Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T08:16:11.857Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of Relative Deprivation on Intention to Rebel: A Multiple Mediation Model

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2018

Xiang-Yu Chen
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Center for Mental Health Education and Research, Jiangxi Key Laboratory of Psychology and Cognition Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China.
Xin-Qiang Wang*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Center for Mental Health Education and Research, Jiangxi Key Laboratory of Psychology and Cognition Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China.
Jian-Ping Liu
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Center for Mental Health Education and Research, Jiangxi Key Laboratory of Psychology and Cognition Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China.
Sheng-Hong Dong
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Center for Mental Health Education and Research, Jiangxi Key Laboratory of Psychology and Cognition Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China.
Jun-Cheng Zhu
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Center for Mental Health Education and Research, Jiangxi Key Laboratory of Psychology and Cognition Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China.
Jun-Yu Huo
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Center for Mental Health Education and Research, Jiangxi Key Laboratory of Psychology and Cognition Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China.
*
Address for correspondence: Xin-Qiang Wang, School of Psychology, Jiangxi Normal University, No 99, Ziyang Road, Nanchang, Jiangxi Province, 330022, China. Email: xinqiangw101@163.com

Abstract

This study examined the mediating effects of future social expectations and interpersonal distrust on the relationship between individual relative deprivation and intention to rebel. Data were gathered from 807 people from multiple occupational backgrounds in a municipality in southwest China. Structural equation modelling showed that individual relative deprivation predicted intention to rebel directly and also that it predicted intention to rebel indirectly via negative future social expectations, interpersonal distrust, and a chain mediating effect of negative future social expectations and interpersonal distrust. These results highlight the importance of the associations between future social expectations and interpersonal distrust with intention to rebel in people who report relative deprivation. The findings also indicate that prevention and intervention programs related to relative deprivation and intention to rebel in China are worthy of further research.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018

Rulers of states and chiefs of families are not concerned lest their people should be poor, but only lest what they have should be ill-proportioned.

Confucian Analects Book XVI: Ke She

In 1978, the Chinese government launched an extensive reform program to reduce poverty and liberalise the economic market. Since then, China has shown extraordinary achievements in economic development and improvements in public services. However, while living and cultural standards have improved, the gap between rich and poor people has expanded. According to data from the National Bureau of Statistics, China's Gini coefficient, a measure of economic inequality, fluctuated around 0.46 between 2003 and 2016, which is above the warning line (i.e., 0.4). A growing body of evidence and theory suggests that many negative social and health outcomes, including violence, conflict, suicide, drug abuse, overweight, lack of trust, and mental disease are associated with income inequality (Rowlingson, Reference Rowlingson2011; Rözer & Volker, Reference Rözer and Volker2016; Wilkinson & Pickett, Reference Wilkinson and Pickett2007, Reference Wilkinson and Pickett2009). Why are rates of happiness falling in a growing economy? Psychologists and sociologists have used the concept of relative deprivation (RD) to explain the phenomenon (Brockmann, Delhey, Welzel, & Yuan, Reference Brockmann, Delhey, Welzel and Yuan2009; Mishra & Novakowski, Reference Mishra and Novakowski2016).

Relative Deprivation

Stouffer, Suchman, Devinney, Star, and Williams (Reference Stouffer, Suchman, Devinney, Star and Williams1949), from the Research Division of the Information Branch of the U.S. Army, coined the term ‘relative deprivation’ in their classic study, The American Soldier. RD, a key consequence of inequality, involves subjective feelings of anger, resentment, and frustration in response to negative social comparisons with relevant others (Bernstein & Crosby, Reference Bernstein and Crosby1980; Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin, & Bialosiewicz, Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012). That is, individuals experience feelings of wanting, deserving, and resentment for not having what others have (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012; Sun & Guo, Reference Sun and Guo2016). This inner feeling of comparative disadvantage stems from individual or intergroup comparison rather than absolute disadvantage (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012). As seen in the epigraph above, Confucius said, ‘[be] not concerned lest . . . people should be poor, but only lest what they have should be ill-proportioned’, an insight that reflects the essence of RD (Xiong & Ye, Reference Xiong and Ye2016).

RD can be divided into egoistic or individual RD (IRD) and fraternal or group RD (GRD); the former involves interpersonal comparisons, while the latter involves intergroup comparisons (Runciman, Reference Runciman1966; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012). In an extensive study of British society, Runciman (Reference Runciman1966) found that his respondents tended to make interpersonal comparisons that produced IRD, rather than broad social comparisons between their group and other groups. It can be seen that the focus of most previous studies on the consequences of IRD was put on internal states and individually oriented behaviour (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012). However, we contend that as an outcome of interpersonal comparisons, IRD should also have significant interpersonal implications and even intergroup implications. Besides, given that some previous studies have shown that GRD is an incentive for intergroup attitudes and collective behaviour (Toizer, Reference Toizer2016; Zhang, Wang, & Zhou, Reference Zhang, Wang and Zhou2010), and the two types of RD are significantly correlated (Guimond & Dubésimard, Reference Guimond and Dubésimard1983), unique longitudinal effects of GRD and IRD on both wellbeing and protest have been identified (Schmitt, Maes, & Widaman, Reference Schmitt, Maes and Widaman2010). Hence, it is reasonable to expect that IRD may influence individuals’ perceptions of social and interpersonal relationships, as well as their intention to fight and/or resulting violent actions. However, little research has investigated the relationship between IRD and collective behaviours in Chinese people. Therefore, it is important to clarify the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel in Chinese people during the reform era, to elucidate the relationships between variables that affect collective behaviour. This is of great theoretical value, as well as having far-reaching, practical significance for maintaining social stability and promoting the construction of a harmonious society.

Intention to Rebel

Rebellion refers to the act of defying lawful authority or relating to authority or convention in a resistant manner (Mathye, Reference Mathye2009). It is characterised by a response to frustration with the community and new values, which often leads to an ideological ‘revolution’ (Kurzman, Reference Kurzman2003; Vold, Bernard, & Snipes, Reference Vold, Bernard and Snipes2005). A growing body of evidence and theory have associated rebellion with violent conflict, antisocial behaviour, and collective behaviour (Gurr, Reference Gurr1970; Jost et al., Reference Jost, Chaikalis-Petritsis, Abrams, Sidanius, van der Toom and Bratt2012; Offer, Reference Offer1971; Pires, Reference Pires2014). Social anomie theory posits that rebellion is an important cause of mass crime (Ma, Reference Ma2012; Merton, Reference Merton and Anshen1949; Shoham, Knepper, & Kett, Reference Shoham, Knepper and Kett2010; Vold et al., Reference Vold, Bernard and Snipes2005). However, examination of any effect of IRD on collective behaviour has not yet provided a unified conclusion. Some research has tried to relate IRD to protest movements (Pettigrew, Reference Pettigrew, Yinger and Cutler1978; Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Maes and Widaman2010; Vanneman & Pettigrew, Reference Vanneman and Pettigrew1972; Zhang, Liu, & Tian, Reference Zhang, Liu and Tian2016). In Why Men Rebel, Gurr (Reference Gurr1970) explained rebellious behaviour using relative derivative theory and frustration-aggression theory. Gurr posited that IRD, defined as the discrepancy between value expectations and value capabilities, induces frustration and social discontent, which may in turn lead to widespread negative emotions, which then may trigger collective and political violence (Folger, Reference Folger, Olson, Herman and Zanna1986; Gurr, Reference Gurr1970; Napoletano, Elgar, Saul, Dirks, & Craig, Reference Napoletano, Elgar, Saul, Dirks and Craig2016). Moreover, Crosby (Reference Crosby1979) claimed that IRD can lead to ‘violence against society’ and ‘constructive change of society’, and sometimes this can be as violent as a riot (Crosby, Reference Crosby1979; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012). Crosby (Reference Crosby1979) also argued that rebellion should be viewed as an extreme example of violence. In addition, just as Schmitt et al. (Reference Schmitt, Maes and Widaman2010) argued that IRD will produce rebellion instead of or in addition to aggressive behaviour, we contend that the experience of IRD may be inherently compatible with rebellious behaviours.

However, researchers are not permitted to simulate rebellion for moral and ethical reasons. According to the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen1985), behavioural intention is the factor that affects behaviour most directly. Therefore, this study researched the intention to rebel rather than rebellious behaviour.

Future Social Expectations

Individuals who experience RD respond in different ways (Osborne, Smith, & Huo, Reference Osborne, Smith and Huo2012), such as raising their own status through hard work (Zoogah, Reference Zoogah2010), changing the reference group to reduce their RD, or disrupting the group's status quo (Sun & Guo, Reference Sun and Guo2016). Which factors mediate the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel? Smith and Huo (Reference Smith and Huo2014) suggested that people's responses to RD depend on an estimate of the possibility of amelioration. That is, if people believe that there is an opportunity for change (i.e., in an open system), they are likely to respond, even to an undeserved disadvantage, with increased normative effort. However, if there is no such opportunity available (in a closed system), they are likely to engage in deviant or confrontational behaviour. In this respect, future social expectation is a mediator variable worth considering. According to Atkinson and Cartwright's (Reference Atkinson and Cartwright1964) theory of expectancy value and Nurmi's (Reference Nurmi1991) theory of future orientation, future social expectation is defined as an individual's estimate of the future development of society and living environments, based on the reality of the current situation. Although there is no direct evidence linking IRD to future social expectation, considerable circumstantial evidence lends support to the idea that IRD is positively associated with negative future social expectation and negatively associated with positive future social expectation. A previous study has shown that underemployment generates the feeling of IRD, and that IRD in turn adversely affects individuals’ attitudes toward their future careers more generally (Feldman, Leana, & Bolino, Reference Feldman, Leana and Bolino2002). IRD has also been shown to predict negative attitudes toward the social systems (Birt & Dion, Reference Birt and Dion1987; Caskell & Smith, Reference Caskell and Smith1984; Smith & Huo, Reference Smith and Huo2014). Furthermore, some studies indicate that the experience of IRD poses a threat to individuals’ belief in a just world (BJW) (Callan, Shead, & Olson, Reference Callan, Shead and Olson2011; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Tian2016). BJW is the belief that people live in a just world where each person usually gets what they deserve (Lerner & Miller, Reference Lerner and Miller1978); the stronger the BJW, the more confident and the more positive the person's estimation about the future (Sutton & Winnard, Reference Sutton and Winnard2007). From this perspective, we suggest that IRD should also be associated with one's attitudes toward future society. In addition, laboratory participants’ feelings of hope for improved conditions in the future have been shown to predict their collective reactions to RD (Wright, Taylor, & Moghaddam, Reference Wright, Taylor and Moghaddam1990). Moreover, individuals with positive expectations of the future have been shown to be less likely to exhibit drug abuse, alcoholism, suicide, antisocial behaviour, and confrontational tendencies (Carmi & Arnon, Reference Carmi and Arnon2014; Cohen-Chen, Halperin, Crisp, & Gross, Reference Cohen-Chen, Halperin, Crisp and Gross2013; Nurmi, Reference Nurmi1991; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002); in contrast, when individuals believe that the future will involve difficulty and threat, they are likely to rebel rather than exhibit positive behaviour (Bar-Tal, Raviv, Shapira, & Kahn, Reference Bar-Tal, Raviv, Shapira, Kahn, Alon and Bar-Tal2016; Cohen-Chen et al., Reference Cohen-Chen, Halperin, Crisp and Gross2013).

Interpersonal Distrust

Trust is one of the Five Constant Virtues of Confucianism (the others are Benevolence, Righteousness, Propriety, and Wisdom), which are the most important virtues in Chinese tradition and exert a significant influence on the psychology and behaviour of Chinese people. Trust is applied in predicting individual cooperative behaviour and compliance with the overall rules of society (Coleman, Reference Coleman1990; Fukuyama, Reference Fukuyama1995; Paxton, Reference Paxton2002; Scholz & Lubell, Reference Scholz and Lubell1998; Sztompka, Reference Sztompka1999; Tatarko, Reference Tatarko2014; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002; van Lange, Reference Van Vugt, Van Lange, Schaller, Simpson and Kenrick2006). In contemporary society, the distrust crisis has led to several issues such as the ‘credibility trap’, tension between doctors and patients, and poor food safety (Kasperson, Golding, & Tuler, Reference Kasperson, Golding and Tuler1992; Zhang, Guo, & Zhang, Reference Zhang, Guo and Zhang2013); however, very few studies have included distrust as a variable. Therefore, it is important to explore the relationships between interpersonal distrust and IRD, intention to rebel, and future social expectations.

Social psychology posits that income inequality affects levels of trust via subjective injustice (Alesina & Ferarra, Reference Alesina and Ferrara2002; Brockner & Siegel, Reference Brockner, Kramer and Tyler1996; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2002). Severe income inequality increases RD levels in low-income earners (Neckerman & Torche, Reference Neckerman and Torche2007; Ishida, Reference Ishida2014; Greitemeyer & Sagioglou, Reference Greitemeyer and Sagioglou2017), making them more likely to refuse to trust society and other people (Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2010). Experimental and theoretical studies have also shown that RD exerts a strong depressive influence on trustworthiness (Anderson, Mellor, & Milyo, Reference Anderson, Mellor and Milyo2005; Wilkinson & Pickett, Reference Wilkinson and Pickett2007, Reference Wilkinson and Pickett2009). In addition, the occurrence of collective rebellious behaviour throughout history has almost always been related to lack of trust, which is one of the most important factors in controlling intergroup conflict. Some studies have shown that interpersonal trust is significantly negatively correlated with aggressive behaviour (Catherall, Reference Catherall1991; Kinard, Reference Kinard1980, Reference Kinard1982; Malti, Averdijk, Ribeaud, Rotenberg, & Eisner, Reference Malti, Averdijk, Ribeaud, Rotenberg and Eisner2013). Furthermore, Giddens (Reference Giddens1984) posited that human life requires a sense of security and trust, and that when distrust deepens, the anxiety and anger it causes leads to violence.

Interpersonal distrust refers to individuals’ expectations of incapability, negative emotion, and harmful behaviour from others (Cho, Reference Cho2006; Rotter, Reference Rotter1971; Schweer & Siebertz-Reckzeh, Reference Schweer and Siebertz-Reckzeh2014; Zhang & Bond, Reference Zhang and Bond1993). The classical cognitive model of interpersonal trust posits that greater expectation of feedback is associated with a stronger sense of trust (Zhang & Bond, Reference Zhang and Bond1993). Furthermore, Uslaner (Reference Uslaner2010, Reference Uslaner2013) reported that optimism affects individuals’ trust and distrust of strangers; that is, positive expectations of the future serve as the foundation of interpersonal trust (Teng, Jin, & Liu, Reference Teng, Jin and Liu2016; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2010, Reference Uslaner2013). Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that negative expectations of the future increase interpersonal distrust, while positive future social expectations will reduce it.

Overview of the Current Study

This study sought to extend the findings of previous research in several ways. First, we recruited a diverse Chinese community sample to maximise variability in IRD and intention to rebel. In addition, we chose to measure IRD specifically because previous research has considered group relative deprivation, rather than IRD, as a predictive factor for collective behaviour. Moreover, many previous studies that have examined the relationship between IRD and collective behaviour have done so only from a theoretical perspective or by conducting separate studies. Therefore, based on theoretical analysis, this study included two novel variables — future social expectations and interpersonal distrust — to enhance current understanding of the relationship between them.

The first hypothesis was that IRD would be a positive predictor of intention to rebel. The second hypothesis was that positive and negative future social expectations would affect the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel. The third hypothesis was that interpersonal distrust would play a mediating role in the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel. The fourth hypothesis was that IRD would affect interpersonal distrust via the two types of future social expectations and subsequently affect intention to rebel.

Method

Participants

We adopted a stratified random sampling strategy to recruit participants older than 18 years from a municipality in southwest China, and a unified testing method was implemented. The questionnaire was completed anonymously and returned to the researchers immediately following completion. A total of 1,000 questionnaires were distributed, and 807 valid questionnaires were returned. Of the 807 participants, 51.8% were women. They represented diverse occupational backgrounds including administrators in the modern state and society (3.9%); business managers (4.2%); private entrepreneurs (3.4%); professional technicians (9.4%); staff from party and governmental offices, companies, and institutions (23.4%); individual industrial and commercial workers (8.3%); business service personnel (6.6%); manual workers (2.2%); farmers (5.1%); full-time students (20.8%); unemployed people (2.7%); freelancers (4.8%); and others (5.2%). The proportions of participants aged 17–25, 26–35, 36–45, 46–55, and >56 years were 28.0%, 30.8%, 24.7%, 12.9%, and 1.5% respectively, while the ages of 2.1% of participants were not provided. With respect to educational levels, 7.9%, 17.5%, 22.9%, 43.5%, 7.3%, and 0.4% of participants were educated to junior high school level or below, high school/special secondary school level, college level, university level, master's degree level, and doctorate level respectively, and 0.5% of participants did not provide this information.

Measures

We developed a general information questionnaire to collect data regarding demographic characteristics such as sex, age, educational level, and occupation.

IRD

IRD was assessed using the Individual Relative Deprivation Scale (Ma, Reference Ma2012), which consists of four items that measure the degree to which people experience RD. Responses are provided using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree); higher scores indicate a stronger sense of RD. The scale items include ‘With the effort I make and my pay, my life should be better than it is now’; ‘I always feel that others take the things that belong to me’; ‘Compared with the people around me, I am at a disadvantage both in life and at work’; and ‘Most of the wealthy people in the community rely on disgraceful means to make a fortune and steal my opportunities’. Cronbach's alpha for the scale was .79 in the current study.

Future social expectations

A four-item scale developed by Ma (Reference Ma2012) was used to measure attitudes toward future social issues or future social expectations. Responses are provided using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The scale consists of two subscales. The first was Positive Future Social Expectations, whose items were ‘Society is always moving forward in time, and I believe life will get better and better’ and ‘I have full confidence in the future of China’; these two items were combined into an overall index of positive future social expectations, r = .67, p < .01. For the second, Negative Future Social Expectations, items were ‘To tell the truth, I think China's future social problems will be more and more difficult to solve’ and ‘Most people are not optimistic about the future of China’; these two items were combined into an index of negative future social expectations, r = .64, p < .01.

Interpersonal distrust

A three-item scale developed by Ma (Reference Ma2012) was used to measure interpersonal distrust. Responses are provided using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree), in which higher scores indicate stronger interpersonal distrust. The scale items were ‘People take advantage of each other; if you don't be careful, you'll suffer losses’; ‘People do more and more hypocritical things in our society’; and ‘Even the closest person can't be trusted in the current society’. Cronbach's alpha for the scale was .64 in the current study.

Intention to rebel

A six-item scale developed by Ma (Reference Ma2012) was used to measure intention to rebel. Responses are provided using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree), in which higher scores indicate stronger intention to rebel. The scale items include ‘I would use violence against corrupt officials as long as someone else did so first’, ‘In this society, people need blood for blood and tit for tat and are not soft’, ‘The laws that are beneficial only to rich and powerful people can be broken’, ‘The only means of attracting attention to the legitimate rights and interests of ordinary people are strikes or sit-downs’, ‘In today's society, use of illegal means to achieve goals is unavoidable’, and ‘Chinese people can't solve their current problems just on their own and need foreign forces and ideas’. Cronbach's alpha for the scale was .77 in the current study.

Data Analysis

The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 20.0, AMOS 17.0, and MPLUS 7.0. SPSS 20.0 was used to analyse descriptive statistics and perform correlations, AMOS 17.0 was used to perform structural equation modeling, and MPLUS 7.0 was used to assess multiple mediating effects with 1,000 bootstrap samples. We developed and assessed structural equation models based on the hypotheses and determined the extent to which they fit the data. We used the maximum likelihood method with the following fit indices: χ2, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), normative fit index (NFI), incremental fit index (IFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root-mean-square residual (RMR), and adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI). Recent statistical research has demonstrated the importance of examining indirect effects separately, given that an overall total effect is unnecessary for the occurrence of mediation (MacKinnon, Reference MacKinnon, Rose, Chassin, Presson and Sherman2000; Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2008; Shrout & Bolger, Reference Shrout and Bolger2002). Bootstrapped confidence intervals are currently considered the best method for the assessment of mediation. In assessing indirect effects, 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals that do not include zero indicate significant mediation. In the current study, a random sample was used to extract 1,000 bootstrap samples from the original data (N = 807).

Results

Correlation Analysis

The mean scores for IRD, positive future expectations, negative future expectations, interpersonal distrust, and intention to rebel were 3.42 (SD = 0.88, range 1.00–6.00), 4.47 (SD = 0.84, range 1.00–6.00), 3.61 (SD = 0.99, range 1.00–6.00), 3.61 (SD = 0.87, range 1.00–6.00), and 3.34 (SD = 0.89, range 1.00–5.83) respectively; Table 1 shows the correlations between these variables, which were significant. Specifically, IRD was significantly positively correlated with negative future social expectations, interpersonal distrust, and intention to rebel (rs = .58 to .70) and significantly negatively correlated with positive future social expectations (r = −.18); positive future social expectations were significantly negatively correlated with negative future social expectations, interpersonal distrust, and intention to rebel (rs = −.11 to −.19); negative future social expectations were significantly positively correlated with interpersonal distrust and intention to rebel (r = .58 to .64); and interpersonal distrust was significantly positively correlated with intention to rebel (r = .64).

Table 1 Zero-Order Correlations Between Individual Relative Deprivation, Future Social Expectations, Interpersonal Distrust, and Intention to Rebel

Note: N = 807.

***p < .001.

Development and Analysis of Structural Equation Models

The final structural equation model is shown in Figure 1. The results regarding goodness of fit were as follows: χ²(1, 807) = 4.89, RMSEA = .07, CFI = .99, NFI = .99, IFI = .99, TLI = .98, RMR = .01, and AGFI = .96. These values were all well within recommended levels, indicating that the model was acceptable.

Note: IRD = individual relative deprivation. *p < .05, ***p < .001.

Figure 1 Structural equation model of the effects of individual relative deprivation, future social expectations, and interpersonal distrust on intention to rebel.

Significance of Mediating Effects

As shown in Table 2, negative future social expectations exerted a mediating effect on the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel (β = .17, p < .001). In addition, the mediating effect of interpersonal distrust on the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel was significant (β = .11, p < .001). IRD affected interpersonal distrust via negative future social expectations and subsequently affected intention to rebel; therefore, negative future social expectations and interpersonal distrust exerted multiple mediating effects on the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel (β = .04, p < .001). The mediating effect of positive future social expectations on the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel was marginally significant (β = .01, p = .062). The multiple mediating effects of positive future social expectations and interpersonal distrust on the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel were non-significant (p = .362).

Table 2 Indirect Effects Based on 1,000 Bootstrapped Samples

Note: BCa = 95% bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence interval, CI = confidence interval, IRD = independent relative deprivation; SE = standard error.

***p < .001.

Discussion

This study examined the mechanisms underlying the association of IRD with intention to rebel. The results showed that IRD exerted a positive effect on intention to rebel, which supported the first hypothesis. In addition, negative future social expectations exerted a mediating effect between IRD and intention to rebel, and the mediating effect of positive future social expectations was marginally significant, which provided partial support for the second hypothesis. Further, interpersonal distrust played a mediating role in the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel, which supported the third hypothesis. Moreover, RD affected interpersonal distrust via negative future social expectations and subsequently influenced the intention to rebel; however, positive future social expectations and interpersonal distrust did not exert a significant mediating effect on the relationship between IRD and intention to rebel; therefore, the results provided partial support for the fourth hypothesis.

The results showed significant correlations between the five variables examined in this study. Of these, intention to rebel was significantly positively correlated with IRD. That is, the likelihood that individuals will participate in rebellion will rise if they experience high levels of IRD, a finding consistent with relative deprivation theory (Crosby, Reference Crosby1976; Gurr, Reference Gurr1970; Walker & Pettigrew, Reference Walker and Pettigrew1984), where IRD increased collective behaviour. There is literature that suggests that the egoistically deprived individual may externalise their suffering and turn against those who have power in order to improve their situation (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Maes and Widaman2010); other scholars have likewise suggested that IRD can influence group-level outcomes (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012), such as intention and action to cooperate (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Liu and Tian2016), protest (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Maes and Widaman2010), and collective action (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Wang and Zhou2010). Thus, if RD is reduced via appropriate treatment, the incidence of rebellion is also likely to decrease. How this occurs is a question out of the range of this research, and we expect that psychologists, sociologists, and political scientists may work together to find ways to reduce IRD in specific social contexts.

The results also showed that IRD influenced intention to rebel via the mediating role of negative future social expectations. A number of researchers have posited that the relationship between attitude and behaviour is very strong (Mishra & Novakowski, Reference Mishra and Novakowski2016; Upmeyer, Reference Upmeyer1990); that is, that behaviour is an expression of attitudes, and future social expectations — attitudes about the future of society — are not only affected by emotions and the external environment but in turn inevitably affect psychological status and behaviour. When individuals who experience high levels of IRD are pessimistic about the future of society and are influenced by negative emotions, such as resentment and anger, they tend to exhibit stronger intention to rebel. In this regard, the current results are consistent with those of previous studies (Caskell & Smith, Reference Caskell and Smith1984; Nurmi, Reference Nurmi1991) and provide empirical support for the theory of RD (Bernstein & Crosby, Reference Bernstein and Crosby1980; Smith & Huo, Reference Smith and Huo2014) in which IRD has a negative effect on future expectations, resulting in unconventional behaviour. Although positive future social expectations were negatively correlated with intention to rebel in the current study, RD did not suppress intention to rebel entirely via positive future social expectations. In other words, high levels of RD and positive expectations of the future of society were insufficient to eliminate rebellious tendencies. Field theory posits that human behaviour is the product of the interaction between (human) internal and external factors. (Lewin, Reference Lewin1943), and additional cognitive variables and unknown events may change the process of movement from attitudes to behaviours. Fishbein and Ajzen (Reference Fishbein and Ajzen2010) also report that people who appeared to hold positive attitudes toward one behaviour rarely engaged in this behaviour. Therefore, additional variables should be explored to clarify this mechanism in future research.

IRD also predicted intention to rebel indirectly via interpersonal distrust. Previous research has shown that economic inequality leads to a sense of injustice and destroys interpersonal distrust (Bernstein & Crosby, Reference Bernstein and Crosby1980; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Pettigrew, Pippin and Bialosiewicz2012; Uslaner, Reference Uslaner2013). On the other hand, Giddens’ duality of structure can be used as theoretical support: distrust as a psychological inducement can affect the individual's subjective choice and produce extreme anxiety and anger, which eventually leads to violent behaviour; the rebellion itself then becomes an influencing factor and alters feelings of trust and behavioural choices, creating a vicious cycle of distrust and violence (Giddens, Reference Giddens and Franklin1998; Rodriguez, Dibello, Øverup, & Neighbors, Reference Rodriguez, Dibello, Øverup and Neighbors2015). Moreover, Schul and Peri (Reference Schul and Peri2015) also report that trust exerted an impact on individuals’ behavioural decisions.

In addition, negative future social expectations exerted a significant influence on the establishment of interpersonal distrust and indirectly increased intention to rebel; however, positive future social expectations did not have this effect. From one perspective, if individuals foresee considerable risk in the future, this results in negative assessment of the future society, reducing the individual's sense of security and affecting the establishment of trust in society. This finding is consistent with those of previous studies (Featherman & Pavlou, Reference Featherman and Pavlou2003; Zhang & Bond, Reference Zhang and Bond1993). From another perspective, some studies have indicated that although Chinese people were convinced that the main reason for the increasing gap between rich and poor people was social injustice, they also considered this situation to be relatively fair and believed that diligence, striving, and talent were the main factors affecting material satisfaction (Whyte, Reference Whyte, Oi, Rozelle and Zhou2010). When individuals in the reform era believe that their efforts will be successful, they rely on and believe in themselves rather than others in society, which could explain the finding that interpersonal distrust did not exert a significant mediating effect on positive future social expectations in this study. This suggests that intervention could alter individuals’ attitudes toward the future of society by reducing negative future social expectations and enhancing interpersonal trust, to promote the optimisation of individual adaptation behaviour.

Implications

This study applied psychological perspectives and methodology to examine RD. To our knowledge, it was the first study to explore the mechanisms underlying IRD, future social expectations, interpersonal distrust, and intention to rebel and to develop a future social expectations and interpersonal distrust model involving the multiple mediation of IRD and intention to rebel. The establishment and examination of the model not only validated previous theoretical analysis of IRD in group events, collective behaviour and, in particular, group crimes, but also revealed additional factors that affect the relationship between future social expectations (particularly those of a negative nature) and interpersonal distrust. Therefore, the study findings provide a theoretical and empirical foundation for the development of strategies to reduce collective behaviour in China in the reform era. In addition, the study sample included multiple classes and groups, which provides a solid foundation for the universality and promotion of the research.

Limitations and Future Directions

The study was subject to some limitations. Most notably, the data in the study were cross-sectional and correlational. Hence, causal direction of any effects between variables in the study cannot be confirmed and any hypothesised causal directions are purely speculative. It would be beneficial to explore how related variables change over time and in relation to each other. Further, including observational methods in future analyses would likely deepen our understanding of the interaction of related variables and provide empirical evidence to prevent collective behaviour. In addition, most of the questionnaires used in the study were one-dimensional measurement instruments. This limited our insight into the differences between internal variables and the effects of variables other than those included in the study. As far as research tools are concerned, it is necessary to establish measurement tools that involve all dimensions and higher reliability in future research. Furthermore, only self-report questionnaires were used to collect data here; as such, the results presented in this study likely underestimate or overplay the relationships between IRD and intention to rebel. Therefore, a combination of different methods, such as experimental research or third-party assessments, should be used in future studies.

Conclusion

The findings indicated that IRD predicted intention to rebel directly and indirectly via negative future social expectations, interpersonal distrust, and the chain mediating effect of negative future social expectations and interpersonal distrust. Therefore, negative future social expectations and interpersonal distrust are important factors that influence intention to rebel. In view of this, research should not only focus on the direct influence of RD on intention to rebel but also emphasise the indirect impact that occurs via negative future social expectations and interpersonal distrust.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely appreciate Professor James Liu and anonymous reviewers for their comments. This work was supported by the National Social Science Foundation of China (14BSH071); the MOE (Ministry of Education in China) Project of Humanities and Social Sciences (16YJCZH105); and the Graduate Student Innovation Project of Jiangxi Normal University (YC2017-S167).

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally to this work and should be considered co-first authors.

References

Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 163.Google Scholar
Alesina, A., & Ferrara, E.L. (2002). Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics, 85, 207234.Google Scholar
Anderson, L.R., Mellor, J.M., & Milyo, J. (2005). An experimental study of the effects of inequality and relative deprivation on trusting behavior (Working Paper no. 14). Williamsburg, VA: College of William and Mary.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J.W., & Cartwright, D. (1964). Some neglected variables in contemporary conceptions of decision and performance. Psychological Reports, 14, 575590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., Shapira, P., & Kahn, D.T. (2016). Lay psychology of trust/distrust and beyond in the context of an intractable conflict: The case of Israeli Jews. In Alon, I. & Bar-Tal, D. (Eds.), The role of trust in conflict resolution (pp. 197213). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Bernstein, M., & Crosby, F. (1980). An empirical examination of relative deprivation theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 442456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birt, C.M., & Dion, K.L. (1987). Relative deprivation theory and responses to discrimination in a gay male and lesbian sample. British Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 139.Google Scholar
Brockner, J. (1996). Understanding the interaction between procedural and distributive justice: The role of trust. In Kramer, R.M. & Tyler, T.R. (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 390413). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brockmann, H., Delhey, J., Welzel, C., & Yuan, H. (2009). The China puzzle: Falling happiness in a rising economy. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10, 387405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callan, M.J., Shead, N.W., & Olson, J.M. (2011). Personal relative deprivation, delay discounting, and gambling. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 101, 955–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmi, N., & Arnon, S. (2014). The role of future orientation in environmental behavior: Analyzing the relationship on the individual and cultural levels. Society & Natural Resources, 27, 13041320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caskell, G., & Smith, P. (1984). Relative deprivation in black and white youth: An empirical investigation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 121131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catherall, D.R. (1991). Aggression and projective identification in the treatment of victims. Psychotherapy Theory Research & Practice, 28, 145149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, J. (2006). The mechanism of trust and distrust formation and their relational outcomes. Journal of Retailing, 82, 2535.Google Scholar
Cohen-Chen, S., Halperin, E., Crisp, R.J., & Gross, J.J. (2013). Hope in the Middle East: Malleability beliefs, hope, and the willingness to compromise for peace. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 5, 6775.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review, 83, 85113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crosby, F. (1979). Relative deprivation revisited: A response to miller, bolce, and halligan. American Political Science Review, 73, 103112.Google Scholar
Featherman, M.S., & Pavlou, P.A. (2003). Predicting e-services adoption: A perceived risk facets perspective. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 59, 451474.Google Scholar
Feldman, D.C., Leana, C.R., & Bolino, M.C. (2002). Underemployment and relative deprivation among re-employed executives. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 75, 453471.Google Scholar
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Folger, R. (1986). A referent cognitions theory of relative deprivation. In Olson, J.M., Herman, C.P., & Zanna, M.P. (Eds.), Relative deprivation and social comparison: The Ontario symposium (vol. 4, pp. 3355). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Fukuyama, F. (1995). Social capital and the global economy. Foreign Affairs, 74, 89103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giddens, A. (1984). The constitution of society. Rationality & Society, 30, 405428.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1998). Risk society: The context of British politics. In Franklin, J. (Ed.), The politics of risk society (p. 23). Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Greitemeyer, T., & Sagioglou, C. (2017). Increasing wealth inequality may increase interpersonal hostility: The relationship between personal relative deprivation and aggression. Journal of Social Psychology, 157, 766776.Google Scholar
Guimond, S., & Dubésimard, L. (1983). Relative deprivation theory and the Quebec nationalist movement: The cognition–emotion distinction and the personal–group deprivation issue. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44, 526535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurr, T.R. (1970). Why men rebel. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ishida, A. (2014). Income inequality and relative deprivation: A formal theoretic view. XVIII ISA World Congress of Sociology.Google Scholar
Jost, J.T., Chaikalis-Petritsis, V., Abrams, D., Sidanius, J., van der Toom, J., & Bratt, C. (2012). Why men (and women) do and don't rebel: Effects of system justification on willingness to protest. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 197208.Google Scholar
Kasperson, R.E., Golding, D., & Tuler, S. (1992). Social distrust as a factor in siting hazardous facilities and communicating risks. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 161187.Google Scholar
Kinard, E.M. (1980). Emotional development in physically abused children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50, 686696.Google Scholar
Kinard, E.M. (1982). Aggression in abused children: Differential responses to the rosenzweig picture-frustration study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 139141.Google Scholar
Kurzman, C. (2003). Review of the book Why Muslims rebel: Repression and resistance in the Islamic world, by Mohammed M. Hafez. Social Forces, 82, 863865.Google Scholar
Lerner, M.J., & Miller, D.T. (1978). Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 10301051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1943). Defining the ‘Field at a given time’. Psychological Review, 50, 292310.Google Scholar
Ma, A. (2012). Relative deprivation and social adaption: The role of mediator and moderator. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44 (3), 377387.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, D. (2000). Contrasts in multiple mediator models. In Rose, J., Chassin, L., Presson, C., & Sherman, S. (Eds.), Multivariate applications in substance use research (pp. 141160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Malti, T., Averdijk, M., Ribeaud, D., Rotenberg, K.J., & Eisner, M.P. (2013). ‘Do you trust him?’ Children's trust beliefs and developmental trajectories of aggressive behavior in an ethnically diverse sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 445456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathye, L.V. (2009). Therapeutic techniques for treatment of adolescents with rebellious behavior. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of South Africa, Pretoria.Google Scholar
Merton, R.K. (1949). Social structure and anomie: Revisions and extensions. In Anshen, R.N. (Ed.), The family: Its function and destiny (pp. 226257). Oxford, England: Harper.Google Scholar
Mishra, S., & Novakowski, D. (2016). Personal relative deprivation and risk: An examination of individual differences in personality, attitudes, and behavioral outcomes. Personality & Individual Differences, 90, 2226.Google Scholar
Napoletano, A., Elgar, F.J., Saul, G., Dirks, M., & Craig, W. (2016). The view from the bottom: Relative deprivation and bullying victimization in canadian adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31, 34433463.Google Scholar
Neckerman, K.M., & Torche, F. (2007). Inequality: Causes and consequences. Annual Review of Sociology, 33, 335357.Google Scholar
Nurmi, J.E. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental Review, 11, 159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Offer, D. (1971). Rebellion and anti-social behavior. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 31, 1319.Google Scholar
Osborne, D., Smith, H.J., & Huo, Y.J. (2012). More than a feeling: Discrete emotions mediate the relationship between relative deprivation and reactions to workplace furloughs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 628641.Google Scholar
Paxton, P. (2002). Social capital and democracy: An interdependent relationship. American Sociological Review, 67, 254277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T.F. (1978). Three issues in ethnicity: Boundaries, deprivations, and perceptions. In Yinger, J.M. & Cutler, S.J. (Eds.), Major social issues: A multidisciplinary view (pp. 2549). New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Pires, B. (2014). When people rebel: A computational approach to violent collective action (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/1920/8973 Google Scholar
Preacher, K., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, L.M., Dibello, A.M., Øverup, C.S., & Neighbors, C. (2015). The price of distrust: Trust, anxious attachment, jealousy, and partner abuse. Partner Abuse, 6, 298319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J.B. (1971). Generalized expectancies for interpersonal trust. American Psychologist, 26, 443452.Google Scholar
Rowlingson, K. (2011). Does income inequality cause health and social problems? New York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Retrieved from http://www.jrf.org.uk/sites/files/jrf/inequality-income-socialproblems-full.pdf Google Scholar
Rözer, J.J., & Volker, B. (2016). Does income inequality have lasting effects on health and trust? Social Science & Medicine, 149, 3745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runciman, W.G. (1966). Relative deprivation and social justice: A study of attitudes to social inequality in twentieth century Britain. Aldershot, UK: Gregg Revivals.Google Scholar
Schmitt, M., Maes, J., & Widaman, K. (2010). Longitudinal effects of egoistic and fraternal relative deprivation on well-being and protest. International Journal of Psychology Journal International De Psychologie, 45, 122130.Google Scholar
Scholz, J.T., & Lubell, M. (1998). Trust and taxpaying: Testing the heuristic approach to collective action. American Journal of Political Science, 42, 398417.Google Scholar
Schul, Y., & Peri, N. (2015). Influences of distrust (and trust) on decision making. Social Cognition, 33, 414435.Google Scholar
Schweer, M., & Siebertz-Reckzeh, K. (2014). Personal, systemic and transsystemic trust: Individual and collective resources for coping with societal challenges. Mindful change in times of permanent reorganization. Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Shoham, S.G., Knepper, P., & Kett, M. (2010). International handbook of criminology (pp. 163169). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Shrout, P.E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422445.Google Scholar
Smith, H.J., & Huo, Y.J. (2014). Relative deprivation: How subjective experiences of inequality influence social behavior and health. Policy Insights from the Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 1, 231238.Google Scholar
Smith, H.J., Pettigrew, T.F., Pippin, G.M., & Bialosiewicz, S. (2012). Relative deprivation a theoretical and meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 203232.Google Scholar
Stouffer, S.A., Suchman, E.A., Devinney, L.C., Star, S.A., & Williams, R.M. (1949). The American soldier: Adjustment during army life (vol. 1). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Sun, D., & Guo, Y. (2016). Relative deprivation: Wanting, deserving, resentment for not having. Journal of Psychological Science, 39, 714719 Google Scholar
Sutton, R.M., & Winnard, E.J. (2007). Looking ahead through lenses of justice: The relevance of just-world beliefs to intentions and confidence in the future. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 649666.Google Scholar
Sztompka, P. (1999). Trust: A sociological theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tatarko, A. (2014). Trust, cooperative behavior and economic success: When trust is the capital of the person? Hse Working Papers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teng, G., Jin, S., & Liu, H. (2016). Effect of social just expectation on interpersonal trust among college students: Mediating role of positive belief. Journal of Dalian University of Technology, 37, 2225.Google Scholar
Toizer, B. (2016). Perceived essentialism, group relative deprivation, and collective action. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Oberlin College, OH.Google Scholar
Upmeyer, A. (Ed.). (1990). Attitudes and behavioral decisions. New York, NY: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2002). Religion and civic engagement in Canada and the United States. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41, 239254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2010). Social capital, television, and the ‘mean world’: Trust, optimism, and civic participation. Political Psychology, 19, 441467.Google Scholar
Uslaner, E.M. (2013). Trust as an alternative to risk. Public Choice, 157, 629639.Google Scholar
Vanneman, R.D., & Pettigrew, T.F. (1972). Race and relative deprivation in the urban united states. Race, 13, 461486.Google Scholar
Van Vugt, M., & Van Lange, P.A.M. (2006). The altruism puzzle: Psychological adaptations for prosocial behavior. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J.A., & Kenrick, D.T. (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 237261). Madison, CT: Psychosocial Press.Google Scholar
Vold, G.B., Bernard, T.J., & Snipes, J.B. (2005). Theoretical criminology. New York, NY: Oxford Universiy Press.Google Scholar
Walker, I., & Pettigrew, T.F. (1984). Relative deprivation theory: an overview and conceptual critique. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23 (4), 301310.Google Scholar
Whyte, M.K. (2010). Fair versus unfair: How do Chinese citizens view current inequalities? In Oi, J.C., Rozelle, S., & Zhou, X. (Eds.), Growing pains: Tensions and opportunity in China's transformation (pp. 305332). Stanford, CA: Shorenstein Center.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, R.G., & Pickett, K.E. (2007). The problems of relative deprivation: Why some societies do better than others. Social Science & Medicine, 65, 19651978.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, R.G., & Pickett, K.E. (2009). Income inequality and social dysfunction. Annual Review of Sociology, 35, 493511.Google Scholar
Wright, S.C., Taylor, D.M., & Moghaddam, F.M. (1990). The relationship of perceptions and emotions to behavior in the face of collective inequality. Social Justice Research, 4, 229250.Google Scholar
Xiong, M., & Ye, Y.D. (2016). The concept, measurement, influencing factors and effects of relative deprivation. Advances in Psychological Science, 24, 438453.Google Scholar
Zhang, H., Liu, M., & Tian, Y. (2016). Individual-based relative deprivation (IRD) decreases prosocial behaviors. Motivation & Emotion, 40, 112.Google Scholar
Zhang, J. & Bond, M.H. (1993). Target-based interpersonal trust: Cross-cultural comparison and its cognitive model. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 25, 5462.Google Scholar
Zhang, J.P., Guo, Y.F., & Zhang, N. (2013). Creating a harmonious doctor-patient relationship based on positive psychology. Chinese Medical Ethics, 24, 7172.Google Scholar
Zhang, S.W., Wang, E.P., & Zhou, J. (2010). Relative deprivation and relative gratification: The motivation of Chinese mass incidents. Journal of Public Management, 7, 95102.Google Scholar
Zoogah, D.B. (2010). Why should I be left behind? Employees’ perceived relative deprivation and participation in development activities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 159179.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Zero-Order Correlations Between Individual Relative Deprivation, Future Social Expectations, Interpersonal Distrust, and Intention to Rebel

Figure 1

Figure 1 Structural equation model of the effects of individual relative deprivation, future social expectations, and interpersonal distrust on intention to rebel.

Note: IRD = individual relative deprivation. *p p
Figure 2

Table 2 Indirect Effects Based on 1,000 Bootstrapped Samples