Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T03:31:11.434Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pointing and interaction initiation: the behaviour of young children with Down's syndrome when looking at books*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Anthony J. Wootton*
Affiliation:
University of York
*
Department of Sociology, University of York, York, YO1 5DD, UK.

Abstract

Data are derived from videotapes made in the homes of six Down's syndrome children with developmental ages varying between 1;1 and 2;1. The analysis concerns those sequences in which parent and child look at a book together, and more specifically those exchanges where the child initiates communication on a new target on the page. The design of these initiations is examined with respect to their various verbal and non-verbal components. The analysis of pointing, and in particular of point duration, reveals a possible developmental progression from a stage at which point and exchange duration are linked together to one in which the organization of pointing becomes more detached from the exchange as a unit of interaction.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

An earlier version of this paper was presented at the annual Developmental Section meeting of the British Psychological Society in September 1988. The research was financially supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (grant C00232296). I am grateful to Dr J. Local for his advice and assistance with certain aspects of this paper.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, J., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Buckley, S., Emslie, M., Haslegrave, G. & Le Prevost, P. (1986). The development of language and reading skills in children with Doom's syndrome. Portsmouth: Psychology Department, Portsmouth Polytechnic.Google Scholar
Carter, A. L. (1978). From sensori-motor vocalizations to words. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978). From gesture to word: on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In Bruner, J. and Garton, A. (eds), Human growth and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Coggins, T. E., Carpenter, R. L. & Owings, N. O. (1983). Examining early intentional communication in Down's syndrome and non-retarded children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 18. 98106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, C. & McArthur, K. (1981). Hearing loss and treatment in young Down's syndrome children. Child 7. 357–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunn, P. (1985). Speech and language. In Lane, D. & Stratford, B. (eds), Current approaches to Down's syndrome. London: Holt Rinehart.Google Scholar
Hannan, T. E. (1987). A cross-sequential assessment of the occurrences of pointing in 3 to 12 month old human infants. Infant Behavior and Development 10. 1122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things: a study of human learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signalling and the transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 12. 93109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, C. M. (1978). Pointing in the context of a shared activity. Child Development 49. 371–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language 5. 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynell, J. & Huntley, M. (1985). Reynell developmental language scales manual. Windsor, U.K.: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A. & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking in conversation. Language 50. 696735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarplee, C. M. (1989). Confirmation and repair. Paper presented at the British Child Language Seminar, Hatfield Polytechnic.Google Scholar
Wootton, A. J. (1989 a). Speech to and from a severely retarded young Down's syndrome child. In Beveridge, M., Conti-Ramsden, G. & Leudar, I. (eds), Language and communication in mentally handicapped people. London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Wootton, A. J. (1989 b). Remarks on the methodology of conversation analysis. In Roger, D. & Bull, P. (eds), Conversation: an interdisciplinary perspective. Clevedon, U.K.: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Zinober, B. & Martlew, M. (1985 a). The development of communicative gestures. In Barrett, M. (ed), Children's single-word speech. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Zinober, B. & Martlew, M. (1985 b). Developmental changes in four types of gesture in relation to acts and vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3. 293306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar