., & Benson, F. (1977). A study of confabulation. Archives of Neurology, 34, 429433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moscovitch, M., & Melo, B. (1997). Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: Evidence from confabulation and amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35, 10171034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pick, A. (1905). Zur Psychologie der Confabulation. Neurologisches Centralblatt, 24, 509516.Google Scholar
Schacter, D.L., Norman, K.A., & Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 289318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnider, A. (2008). The confabulating mind: How the brain creates reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A., Gutbrod, K., Hess, C.W., & Schroth, G. (1996). Memory without context: Amnesia with confabulations after infarction of the right capsular genu. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 61, 186193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnider, A., & Ptak, R. (1999). Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 677681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnider, A., Ptak, R., von Daniken, D.C., & Remonda, L. (2000). Recovery from spontaneous confabulations parallels recovery of temporal confusion in memory. Neurology, 55, 7483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnider, A., Treyer, V., & Buck, A. (2000). Selection of currently relevant memories by the human posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscence, 20, 58805884.Google ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., & Cooper, R.P. (in press). The organisation of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Simons, J.S., Davis, S.W., Gilbert, S.J., Frith, C.D., & Burgess, P.W. (2006). Discriminating imagined from perceived information engages brain areas implicated in schizophrenia. Neuroimage, 32, 696703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, J.S., Henson, R.N.A., Gilbert, S.J., & Fletcher, P.C. (2008). Separable forms of reality monitoring supported by anterior prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 447457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snodgrass, J.G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980) A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174215.Google Scholar
Treyer, V., Buck, A., & Schnider, A. (2003). Subcortical loop activation during selection of currently relevant memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 610618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, M.S., Cipolotti, L., Yousry, T.A., & Shallice, T. (2008a). Confabulation: Damage to a specific inferior medial prefrontal system. Cortex, 44, 637648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M.S., & Coltheart, M. (2010). Confabulation and delusion: A common monitoring framework. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 346376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M.S., Simons, J.S., Gilbert, S.J., Frith, C.D., & Burgess, P.W. (2008b). Distinct roles for lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex in source monitoring of perceived and imagined events. Neuropsychologia, 46, 14421453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinogradov, S., Luks, T.L., Simpson, G.V., Schulman, B.J., Glenn, S., & Wong, A.E. (2006). Brain activation patterns during memory of cognitive agency. Neuroimage, 31, 896905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15
Cited by

Send article to Kindle

To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Spontaneous confabulation, temporal context confusion and reality monitoring: A study of three patients with anterior communicating artery aneurysms
Available formats
×

Send article to Dropbox

To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

Spontaneous confabulation, temporal context confusion and reality monitoring: A study of three patients with anterior communicating artery aneurysms
Available formats
×

Send article to Google Drive

To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

Spontaneous confabulation, temporal context confusion and reality monitoring: A study of three patients with anterior communicating artery aneurysms
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *