Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T04:08:38.774Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Constructivist Approaches to First Language Acquisition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 August 2021

Heike BEHRENS*
Affiliation:
University of Basel
*
Address for correspondence: Heike Behrens, University of Basel - German Department, Nadelberg 4 Basel 4001, Switzerland. E-mail: Heike.Behrens@unibas.ch
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Constructivist approaches to language acquisition predict that form-function mappings are derived from distributional patterns in the input, and their contextual embedding. This requires a detailed analysis of the input, and the integration of information from different contingencies. Regarding the acquisition of morphology, it is shown which types of information leads to the induction of (lexical) categories, and to paradigm building. Regarding the acquisition of word order, it is shown how languages with fixed or variable word order profit from stable syntactic hyperschemas, but require a more detailed analyses of the form-function contingencies to identify the underlying, more specific semantic, syntactic and morphological patterns. At a theoretical level, it is shown how findings from acquisition and processing converge into new linguistic theories that aim to account for regular as well as irregular phenomena in language.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Definition and History of Constructivism

Constructivism sees learning as a process by which the learners construct new knowledge based on their current knowledge by integrating the incoming information. With respect to language acquisition, it is conceptually related to acquisition theories such as emergentism (with a focus on how new qualities/representations arise from the interaction of incoming and existing information, for example, how linguistic categories are constructed from distributional information, and usage-based theories which focus on the homology between the information attested in the input and the child's developing language use. The focus is on the child's ability to generalize over the input by studying the affordances of the input through targeted corpus analyses of Child Directed Speech; the way in which the child extracts information and generalizes it to new, related situations; and the generalization processes as such, like the speed and nature of schema and category formation, in dependence of the frequency and functional transparency of the input.

Jerome Bruner, the person associated most prominently with formulating constructivist approach to language learning, sketched the task of the language learner as threefold (Bruner, Reference Bruner1983): to learn the linguistic system (syntax), to learn to refer and express meaning (semantics), and to get something done with words and communicate effectively (pragmatics). While the acquisition of morphosyntax is at the heart of this volume, constructivist and usage-based theories start out from this comprehensive view of language use, and see language learning as driven by the need to communicate. This has implications for what has to be explained (language as explanandum, see below), as well as for the resources available to the learner (information about the linguistic system itself and the distribution and function of units in the input, the explanans). Current versions of constructivism are closely aligned with connectionism (probability-based pattern association) and Construction Grammar as a linguistic theory.

Constructivist theories explain how children reconstruct the system of their target language(s) given their emergent social-cognitive abilities, and their growing experience with the ambient language usage. They start out from the assumption that there innate no innate specifically linguistic knowledge in the form of a “Universal Grammar” that serves as a blueprint of all possible grammatical sentences of all languages, but that children derive their knowledge of language from their environment or input. Language and cognition are dynamic: Language use affects learning because different input styles will lead to individual differences in the learning outcome, just as subsequent changes in language use will affect the linguistic system (language change, lectal variation) (Bybee, Reference Bybee2010; Diessel, Reference Diessel2019).

A major branch of constructivist theories subscribes to the usage-based approach in linguistics. The term was coined in the 1980ies by Ronald Langacker (Reference Langacker1987) and applied to acquisition most notably by Michael Tomasello:

In usage based models of language … all things flow from the actual usage events in which people communicate linguistically with one another. The linguistic skills that a person possesses at any given moment in time - in the form of a “structured inventory of symbolic units” - result from her accumulated experience with language across the totality of usage events in her life. (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2000, p. 61–62)

This leads to the following general predictions:

  1. a) Usage-based theory is compatible with universal linguistic categories, but these would be derived from general processing constraints because only structures that are learnable by the next generation can be transmitted (Christiansen & Chater, Reference Christiansen and Chater2016; Greenhill et al., Reference Greenhill, Wu, Hua, Dunn, Levinson and Gray2017; Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2020; Kirby, Reference Kirby2017). Hence, linguistic structures are shaped by domain general learning processes (Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2020).

  2. b) Constructivist research covers the whole range of linguistic diversity. Individual languages pose individual acquisition spaces (Slobin, Reference Slobin and Slobin1985), such that children induce the form-function patterns of their target language from the input. Hence, we not only expect typological differences between vastly different languages, but also intra-typological differences between closely related languages or varieties of a language (e.g., British vs. America English). Consequently, there is a close match between the linguistic structures used by the child and the input s/he receives, which is particularly noticeable in intra-typological comparisons.

  3. c) If linguistic knowledge is language-specific, emergent, and usage-based, different experiences should lead to individual differences in linguistic representations (Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2012; Kidd, Donnelly & Christiansen, Reference Kidd, Donnelly and Christiansen2018).

Linguistic Theory: Cognitive Linguistics and Construction Grammar

Construction Grammar integrates formal and functional properties of regular as well as (partially) idiomatic constructions (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg1995; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello1998). Tomasello furthermore draws on elements of Langacker's Cognitive Grammar (Reference Langacker1987) regarding the nature of linguistic representation and generalization. The view on language and cognition is broader than that of linguistic nativism with its focus on (innate) syntax, as the task of the learner is to acquire all of language, irrespective of how common or generalizable certain constructions are (Allen & Behrens, Reference Allen, Behrens and Hagoort2019; Ambridge & Lieven, Reference Ambridge and Lieven2011; Ibbotson, Reference Ibbotson2013, Reference Ibbotson2020).

The Target of language development: The nature of linguistic representations

In order to define acquisition processes, not only the explanans (learning mechanisms, available data) needs to be identified, but also the explanandum (theory of language). Usage-based implementations of Construction Grammar challenge the assumption that language is in essence a structured rule-based system, and propose that it is an inventory of more or less variable schemas instead:

(W)hen speakers use language for communicative purposes, they repeat recurrent linguistic forms to reach recurrent communicative goals. The repeated correlations between linguistic forms and communicative goals contribute to keeping two kinds of processes in motion: social processes taking place in speech communities which establish and sustain linguistic conventions, and cognitive processes operating in the minds of individual speakers which establish and sustain individual linguistic knowledge. usage and the social and cognitive processes keep reinforcing each other as long as speakers keep using the language, and this allows shared conventions and individual knowledge to emerge, persist, change, and embrace more or less creative innovations, both in language and in the external world. All this taken together, and in fact not much else, makes a language. (Schmid, Reference Schmid2020, p. 3)

Language emerges through entrenchment in the individual mind, and conventionalization as a cooperative process in the society. Current usage-based models see linguistic structures as an emergent property of communication and routinization. Shared communicative practices lead to the stabilization of structures, which are flexible at the same time because communicative conventions are subject to change. As differences in the generalizability of constructions are the norm, low-frequency or emergent schemas are a part of language just as the more regular ones. Linguistic “rules” are very general schemas (Langacker, Reference Langacker1987), but do not hold a special status in the system (cf. the “rule-list-fallacy”, Langacker, Reference Langacker1987, p. 42).

Individual differences

Constructivist theories as discussed here do not assume innate linguistic representations shared by all speakers. This alters the task of language learning from activating innate settings and linking them to the units of the E-language, to reconstructing the underlying system from the evidence learners hear:

From a usage-based perspective, such (individual) differences are expected. Language learners do not have direct access to the cognitive routines that other speakers use to produce utterances, only to the utterances themselves. (Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2020, p. 215)

Speakers converge by and large to the certain linguistic conventions, but this does not entail that they share the same mental representation (Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2020; Kidd & Donnelly, Reference Kidd and Donnelly2020; Kidd et al., Reference Kidd, Donnelly and Christiansen2018). Utterance can be (re)produced by different cognitive routines, given different types of experiences of the speakers, and different kinds or degrees of generalization. Dąbrowska (Reference Dąbrowska2020) calls this multicollinearity: The convergence of cues can lead to convergence between speakers, even if they identify and process different cues.

A further implication of individual differences is that speakers’ generalizations may differ from the generalizations or “rules” that linguists identify (Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2020). Valid generalizations about aspects of a language need not be “captured in the mental grammars of all, or even most, speakers” of that language (p. 223). L2-learners of Polish genitives but also native speakers come up with different generalization patterns in task were they have to inflect unknown words. While some pick up on the semantics underlying the distribution of allomorphs (the ending -a correlates highly with animate masculine nouns, whereas -u correlates with inanimate nouns), others seem to have rote-learned the markers and inflect new nouns based on phonological similarities, or even randomly.

The continuity, linking and developmental problem of acquisition

Recall that the rationale for proposing a Universal Grammar was that certain linguistic operations could not be abstracted from language use, because they relied on structure-dependent operations which are conceived as symbolic operations. A further assumption is that innate categories allow the child to operate with adult-like categories (continuity assumption). According to Pinker (Reference Pinker1984) continuity in representation helps to avoid the problem of defining transitory categories and how/when they turn into adult-like categories. If one assumes that the mind operates with symbolic rules, a form of “mentalese” (Pinker, Reference Pinker1994), continuity is needed. In this model, the mind translates the items to be processed (e.g., sequences of morphemes) into grammatical categories that the syntactic rules operate over. Such a “dual-mechanism model” reduces storage or memory load, since only exceptions to the rules (e.g., irregular forms) need to be memorized like lexical items, whereas all regular structures would be generated by rule (Pinker & Prince, Reference Pinker and Prince1991). This model requires that all incoming linguistic strings can be parsed and linked unambiguously to their respective grammatical categories in order for the rules to work (the “linking-problem”), as well as a specification of all the operations units of a certain category can undergo in the operation of “feature checking” in the merging process as proposed in the recent minimalist program (Yang, Crain, Berwick, Chomsky & Bolhuis, Reference Yang, Crain, Berwick, Chomsky and Bolhuis2017).

These mechanisms are not necessary if one does not assume innate linguistic categories, but if more or less generalizable schemas are derived from input patterns. The alternative “single mechanism model” was first implemented in connectionist models and based on findings from cognitive neuroscience (Elman, Bates, Johnson, Karmiloff-Smith, Parisi & Plunkett, Reference Elman, Bates, Johnson, Karmiloff-Smith, Parisi and Plunkett1996; Rumelhart & McClelland, Reference Rumelhart, McClelland and MacWhinney1987; Twomey & Westermann, Reference Twomey, Westermann, Horst and Torkildsen2019). The strength of constructivist accounts is their focus on productivity in order to determine the nature and extent of the generalizations children make across development by looking at detailed naturalistic corpora, or by experimentally testing children's performance with familiar and less familiar items. This makes the single-mechanism model parsimonious, as models that can learn structures with low generalizability should easily learn regular phenomena as well (Fillmore, Reference Fillmore1988).

Language learning as a re-construction of the target language

Like connectionist models, usage-based approaches to acquisition are based on pattern association. For the beginning language learner, some of the linguistically complex units are represented as chunks with no representation of the internal constituency. Automatized “units” facilitate processing:

Unit: A cognitive structure mastered by a speaker to the point that it can be employed in a largely automated fashion, without requiring attention to its individual parts or their arrangements. A unit is sufficiently well entrenched to be easily evoked as an integrated whole, that is, carried out more or less automatically once initiated. (Langacker, Reference Langacker1987, p. 494)

Consequently, constructivist accounts ask: (a) what are the units developing learners operate with, and why; and (b) how do they generalize over different units? A key finding is that children's early generalizations are item-based or low-level schemas. E.g., children first learn about the participants in events of kissing (kisser – kissee), throwing (thrower – thing-thrown – recipient), before they see semantically based similarities (agent, patient, recipient, instrument etc.). Later, such roles can become associated with domain-general grammatical functions like subject and object (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello1992). A fundamental process is drawing associations across related instances. In order to do so, children need to build memory traces that allow them to compare new instances with entrenched ones:

This accumulated linguistic experience undergoes processes of entrenchment, due to repeated uses of particular expressions across usage events, and abstraction, due to type variation in constituents of particular expressions across usage events. (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2000, p. 62).

Langacker (Reference Langacker, Barlow and Kemmer2000, p. 4–6) defines the processes of schema formation and categorization for linguistic units. For example, learners recognize that reference to plural entities or past events correlate with certain affixes. These can develop into schematic frames like she VERB-ed or two NOUN-s. If learners already knows a few past tense forms, and hears a new form like gorped in a similar context, they will be able to associate or categorize this new form with known past tense forms. In sum, constructivist accounts rely on associative learning mechanism (as in connectionist models). But the human learner – unlike current computational models – has full access to the information available through the communicative grounding, and s/he has a communicative intention or goal. Hence, associative learning mechanisms like pattern finding and social-cognitive learning mechanisms like intention reading work together in establishing form-function relationships (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2003).

Complexity reduction through input simplification

The learning mechanisms proposed here draw on the child's ability to distil categorical and functional information from input patterns. For a realistic assessment of the input conditions, detailed analyses of the distributional evidence available to the child in (dense) corpora of naturalistic speech are made (Theakston & Lieven, Reference Theakston and Lieven2017). There is robust evidence that Child Directed Speech (CDS) represents only a subset of the complexity and variation attested in adult language. In a comparative study on Dutch, Danish, German and Hebrew, Ravid et al. (Reference Ravid, Dressler, Nir-Sagiv, Korecky-Kröll, Souman, Rehfeldt, Gillis and Behrens2008) found that CDS typically uses a smaller core set of markers which depict certain phonotactic regularities more clearly than the more variable adult–adult language. Xanthos et al. (Reference Xanthos, Laaha, Gillis, Stephany, Aksu-Koç, Christofidou and Dressler2011) demonstrated complexity reduction in terms of smaller paradigms even in morphologically rich languages. Allen (Reference Allen, Fortescue, Mithun and Evans2017) makes a similar point for input in polysynthetic Inuit languages. Caregivers use a simplified register including shorter nursery words to younger children. Complexity reduction in the input of course increases the learnability. Hence, distributional analyses of CDS are essential to derive corpus-based control-data for experimental studies.

Predictions for the acquisition of specific domains

In the following I will characterize the predictions for morphological categories, and then deal more specifically with the rich set of factors that contribute to the acquisition of fixed and variable word order constructions.

A. Acquisition of morphology

Learning morphology involves two problems:

  1. a) Paradigm-building: Identifying the inflectional categories encoded in a language, and the markers in different distributional contexts.

  2. b) Rule-abstraction: identifying the criteria for allomorphy, i.e., when one inflectional category is encoded by several markers (e.g., regular vs. irregular markers).

Rule-abstraction has been subject to fierce and detailed theoretical debates between association learning (Kirov & Cotterell, Reference Kirov and Cotterell2018) and rule-based dual-mechanisms accounts (Pinker & Prince, Reference Pinker and Prince1991), and will not be discussed here. Less attention has been paid to paradigm-building and the nature of morphological categories. Here, the child has to identify the compositional nature of word forms as well as the functions that the affixes encode. In a large typological survey, Slobin (Reference Slobin and Slobin1997) shows that there is no fixed set of privileged grammaticizable notions, so learning mechanisms are needed that can account for the full range of language-specific categories and their idiosyncrasies.

The paradigm structure of morphological categories can be quite different. Many languages encode subject-verb agreement, but only a few have a specific marker for each person-number combination (e.g., Latin), whereas others have some syncretism (the same form for several person-number distinctions) like German where 1st and 3rd person plural endings are identical. Yet other languages have broken paradigms with overt marking in only a subset of the paradigm, as in English, where only third person singular present tense is overtly encoded, but not on modal verbs. Such isolated inflectional markers are often remnants of earlier, more complete paradigms where the high frequency of some forms has led to conserving effects and occasional redundant marking (Bybee, Reference Bybee2010), as subject-verb relations is now reliably encoded by word order in English. As a consequence, a learning mind that looks for specific, innate categories and/or strives for maximal generalization would be confronted with a scenario where not all categories that might be expected (e.g., number or tense) are in fact realized in a given language, while the same language might encode more particular, non-universal categories that necessarily have to be derived from language use, and where the paradigms are often not only irregular but broken. Therefore morphology provides a parade-case where irregularity and language-specificity is the norm rather than the exception. So what are the cues that children can use to learn morphology? The following predictions should apply to all morphological categories, irrespective of how regular or productive they are.

Segmentation of morphemes through variation

The first task is to identify the compositional nature of word forms. In English, this is an additive process as children tend to start out with uninflected forms and acquire the rather limited number of inflectional and derivational morphemes in a rather slow fashion (Brown, Reference Brown1973). More commonly are deconstructive processes in languages where the child only encounters inflected word forms, but no bare stems. Here, tracing the formal variation in dependence of the context is needed. This process is both word-based and pattern-based. Regarding individual words (e.g., nouns or verbs), children are informed by changes of the word form in different distributional contexts that allows them to segment words into stem and affix(es) and build (mini-)paradigms (Ravid, Keuleers & Dressler, Reference Ravid, Keuleers, Dressler, Pirrelli, Plag and Dressler2020).

Semantic accessibility

A further factor is semantic accessibility. Plural inflection is typically acquired early because it has a clear observable correlate (Clark & Nikitina, Reference Clark and Nikitina2009), but this is not the case for all morphological categories. The classification of German nouns into masculine, feminine and neuter is not based on biological sex, but mainly based on phonotactic properties of the noun, with some semantic predictors (Szagun, Stumper, Sondag & Franik, Reference Szagun, Stumper, Sondag and Franik2007). The different inflectional classes of the Polish genitive have some semantic correlates, but they are not identified by all speakers (Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2012).

Prediction regarding paradigms with different phonological or semantic family size

In cases where the inflectional distinctions do not correlate with semantic categorization, forms will have to be stored as exemplars with low generalizability, unless phonological cues will allow pattern abstraction. The prediction that therefore follows here is an interaction between form frequency and phonological neighborhood density on the proportion of correctly-inflected forms vs errors (e.g., in an elicited production study of verb tense and agreement morphology), such that higher form frequency predicts higher rates of correct production (and fewer errors) unless the phonological neighborhood density is high. Repeated failure to find this interaction, across a range of well-powered studies of different inflectional paradigms, in which children are producing sufficiently high rates of errors to test the prediction, would therefore constitute evidence against this account. German past participles would provide a suitable testing ground. They are formed by up to three markers: the unstressed prefix ge-; the weak or strong ending -t or -en, and, in case of strong verbs, a wide range of changes to the stem vowel. Weak verbs like machen – gemacht ‘made – made’ are productive in contemporary German and have a high type frequency. All strong verbs take the ending -en, but there are 15 different types of vowel gradation with 4 to 21 members (count based on personal coding of German CHILDES-corpora), e.g., kriechen – gekrochen (crawl – crawled) or reissen – gerissen (tear – torn). Previous research has shown that children make errors on the prefix, the suffix, and vowel gradation (Szagun, Reference Szagun2011). Strong verbs have lower type, but high token frequency. Within strong verbs, the suffix -en has higher type frequency than the less predictable and more variable vowel gradation. In frequency controlled nonce-verb tasks it should be easier to induce -en errors than stem vowel errors.

Productivity through (frequent) morphological frames

Lexical items often occur in morphological frames like The NOUN VERB-s, where the slots can be filled by different items. Frequent frames contribute to category formation (Mintz, Reference Mintz2003). In a crosslinguistic study on seven typologically diverse languages Moran et al. (Reference Moran, Blasi, Schikowski, Kuntay, Pfeiler, Allen and Stoll2018) looked at trigrams (A-B-C) and investigated the predictive power categorizing the B-element based on A and C. Irrespective of the language type, the results were most stable for inferring lexical categories like nouns and verbs, presumably because the stable context allowed the children to abstract away from the details of the inflectional form.

Prediction regarding category induction

Morphological frames promote category induction by opening a “similarity space” for the slot-filling element(s). Thus, when controlling for other factors such as semantic and discourse properties – morphological frames with higher transitional frequency (e.g., He's VERB-ing) will be acquired earlier than similar frames with lower transitional frequency (e.g., They're VERB-ing) such that, for example, children will be able to use a novel verb at a younger age in the former frame than the latter in an experimental elicited production study. Repeated failure to find such a difference in suitably controlled and well-powered studies across a range of domains would therefore constitute evidence against the present account.

In the next sections (B, C), I will focus on the acquisition of word order in three different scenarios: fixed word order as in English, variable word order as in German and Dutch. In both cases, the surface patterns are high-frequency schemas, but the underlying form-function relations require a more detailed analysis, based on the integration of different cues. I will argue that both fixed and variable word order languages can be seen as compilations of different constructions that have their own developmental trajectories before network associations are formed. As a third example, word order in complex wh-questions is briefly discussed (D). Here, there is limited evidence in the input for the concrete structures, such that children will have to rely on inferences based on simpler component structures.

B. Acquisition of fixed word order languages

Constructivist accounts predict that children will exploit distributional cues to establish form-function mappings. According to the competition model (Bates & MacWhinney, Reference Bates, MacWhinney and MacWhinney1987), languages differ as to how many different cues there are to a certain function, how reliable these cues are, and how difficult they are to process in terms of perceptual salience and frequency. The more fixed and canonical word order patterns are, the more predictive are the transitional probabilities (Ibbotson, Solnikov & Walker, Reference Ibbotson2020). English sentences are highly templatic given the relatively small number of determiners and pronouns, mostly without case distinctions, and verbal inflections. This should facilitate the abstraction of typical word order patterns. Yet even in English learning word order is demanding because variation in word order encodes different functions. Two issues are relevant here: The identification of different sentence types, and the association between position and thematic roles that leads to different interpretations of the sentences.

Word order and sentence type

In an analysis of input addressed to English-speaking children, Cameron-Faulkner, Lieven, and Tomasello (Reference Cameron-Faulkner, Lieven and Tomasello2003) found that only 15% of the input sentence addressed to 2-3-yr-olds had basic SVO word order, whereas the majority of utterances were questions, imperatives, demonstrative copulaic constructions (that's, there's, here's X), or sentence fragments. However, there was a limited number of different sentence onsets with high token-frequency, which facilitates the identification of sentence types.

At first sight, the simple English transitive poses a problem to the tenet of Construction Grammar that constructions are form-function units, since it seems to be impossible to abstract a meaning for quite different relations. The problem of such “meaningless constructions” is resolved when one assumes that the transitive surface pattern is rather a “hyperschema” that subsumes different form-function mappings or linking relations between different verbs and their arguments (cf. Hilpert, Reference Hilpert2019, p. 70ff.; examples from Ambridge, Reference Ambridge2019, p. 20):

  1. (1) Linking-relations in the English transitive

    Contact (non-causative) [AGENT] [ACTION] [PATIENT] John hit Bill

    Causative [CAUSER] [ACTION] [UNDERGOER] John broke the plate

    Experiencer-Theme [EXPERIENCER] [EXPERIENCE] [THEME] John feared Bill

    Theme-Experiencer [THEME] [EXPERIENCE] [EXPERIENCER] John frightened Bil

    ‘Weigh’ Construction [THING] [MEASURE/COST/WEIGH] [AMOUNT] John weighed 100lbs

    ‘Contain’ Construction [CONTAINER] [CONTAIN] [CONTENTS] The tent sleeps four people

Ambridge (Reference Ambridge2019) follows Abbot-Smith and Tomasello (Reference Abbot-Smith and Tomasello2006) in arguing that children have to proceed in an exemplar-based fashion and learn these constructions in an item-specific way. Fixed generalizations by, for example, equating the grammatical subject with the agent-role would lead to a wrong interpretation of John feared Bill. Only experience can teach children which form-function mappings generalize because they have a prototypical core, and which represent “constructional isolates” or specific pattern that are instantiated by few verbs only.

Sentence interpretation: Association between word order and thematic roles

According to the competition model (Bates & MacWhinney, Reference Bates, MacWhinney and MacWhinney1987), languages differ as to how many different cues there are to function, how reliable these cues are, and how difficult they are to process in terms of perceptual salience. In a cross-linguistic sentence-interpretation task with transitive sentences such as The dog the cat chases or The eraser chases the pig, Bates et al. (Reference Bates, MacWhinney, Caselli, Devescovi, Natale and Venza1984) found that depending on the language, speakers weigh cues differently. In English, word order was the most reliable cue for the interpretation of such prototypical transitive sentences for adults. Since children learning English do not hear variable word order in declaratives, and learn to use word order as a strong cue for sentence processing, they do not automatically generalize that a verb heard intransitively (The cow is meeking) can be used transitively even if it depicts a transitive action (Brooks & Tomasello, Reference Brooks and Tomasello1999; Tomasello & Brooks, Reference Tomasello and Brooks1998). Children's reliance on item-specific vs verb-general word order was also tested in weird-word-order experiments: When English-speaking children were taught a new verb describing a transitive action in non-canonical word order (SOV: Elmo the car dacked; VSO: Dacked Elmo the car), they did not reliably correct it to default SVO-word order until 4 years of age (Akhtar, Reference Akhtar1999). The younger groups (2 and 3 year-olds) predominantly copied the word order they heard the particular verb in, but did correct “weird word orders” with familiar verbs like push to a higher extent.

Goldberg and Casenhiser (Reference Goldberg and Casenhiser2005) tested when and whether children are ready to accept non-canonical word order if it represents a new function, in this case appearance (The rabbit the hat mopoed), where the second NP denotes the location of the appearance (video: rabbit appeared on the hat), and all nonce verbs ended on -o to provide a morphological cue (moopo, vako, suto, keebo, fego). They found that 5 to 7-yr-old children were able to learn that new construction, in particular when the frequency distribution was skewed towards one verb. Experiments like this show that children become better at fast mapping argument structure constructions with age, especially when morphological and distributional cues support unambiguous generalization.

Prediction

The acquisition of fixed word order is facilitated by the templatic nature of the input patterns. However, even simple transitive sentences represents a number of different constructions in terms of form-function mappings, such that too early generalizations based on highly frequent or prototypical linking patterns could hinder the acquisition of non-prototypical and/or low frequency patterns. Whether learners at different stages treat different thematic role assignments (e.g., experiencer-theme vs theme-experience) with the same surface word order (SVO) as the same or as different constructions could be tested by sentence processing experiments. In the visual-world paradigm, eye-tracking information is used to trace the online processing. When analyzing sentences like Susan frightened John vs Susan feared John, the method could be used to see if children have an “agent-first” interpretation strategy, or whether they wait for the verb to identify the correct thematic role.

C. Acquisition of “movement constructions” in variable word order languages

In Construction Grammar no movement operations are assumed. Instead, linguistic units that differ in word order and function constitute different constructions with their respective functions. The basic hypothesis is again that each construction would be acquired from exemplars of that same construction. In principle, these constructional schemas could be independent of one another, but since they overlap in lexical material and co-occur in conversations, children should be able to infer the relationship between different constructions based on distributional evidence (see Diessel, Reference Diessel2019, for a survey of types of network relations). The acquisition problem addressed in this section deals specifically with the different kinds of network relations that result from stable form-function and position-mappings in languages that show a more variable word order.

Küntay and Slobin (Reference Küntay, Slobin, Slobin, Gerhardt, Guo and Kyratzis1996, p. 267) identified variation sets in the input where the communicative goal is kept constant but the form varies, as in these examples from a father to his 2;3 year-old son:

  1. (2) Who did we see when we went to the store?

    Who did we see?

    Who did we see in the store?

    Who did we see today?

    When we went out to shopping, who did we see?

Such variation sets help children to build paradigmatic and syntagmatic relations in that they realize which elements can replace one another, and also how structures can be expanded or moved around, in this case for pragmatic topicalization. At the same time, the stable core of the utterance schema is entrenched through repetition.

Similar variation occurs in languages that have variable word order. A classic case is the so-called verb-second phenomenon in some West-Germanic languages like Dutch and German. Finite and nonfinite parts of the inflected verbs occur in different positions and form a so-called “sentential bracket” or Satzklammer (3). This leads to five topological fields: Finite verbs occur in second position (declaratives, 3.a and b) or sentence initially in yes/no-questions (3.c), but in final position in subclauses (d). When the verb is complex (either a separable particle verb or a main verb plus auxiliary), the verb complex is separated by the middle (3 b,c). With few exceptions, only one constituent occupies the pre-field. The pre-field can be occupied by the subject and wh-pronouns like in English, but also by other topicalized constituents (3.b). As a consequence, most arguments and adjuncts fill the middle-field, which leads to discontinuity between the finite and non-finite part of complex verbs, though extraposition to the post-field is possible and reduces discontinuity (3.b).

  1. (3) The topological model of sentence structure in German

    pre-field V1/V2 middle field V-final post-field

    1. a. Simplex verb: SVO in main clauses

      Peter bringt dem Kind Geschenke

      Peter brings the child gifts

    2. b. Sentential bracket with complex verbs or auxiliary + verb

      Peter bringt dem Kind Geschenke mit

      Peter brings the child gifts along

      Dem Kind will Peter Geschenke bringen zum Spielen

      The child-DAT will Peter-NOM gifts bring to play

      Geschenke hat Peter dem Kind mitgebracht

      Gifts-ACC has Peter the child along-brought

    3. c. yes/no-questions:

      Bringt Peter dem Kind Geschenke mit?

      Brings Peter the child gifts with?

      Wird Peter dem Kind Geschenke bringen?

      Will Peter the child gifts bring?

    4. d. subclause: SOV

      …, weil er dem Kind Geschenke bringen will.

      …, because he the child gifts bring will.

Learners have to deal with the discontinuous nature of complex verbs and to identify the regular association between type of inflection (finite/non-finite) and position. This phenomenon has attracted considerable attention in the generative literature where it is assumed that the finite verb raises from the finite SOV-base position to the V1/V2 position in main clauses (“verb second effect”). As this phenomenon is attested in some (German, Dutch, Afrikaans), but not all Germanic languages, it was argued that the type and extent of verb movement is licensed by aspects of verb morphology (Ambridge & Lieven, Reference Ambridge and Lieven2011; Blom, Reference Blom2007; Lasser, Reference Lasser2002; Westergaard, Reference Westergaard2009).

Alternatively, the sentential bracket can be described as a coalition of different sentence structures, just like the hyperschema for English transitives. The basic distributional facts are quite simple: With few exceptions, only one constituent can, but need not precede the finite verb in declarative sentences and questions, and the non-finite verb or verb particle is typically found at the end of the clause. Traditional grammar accounts identified 28 basic syntactic templates, based on argument structure and the distributional constraints of the verbs (Wöllstein-Leisten, Hilmann, Stephan & Vikner, Reference Wöllstein-Leisten, Hilmann, Stephan and Vikner1997). There is ample evidence for the position of finite and non-finite verbs in the input which should allow for rapid pattern abstraction. Behrens (Reference Behrens2006) computed the production rate in a dense-database of child and child-directed speech, when the child was between 4;4 and 4;11. In a lively conversation, the child hears and produces several hundred verb forms per hour (4).

  1. (4) Production rate per hour (based on a 40-hour sample)

    Child Adults

    Main verbs 209 469

    Copulas 58 120

    Auxiliaries 49 136

However, like in English, the acquisition problem becomes more complex when semantic-pragmatic effects of topicalization and extraposition are concerned (see below). From a Construction Grammar perspective, verb-first-constructions like yes-/no-questions, imperatives, and certain types of conditionals can be characterized by their pragmatic properties as non-assertive (Diessel, Reference Diessel, Verspoor, Dong and Sweetser1997). I.e., in adult German we see word order variation that is linked to sentence type (question, declarative, imperative; main clause vs. subclause) and morphology that encodes semantic and pragmatic distinctions (agreement, tense, modality). So rather than seeing word order variation as a uniform phenomenon where the verb “moves” depending on sentence type and verb inflection, one can conceive of German word order as a family of constructions where word order variation encodes different types of information, but where these constructions converge on the limitations imposed by the schematic nature of the sentential bracket. Hence, Construction Grammar does not assume that there is a basic word order from which others are derived by movement, but rather sees different word order patterns as schematic representations that encode different sentence types as well as information structure (Boas & Ziem, Reference Boas, Ziem, Boas and Ziem2018; Welke, Reference Welke2019). This entails that the learning problem turns from the acquisition of a rule that licenses verb-movement to one where numerous constructional schemas have to be acquired (i.e., the form-function correspondences between different sentence types and inflectional markers).

In the acquisition of word order in German and Dutch we see a remarkable developmental change in children's grammar in the second year of life: children initially predominantly use non-finite particles, infinitives and past participles in sentence-final word order, and a smaller set of finite verbs in correct V1/2-position (Behrens, Reference Behrens1993; Poeppel & Wexler, Reference Poeppel and Wexler1993; Wijnen, Kempen & Gillis, Reference Wijnen, Kempen and Gillis2001). Previous studies identified several sources of information in the input that contribute to building syntagmatic and paradigmatic relations, but their exact interplay across developmental time has yet to be determined. Based on these findings, I will sketch a scenario with changing predictors over developmental time, where finite and non-finite verbs and their arguments will be acquired as different clusters, and where the frequency of specific verb forms and their left and right neighbors interacts with an attentional focus on the beginning or end of the utterance and a growing processing window.

Sentence processing: Learning from the right and left periphery

Attention tuning, to find out where and how the relevant information is encoded, is essential for language learning (Ellis, Reference Ellis2006a, Reference Ellis2006b; Slobin, Reference Slobin and Slobin1985). Utterance-initial and utterance-final positions are particularly salient, such that tuning in on these positions provides the child with strong cues about word order. This was shown in a computational modelling study of the acquisition of word order where the model expanded the processing window with either a focus on the left periphery for English and Spanish, or a focus on the right periphery for Dutch and German (Freudenthal, Pine, Aguado-Orea & Gobet, Reference Freudenthal, Pine, Aguado-Orea and Gobet2007).

Distributional evidence from utterance-final clusters with non-finite verb forms

A large proportion of early verb forms in German and Dutch are nonfinite infinitives or past participles (runtergefallen ‘down-fallen’), often preceded by the topic object (Ball spielen ‘ball play-INF’). The predominance of infinitives in early child Dutch or German is an effect of their salient sentence final position and the semantically heavy content of lexical verbs (activity, achievement and accomplishment verbs). Non-finite verb forms are typically preceded by direct and indirect objects, hence the end of the clause is occupied by lexical elements rich in information about the event itself. In the absence of finiteness markers for agreement, tense and modality, most utterances have what researchers called a “modal” or non-actual reading because it is not anchored on the time-line: An utterance like Eis essen ‘ice cream eat-INF’ encodes deontic modality as in I want to eat ice cream (Blom, Reference Blom2007; Hoekstra & Hyams, Reference Hoekstra and Hyams1998; |Jordens, Reference Jordens, Jordens and Lalleman1988, Reference Jordens2002; Lasser, Reference Lasser and Schaeffer1997; Schimke & Dimroth, Reference Schimke and Dimroth2018).

In sum, children derive clusters rich in information from the right periphery of German clauses that encode the “comment” in terms of topic-comment information structure. They will produce truncated subject- or object-verb clusters with activity and telic verbs that are frequently attested in clause-final position. By now, this is quite a robust finding in the acquisition literature (Freudenthal, Pine & Gobet, Reference Freudenthal, Pine and Gobet2009; Wijnen et al., Reference Wijnen, Kempen and Gillis2001). A new take on this distributional facts is to study the development over time, in particular how children become flexible with using the same lexical items in both positions (see below). In order to develop this argument, we will first look at how children acquire the left periphery of German clauses where finite verbs are found.

Distributional evidence from utterance-initial clusters with finite verb forms

In contrast to the event-centered information at the right periphery, utterance initial clusters provide information on the semantic status of the utterance (sentence type, tense and modality, and negation, among others). There is evidence that early on, finite verb forms constitute a smaller and functionally different set of verbs: In Dutch, early finite verbs are mostly (modal) auxiliaries or static verbs like the copula be and the verb to have (de Haan, Reference de Haan, Haan and Zonneveld1987). Schimke and Dimroth (Reference Schimke and Dimroth2018) found similar effects of verb semantics in German: semantically light like make, do, come, go verbs occur with finiteness marking, full lexical verbs in non-finite forms. This corresponds to recent findings from spoken adult corpora in German, Dutch, and English, where finite forms are much more frequent than nonfinite forms, but also lexically restricted to a small set of high-frequency, but semantically light verbs or auxiliaries. Kempen and Harbusch (Reference Kempen and Harbusch2019) argue that the predominance of only a few high-frequency verbs early in the utterance facilitates processing. When looking at their list of the 50 most frequent German finite verbs in written corpora, it seems that many of them are not typical in CDS such that the set of high-frequency finite verbs addressed to children should be even smaller. This would also substantially reduce the problem of learning verbal inflection because children only have to deal with a small set of verbs that carry agreement and tense marking. A further facilitating factor could be the frequent use of separable particle verbs like reinmachen – mach X rein ‘put in’ (Behrens, Reference Behrens1998). They contribute massively to the lexical richness, but are based on a smaller number of high-frequency and often semantically light verb roots, which adds further support to the hypothesis that the set of finite verbs is more limited than the set of nonfinite forms found clause-finally.

Most likely, we should see effects of specific inflected word forms, not of the verb lemma in general. In German, many high frequency verbs are semantically light and polysemous. Finite and and non-finite forms not only occur in different positions, but also with different functions (have or go as auxiliary or main verb) or semantics / argument structure. There is evidence that item-effects occur at the level of word forms, not across all different usage-types involving the same verb lemma (Behrens, Reference Behrens, Haberzettl and Wegener2003; Lange, Reference Lange2007; Theakston, Lieven, Pine & Rowland, Reference Theakston, Lieven, Pine and Rowland2002).

In terms of information structure, the onset of utterances provides evidence of the sentence type (question, declarative or imperative), and hosts negation (nicht ‘not’) as well as additive particles like auch ‘too, also’ used to be included in activities (ich will auch spielen ‘I want to play, too’). These particles could pave the way for finite verb forms because the typically occur after the finite verb (das geht nicht/auch rein ‘that goes not/also in’), and encode relevant information regarding the intention of the speaker (negation, addition/inclusion). This allows learners to recognize that relevant types of information regarding speech act type, negation or addition/inclusion are encoded clause-initially. Schimke and Dimroth (Reference Schimke and Dimroth2018) show for that first language learners, these particles operate like a boundary or pivot when they realize that the semantically light and phonologically non-salient finite verb precedes these particles. Hence, finite verbs form local clusters with preceding onset subject (pro)nouns and/or adjacent focus/negation particles (5).

  1. (5) Prominent schemas in the left sentence periphery

    (Pro)noun V-FIN (nicht/auch) declaratives

    V-FIN (pro)noun (nicht/auch) y/n-questions

    Wh-word V-FIN (nicht/auch) wh-questions

Prediction for the acquisition of finite verbforms at the left periphery

Finite verb forms are acquired based on a limited number of local, clause initial clusters. When tested for their knowledge of agreement and tense inflection, children should make fewer errors with high-frequency verb forms attested in this position, than with low-frequency ones. A failure to find such token-frequency effects, but verb-general success in agreement marking would provide counterevidence.

In sum, early German and Dutch child language shows a strong correlation between verb form and position, and an effect of complexity reduction in CDS: There are significant associations between verb position and verb semantics. Modal, auxiliary, and static verbs tend to occur as finite verbs in V1/V2 position, whereas activity and telic verbs tend to occur as infinitives and participles at the high-saliency end of utterances (Wijnen et al., Reference Wijnen, Kempen and Gillis2001). Thus, learners are not confronted with all possible variations in the input, but with a limited set of lexical frames, which facilitates pattern abstraction. Also, children find different types of information at the onset and end of utterances, and this tunes their attention to both the end and the beginning of utterances, and should allow them to make inferences about the pragmatics of information structure. In a series of computational modellings, the acquisition of sentence structures in German, Dutch, Spanish and English could be simulated by installing a limited working memory with an attentional focus to the beginning and/or end of utterances to model primacy and recency effects (Freudenthal, Pine, Jones, and Gobet (Reference Freudenthal, Pine, Jones and Gobet2015). They explain the emerging word order patterns by attention tuning and extending the working memory of the model, which allows the processing of increasingly larger clusters. In the following, I will spell out how this might work for German and Dutch word order.

Becoming productive: Effects of semantics and lexical overlap

Despite the distributional biases discussed above, all German and Dutch verbs can occur in finite or non-finite forms, hence children will have to expand the range of verbs attested in different constructions. And they will have to learn to produce longer sentences with finite and non-finite verbs.

Initially, we do not expect much overlap between verbs occurring in finite form and position and those occurring in non-finite form and position. Then children learn more constructional schemas, and the fact that the same verb can occur in either position, with the appropriate morphological finiteness-marking. Whether and how children build productive associations between the different verb positions should depend on the amount of overlap of the same verb in different tenses, for example present perfect with discontinuous verb position and simple past with fronted position as in (6):

  1. (6) Hast du mir ein Geschenk gebracht? ‘Did you bring me a gift?’

    Bringst du mir ein Geschenk? ‘Will you bring me a gift?’

As in the variation sets described above, the syntactic relationship between the two word order patterns should be clearer when the function is similar. This is particularly clear with separable particle verbs where the semantics is the same in continuous or discontinuous order (7):

  1. (7) Was machst du da rein? Was hast du da reingemacht?

    ‘what do you put in?’ ‘what have you there in-put?’

However, many verbs are highly polysemous, and this can obscure the formal relationships between early utterance schemas such that children proceed with not just lexeme-specific frames, but even wordform-specific frames. The verb to go, for example, is used in different functions that are associated with different inflectional forms (going, gone, goes, went etc.). Distributional analyses suggest that children do not link these different schemas, as would be indicated by flexible variation of word order and inflection, but use them as wordform-specific templates (Behrens, Reference Behrens, Haberzettl and Wegener2003; Theakston et al., Reference Theakston, Lieven, Pine and Rowland2002). Both studies found that different forms of the verb go in English, Dutch and German appear in their own distributional environments with different functions.

Prediction

Learning the association between verb form and position can facilitated by overlap in form, but inhibited by differences in semantics, even at the level of inflectional forms. Separable particle verbs could be path-breaking elements for lexical overlap, as their semantics is stable, and as they are formed with a limited number of high-frequency verb roots. We expect the first verbs to be used in finite and nonfinite forms to be high-frequency, and within the same semantic function. To find a large set of semantically different verbs used in both positions at an early stage of development would provide counterevidence.

Accounting for Agreement Errors with Transitional Probabilities

Transitional probabilities help to explain both correct and incorrect patterns. Poeppel and Wexler (Reference Poeppel and Wexler1993), in a case study of a single transcript of a boy aged 2;1, argued that even at this early age, there were hardly any agreement errors between subject and verb. They consider this as evidence that the finite inflection of the verb licenses subjects. From a constructivist position, the argument could be reversed: A given subject could trigger the corresponding verb form because children build up verb frames (NAME/pronoun V-AGR). If this is the case, we expect lower error rates in well-established units with an overt subject than in clusters where the subject is elided or infrequent.

Subsequent studies reported agreement errors in German and Dutch where a subject is used with a non-finite verb (so-called Optional Infinitive or OI-errors) as in English he go or German Mama machen ‘mummy make-INF’. Whereas the frequency of such OI errors does not depend on the sentence type (declarative or wh-question) in English (OI errors frequent in both constructions) or Spanish (OI errors infrequent in both constructions), they do in Dutch and German: Here, OI-errors are infrequent with WH-questions, and more frequent in declaratives (Freudenthal et al., Reference Freudenthal, Pine, Jones and Gobet2015). This can be explained by the distributional facts imposed by the topological model: In German and Dutch, a wh-element is always followed by a finite verb. This leads to the prediction that children can form frames like wh-word …s for 3rd person singular, based on transitional probabilities. The only source of confusion is that verbs in present tense plural (1st and 3rd person) are homophonous with the infinitive (cf. Wohin gehen sie) ‘where go-3p they?’ and Wohin sollen sie gehen? ‘where shall-INF they go-INF’)

The picture sketched here is that learning “verb-second” in German or Dutch is not a uniform process where inflectional properties license verb movement, as proposed in the generative literature from the 1990ies. Instead, the topological model of sentence structure serves as a stable hyperschema that specifies the word order of different sentence types. Acquisition starts from utterance-initial and -final positions, as evidenced by acquisitional sequences and error patterns that can be predicted from local clusters and their transitional probabilities. The existing literature shows that the development from a mainly non-finite to a finite system takes place in the 3rd year of life, based on abundant evidence sentence in the input. But how does a constructivist account fare for low-frequency target structures?

D. Acquisition of complex structures with limited evidence in the input

The “logical problem” of language acquisition does not exist in a usage-based perspective (Hendriks, Reference Hendriks, Gleitman and Joshi2000; Pullum & Scholz, Reference Pullum and Scholz2002). If there are no innate constraints on possible syntactic structures, there cannot be a mismatch between what the child knows and what s/he can derive from the input. This does not imply that children need to hear all possible sentences or all possible structures of a language before they can process them. Rather, gaps in the evidence must be filled by inferencing, i.e., going beyond the input and generalize over (a combination of) functional and formal aspects relevant for the construction at stake, or indirect positive evidence (Lewis & Elman, Reference Lewis and Elman2001b).

Constructivist approaches propose that learners can induce such complex structures deriving knowledge about grammatical and ungrammatical sequences from transitional probabilities (e.g., Reali & Christiansen, Reference Reali and Christiansen2005), and by schema combinations, i.e., building up the respective knowledge from simpler, related structures (Abbot-Smith & Behrens, Reference Abbot-Smith and Behrens2006; Ambridge, Rowland & Gummery, Reference Ambridge, Rowland and Gummery2020; Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2008, Reference Dąbrowska2015; Lewis & Elman, Reference Lewis and Elman2001a). I will illustrate the sources children can draw on with the acquisition of complex yes-/no-questions in English.

Complex yes-/no-questions

When asking a yes-/no-question with an embedded relative clause, subject-verb inversion is required (8 a,b). In generative grammar, this is a structure dependent operation that respects the syntactic hierarchy by fronting the auxiliary of the main clause, leaving the embedded clause (underlined) unaffected. Errors as in (8.c) can supposedly only be prevented if the speaker is aware of the structure dependence.

  1. (8) a. The boy who is smoking is crazy.

    b. Is the boy who is smoking crazy?

    c. Is the boy who smoking is crazy?

The hypothesis that children should commit errors as in (8.c) is based on the assumption that learners process the structure in terms of abstract categories (i.e., processes the two occurrences of is as AUX), and apply a movement rule, but erroneously move the first auxiliary if they are not aware of the syntactic embeddedness of the subject relative. None of these assumptions is made by constructivism. Instead, a much simpler analysis of the necessary operation is provided: The familiar NP the boy is elaborated with a relative clause, and this forms the syntactic and semantic unit The boy who is smoking. The syntactic process required to produce (8.b) boils down to slotting the complex NP into the open slot of the yes/no-question construction that already has the correct word order (Is X crazy?), and does not require movement (Ambridge et al., Reference Ambridge, Rowland and Gummery2020; Ambridge, Rowland & Pine, Reference Ambridge, Rowland and Pine2008; Dąbrowska, Reference Dąbrowska2008, Reference Dąbrowska, Schmid and Handl2010). In sum, the knowledge of the yes/no-question construction plus the knowledge of relative clause structures should suffice to process these sentences even if their combination is very rare or non-existent in the input.

Transitional probabilities (n-gram statistics).

Reali and Christiansen (Reference Reali and Christiansen2005) provide a more elaborate discussion of what can count as “relevant data” for the acquisition of such constructions, and provide statistical evidence from actual input data from the CHILDES corpus. They used simple bigram- and trigram-statistics (two-word and three-word sequences) for transitional probabilities between words and as evidence for chunks. Note that, in (8.c), the incorrect auxiliary extraction would lead to the non-attested string who smoking. Statistical analyses of the input and the child data showed that statistical learning based on bigrams and trigrams successfully predicted grammatical and ungrammatical auxiliary extraction. In a further network simulation using the same data, Reali and Christiansen (Reference Reali and Christiansen2005) showed how inferences drawn from simple transitional probabilities could additionally lead to part-of-speech-category induction by means of multiple cue integration. However, subsequent research showed that the statistical predictions derived from bigrams alone are not as reliable when other inversion structures, or structures involving a different or wider range of verbs and tenses were analyzed. In addition, the results – typically based on small corpora – differed considerably depending on which corpus was used and what analyses were carried out, raising the question of what is a suitable reference corpus for input studies (see Fitz & Chang, (Reference Fitz and Chang2017) for a review of the critical issues, and additional cues by the inclusion of semantic relations). While transitional probabilities alone might not suffice to account for complex constructions, schema combination-accounts promise to be more successful, because they also preserve the knowledge about the form-function correspondences of the component structures.

Schema combination (Induction from precursor structures).

Lewis and Elman (Reference Lewis and Elman2001b) trained a Simple Recurrent Network to infer such complex yes/no-questions as in (8) from simpler constructions that provided the system with all necessary grammatical information (agreement, yes/no-questions in simple sentences, complex sentences with embedded relative clauses).

In a similar vein, Ambridge, et al. (Reference Ambridge, Rowland and Gummery2020) conducted a training study to test whether children can learn the structure in (9.c) when trained on the component schemas (9.a and 9.b), which are attested in the input to children:

  1. (9) a. Is [THING] [ACTION]ing? e.g., Is the bird cleaning?

    b. The [THING] who's [PROPERTY] e.g., The bird who is sad.

    c. Is [the [THING] who's [PROPERTY]] ACTIONing? e.g., Is the bird who is sad cleaning?

The study with 4- to 6-year-old children was successful in that children in the experimental group where significantly better in producing structures with the trained relative pronoun who. However, children in both the experimental and the control group (which was trained with simple NPs like the sag bird instead of (9.b) frequently produced relative clauses with that, the more colloquial option (the bird that is sad). When these were included as correct instances of the target structure, the advantage of the experimental group disappeared as even the children in the control group produced complex questions with embeddings. This suggests a contribution of lexical-specificity and structural priming: In this age range, most children are able to produce questions with embedded clauses with that, and – upon training – also with lower-frequency who.

Prediction

Complex constructions with low input frequency can be acquired on the basis of indirect positive evidence derived from their component parts. Transitional probabilities provide evidence for likely or unlikely combinations, and schema combination simplifies the task. For example, Abbot-Smith and Behrens (Reference Abbot-Smith and Behrens2006) found different acquisition rates for German werden- and sein-passives (werden/sein + past participle). Both are equally complex and frequent in the input, but the sein-passive profited from the previous acquisition of its component parts (past participles and the verb sein used as copula and auxiliary). They also found that future constructions (auxiliary werden + infinitive) were learned later than werden-passives despite similar input frequency. Here, one would hypothesize to find fewer inflectional and lexical constraints than in the earlier-learned constructions, because children should be able to transfer their knowledge from already acquired related construction. To find productive complex constructions in child language without assessing the simpler constructions first would provide counterevidence for this constructivist account.

Back to theory: The nature of linguistic processing and representation: shallow or hierarchical?

Constructivist approaches question the underlying motivations of structuralist and generativist linguistics, that linguistic generalizations have to be as powerful as possible. If children learn from local clusters, and manage to generalize on the basis of quite concrete form-function associations, are “abstract” generalizations needed and attested at all, and what is their added value (see Langacker's rule-list fallacy above)? In other words: Are the generalizations that linguists derive the same as those made by speakers, and is processing always based on the most abstract available representation?

Exemplar-storage and low-level analogies

Within usage-based approaches, the general idea is that initial generalizations are “concrete” or item-based (i.e., chunks or n-grams) and turn more “abstract” later (from slot & frame-patterns with some variability to more productive semantically-based constructions (agent/patient) and finally to rules based on grammatical categories (subject, object, accusative, dative etc.). Ambridge (Reference Ambridge2019) criticizes usage-based accounts for accepting the view that the endstate of language development are abstract rules, and the implication that the nature of processing changes: after certain (to be defined) productivity thresholds, the mind would start to translate the words into more abstract categories – from kisser to agent to subject. But processing studies with children and adults, as well as computational models of early language acquisition suggest that processing based on concrete units of language (words and n-grams or multiword strings/chunks) is successful, and often superior to rule-based accounts. Such exemplar-effects last into adulthood. In a literature review, McCauley and Christiansen (Reference McCauley and Christiansen2019) show that entrenched multiword units are processed faster in comprehension and production. They argue that multiword units “retain their significance throughout development” (McCauley & Christiansen, Reference McCauley and Christiansen2019, p. 3). So-called “shallow” or non-hierarchical, underspecified processing in adults such suggests that there is more continuity between child and adult processing than previously assumed (McCauley & Christiansen, Reference McCauley and Christiansen2019, p. 4).

In a similar vein, Ambridge (Reference Ambridge2019) argues in favor of radical exemplar-based processing because the minimal variation of stored exemplars based on analogy bypasses a number of problems of accounts that assume processing by rule. If low-level generalization were stored schemas, they could be retrieved and used with minimal adaptations in similar communicative contexts. The basic argument (“lumping and splitting”) is that the more general an abstraction, the more heterogeneous cases it subsumes and the lower its predictiveness (see the low predictiveness / high heterogeneity of hyperschemas discussed above) such that the general categories would have to be re-represented as different subtypes. The radical versions of exemplar- and usage-based model seem controversial because the essence of learning is making predictions based on generalizations (Ambridge, Reference Ambridge2020). But it makes developmental sense if one starts out with the assumption that the child has to build-up experience first. Exemplar-based abstraction leads to precise generalizations based on similarity because one has to learn the details before one can lump. At start, it does not matter whether a certain construction is generalizable or not because the child does not have a baseline for comparisons yet.

Linguistic theory

Exemplar-processing has informed constructivist linguistic theory regarding language typology, and historical and developmental language change. Croft (Reference Croft2001) and Diessel (Reference Diessel2019) argue that there are no universal categories, and that the scope of possible generalizations is typically quite limited if one looks at the totality of constructions in a language. Diessel (Reference Diessel2019) integrates findings from language change and processing to identify the forces that lead to grammatical relations. He argues that linguistic relation can be accounted for in terms of symbolic relations (form-function associations), sequential relations (that also lead to categorization or unit-formation), and taxonomic relations (hierarchical inheritance links between constructions). Taxonomic relations capture the similarities and specifics between different genitive constructions, for example.

Schmid's entrenchment and conventionalization model (Schmid, Reference Schmid2015, Reference Schmid2020) adds a social dimension that promotes and constrains generalization. While entrenchment happens in the individual mind, conventionalization requires convergent generalizations between speakers. Currently, there is little research on the social mechanisms that drive language learning, be it in terms of inferencing the interlocutor's intentions (Matthews, Reference Matthews2014) or in making use of discourse-level information (Casillas & Frank, Reference Casillas and Frank2017). Many arguments for constructivist accounts are based on large databases of naturalistic interactions. A closer look at the concrete usage-situations will help us to refine the effect of distributional information by looking at the pragmatic and functional properties in more detail.

References

Abbot-Smith, K., & Behrens, H. (2006). How known construction influence the acquisition of other constructions: the German periphrastic passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science, 30, 9951026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Exemplar versus prototype models in syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review, 23, 275290.Google Scholar
Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child Language, 26(2), 339356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, S. (2017). Polysynthesis in the acquisition of Inuit languages. In Fortescue, M., Mithun, M., & Evans, N. (Eds.), Handbook of Polysynthesis (pp. 449472). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Allen, S., & Behrens, H. (2019). Insights into understanding human language from children's acquisition of morphology and syntax: A historical and current perspective on central questions in the field. In Hagoort, P. (Ed.), Human Language: From Genes and Brains to Behavior (pp. 127145). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ambridge, B. (2019). Against stored abstractions: A radical exemplar model of language acquisition. First Language. doi:10.1177/0142723719869731Google Scholar
Ambridge, B. (2020). Abstractions made of exemplars or ‘You're all right, and I've changed my mind’: Response to commentators. First Language, 40(5–6), 640659. doi:10.1177/0142723720949723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., & Lieven, E. (2011). Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., Rowland, C. F., & Gummery, A. (2020). Teaching the unlearnable: A training study of complex yes/no questions. Language and Cognition, 12(2), 385410. doi:10.1017/langcog.2020.5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., Rowland, C. F., & Pine, J. M. (2008). Is structure dependence an innate constraint? New experimental evidence from children's complex-question production. Cognitive Science, 32, 222257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation and language learning. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 157193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984). A crosslinguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child Development, 55, 341354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, H. (1993). Temporal reference in German child language: form and function of early verb use. Universiteit van Amsterdam: Doctoral dissertation, Instituut voor Algemene Taalwetenschap.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. (1998). How difficult are complex verbs: evidence from German, English and Dutch. Linguistics (Special Issue: Developing a verb category: crosslinguistic perspectives), 36, 679712.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. (2003). Bedeutungserwerb, Grammatikalisierung und Polysemie: Zum Erwerb von “gehen” im Deutschen, Niederländischen und Englischen. In Haberzettl, S. & Wegener, H. (Eds.), Spracherwerb und Konzeptualisierung (pp. 161181). Frankfurt/M.: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. (2006). The input-output relationship in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(1–3), 224. doi:10.1080/01690960400001721CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blom, E. (2007). From Root Infinitive to Finite Sentence: The Acquisition of Verbal Inflections and Auxiliaries. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Boas, H. C., & Ziem, A. (2018). Approaching German syntax from a constructionist perspective. In Boas, H. C. & Ziem, A. (Eds.), Constructional Approaches to Syntactic Structures in German (pp. 181203). Berlin/Boston: De GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language, 75, 720738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Children's talk: learning to use language. New York: Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27(6), 843873. doi:10.1016/J.Cogsci2003.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M. & Frank, M. C. (2017). The development of children's ability to track and predict turn structure in conversation. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 234253. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2016.06.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). Creating language: Integrating evolution, acquisition, and processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Nikitina, T. N. (2009). One vs. more than one: Antecedents to plurality in early language acquisition. Linguistics, 47(1), 103139. doi:10.1515/LING.2009.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. (2001). Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic Theory in Typological Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2008). Questions with long-distance dependencies: A usage-based perspective. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(3). doi:10.1515/cogl.2008.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2010). The mean lean grammar machine meets the human mind: Empirical investigations of the mental status of linguistic rules. In Schmid, H.-J. & Handl, S. (Eds.), Cognitive Foundations of Linguistic Usage Patterns: Empirical Studies (pp. 149170).Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2012). Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(3), 219253. doi:10.1075/lab.2.3.01dabCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2015). What exactly is Universal Grammar, and has anyone seen it? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 852. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2020). Language as a phenomenon of the third kind. Cognitive Linguistics, 31(2), 213229. doi:doi.org/10.1515/cog-2019-0029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Haan, G. J. (1987). A theory-bound approach to the acquisition of verb placement. In Haan, G. D. & Zonneveld, W. (Eds.), Formal Parameters of Generative Grammar (OTS Yearbook III) (pp. 1530). Dordrecht: ICG.Google Scholar
Diessel, H. (1997). Verb-first constructions in German. In Verspoor, M., Dong, L. K., & Sweetser, E. (Eds.), Lexical and Syntactical Constructions and the Construction of Meaning (pp. 5168). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H. (2019). The Grammar Network. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2006a). Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 124. doi:10.1093/applin/ami038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2006b). Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(2), 164194. doi:doi:10.1093/applin/aml015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1988). The mechanisms of Construction Grammar. BLS, 14, 3555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitz, H., & Chang, F. (2017). Meaningful questions: The acquisition of auxiliary inversion in a connectionist model of sentence production. Cognition, 166, 225250. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Aguado-Orea, J., & Gobet, F. (2007). Modelling the developmental patterning of finiteness marking in English, Dutch, German and Spanish using MOSAIC. Cognitive Science, 31, 311341.Google Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., & Gobet, F. (2009). Simulating the referential properties of Dutch, German, and English Root Infinitives in MOSAIC. Language learning and development, 5(1), 129. doi:10.1080/15475440802502437Google Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Jones, G., & Gobet, F. (2015). Simulating the cross-linguistic pattern of Optional Infinitive errors in children's declaratives and Wh- questions. Cognition, 143, 6176. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.027Google ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E., & Casenhiser, D. M. (2005). Fast mapping between a phrasal form and meaning. Developmental Science, 8, 500508.Google Scholar
Greenhill, S. J., Wu, C. H., Hua, X., Dunn, M., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2017). Evolutionary dynamics of language systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 114(42), E8822E8829. doi:10.1073/pnas.1700388114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendriks, P. (2000). The problem with logic in the logical problem of language acquisition. In Gleitman, L. R. & Joshi, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 220225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hilpert, M. (2019). Construction Grammar and its Application to English (2). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Hoekstra, T., & Hyams, N. (1998). Aspects of root infinitives. Lingua, 106, 81112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, P. (2013). The scope of usage-based theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 255. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00255CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ibbotson, P. (2020). What it Takes to Talk. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordens, P. (1988). The aquisition of word order in L2 Dutch and German. In Jordens, P. & Lalleman, J. (Eds.), Language development. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordens, P. (2002). Finiteness in early child Dutch. Linguistics, 40, 687765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempen, G., & Harbusch, K. (2019). Mutual attraction between high-frequency verbs and clause types with finite verbs in early positions: corpus evidence from spoken English, Dutch, and German. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(9), 11401151. doi:10.1080/23273798.2019.1642498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., & Donnelly, S. (2020). Individual differences in first language acquisition. Annual Review of Linguistics, 6:8.18.22. doi:doi.org/annurev-linguistics-011619-030326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Donnelly, S., & Christiansen, M. H. (2018). Individual differences in language acquisition and processing. Trends Cogn Sci, 22(2), 154169. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, S. (2017). Culture and biology in the origins of linguistic structure. Psychon Bull Rev, 24(1), 118137. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1166-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirov, C., & Cotterell, R. (2018). Recurrent neural networks in linguistic theory: Revisiting Pinker and Prince (1988) and the past tense debate. Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics (6), 661665.Google Scholar
Küntay, A., & Slobin, D. I. (1996). Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Guo, J., & Kyratzis, A. (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 265286). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2000). A dynamic usage-based model. In Barlow, M. & Kemmer, S. (Eds.), Usage-based Models of Language (pp. 163). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Lange, B. (2007). machen, haben, gehen, kommen. Einige “Passepartout”-Verben im Primärspracherwerb des Deutschen. (Vol. 45). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lasser, I. (1997). The interpretation of root infinitive constructions in adult and child German. In Schaeffer, J. C. (Ed.), The interpretation of root infinitives and bare nouns in child language (=MIT Working Papers in Linguistics # 12). Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
Lasser, I. (2002). The roots of root infinitives: Remarks on infinitival main clauses in adult and child language. Linguistics, 40, 767796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, J. D., & Elman, J. L. (2001a). A connectionist investigation of linguistic arguments from the poverty of the stimulus: learning the unlearnable. Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 552-557.Google Scholar
Lewis, J. D., & Elman, J. L. (2001b). Learnability and the statistical structure of language: Poverty of stimulus arguments revisited. Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 1, 359370.Google Scholar
Matthews, D. (Ed.) (2014). Pragmatic development in first language acquisition. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2019). Language learning as language use: A cross-linguistic model of child language development. Psychol Rev, 126(1), 151. doi:10.1037/rev0000126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mintz, T. H. (2003). Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition, 90(1), 91117. doi:10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00140-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, S., Blasi, D. E., Schikowski, R., Kuntay, A. C., Pfeiler, B., Allen, S., & Stoll, S. (2018). A universal cue for grammatical categories in the input to children: Frequent frames. Cognition, 175, 131140. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York, NY and London, UK: William Morrow & Co (New York), Penguin (London).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1991). Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. BLS, 17, 230251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poeppel, D., & Wexler, K. (1993). The Full Competence Hypothesis of clause structure in early German. Language, 69, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pullum, G. K., & Scholz, B. C. (2002). Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review, 19, 950.Google Scholar
Ravid, D., Dressler, W. U., Nir-Sagiv, B., Korecky-Kröll, K., Souman, A., Rehfeldt, K., . . . Gillis, S. (2008). Core morphology in child directed speech: Crosslinguistic corpus analyses of noun plurals. In Behrens, H. (Ed.), Corpora in language acquisition research: Finding structure in data (pp. 2560). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D., Keuleers, E., & Dressler, W. U. (2020). Emergence and early development of lexicon and morphology. In Pirrelli, V., Plag, I., & Dressler, W. U. (Eds.), Word Knowledge and Word Usage (pp. 593633). Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Reali, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). Uncovering the richness of the stimulus: Structure dependence and indirect statistical evidence. Cognitive Science, 29, 10071028. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Learning the past tense of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel processing? In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 195248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schimke, S., & Dimroth, C. (2018). The influence of finiteness and lightness on verb placement in L2 German: Comparing child and adult learners. Second Language Research, 34(2), 229256. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658317723071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, H.-J. (2015). A blueprint of the Entrenchment-and- Conventionalization Model. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association (CGLA), 3(1), 325. doi:10.1515/gcla-2015-0002Google Scholar
Schmid, H.-J. (2020). The Dynamics of the Linguistic System: Usage, Conventionalization, and Entrenchment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 11571249). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1997). The origins of grammaticizable notions: beyond the individual mind. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 5: Expanding the contexts (pp. 265323). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Szagun, G. (2011). Regular/irregular is not the whole story: The role of frequency and generalization in the acquisition of German past participle inflection. Journal of Child Language, 38(4), 731762. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szagun, G., Stumper, B., Sondag, N., & Franik, M. (2007). The acquisition of gender marking by young German-speaking children: Evidence for learning guided by phonological regularities. Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 445471. doi:dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000906007951CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theakston, A. L., & Lieven, E. (2017). Multiunit Sequences in First Language Acquisition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9(3), 588603. doi:10.1111/tops.12268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2002). Going, going, gone: The acquisition of the verb “go”. Journal of Child Language, 29, 783811. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090200538XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A. case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1998). The return of constructions. Journal of Child Language, 25, 431442. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 6182.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based account of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Brooks, P. (1998). Young children's earliest transitive and intransitive constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9(4), 379395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twomey, K. E., & Westermann, G. (2019). Building the foundations of language: Mechanisms of curiosity-driven learning. In Horst, J. & Torkildsen, J. von Koss (Eds.), International handbook of language acquisition (pp. 102114). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welke, K. (2019). Konstruktionsgrammatik des Deutschen. Ein sprachgebrauchsbezogener Ansatz. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, M. (2009). The acquisition of word order: Micro-cues, information structure, and economy. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wijnen, F., Kempen, M., & Gillis, S. (2001). Root infinitives in early Dutch child language: An effect of input? Journal of Child Language, 28(3), 629660. doi:10.1017/S0305000901004809CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wöllstein-Leisten, A., Hilmann, A., Stephan, P., & Vikner, S. (1997). Deutsche Satzstruktur: Grundlagen der syntaktischen Analyse. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Xanthos, A., Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Stephany, U., Aksu-Koç, A., Christofidou, A., . . . Dressler, W. U. (2011). On the role of morphological richness in the early development of noun and verb inflection. First Language, 31(4), 461479. doi:10.1177/0142723711409976CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C., Crain, S., Berwick, R. C., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2017). The growth of language: Universal Grammar, experience and principles of computation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed