Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-28T01:47:22.627Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Consistency in the pattern of change in mothers' speech: some discriminant analyses*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

David Bellinger
Affiliation:
Children's Hospital Medical Center

Abstract

This study demonstrates a striking absence of variability in the speech which mothers address to children of the same age within the 1- to 5-year range. Speech to children of 1; 0, 1; 8, 2; 3 and 5; 0 years was analysed for aspects of syntactic, semantic, pragmatic and discourse structure. Discriminant function analyses showed that the age of the child to whom a mother was speaking could be predicted very accurately from her speech. The rate at which mothers' speech changed was greatest when the children were 1; 8–2; 3, and least when they were 2; 3–5; 0. These trends in the evolution of the maternal speech register are interpreted as responses to concurrent changes in children's language behaviour.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baldwin, A. & Baldwin, C. (1973). The study of mother–child interaction. AmSci 61. 714–21.Google Scholar
Bartak, L., Rutter, M. & Cox, A. (1977). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorders. Ill: discriminant function analysis. JAutChildhoodSchiz 7. 383–96.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers' directives as children age. JChLang 6. 443–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Blount, B. (1972). Parental speech and language acquisition: some Luo and Samoan examples. AnL 14. 119–30.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. (1978). The feedback model of speech to children in a nonfeedback situation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Atlanta, May, 1978.Google Scholar
Broen, P. (1972). The verbal environment of the language-learning child. ASHA Monogr. 17.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buium, N., Rynders, J. & Turnure, J. (1973). Early maternal linguistic environment of normal and non-normal language-learning children. Proceedings of the 81st Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 79–80.Google Scholar
Carpenter, W., Strauss, J. & Bartko, J. (1973). Flxexible system for the diagnosis of schizophrenia: report from the WHO International Pilot Study of Schizophrenia. Science 182. 1275–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooley, W. & Lohnes, P. (1971). Multivariate data analysis. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cramblitt, N. & Siegel, G. (1977). The verbal environment of a language-impaired child. JSHD 42. 474–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, T. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: the contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Darlington, R. (1968). Multiple regression in psychological research and practice. Psych-Bull 69. 161–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DePaulo, B. & Bonvillian, J. (1978). The effect on language development of the special characteristics of speech addressed to children. JPsycholingRes 7. 189211.Google Scholar
Dolezel, L. & Bailey, R. (1969). Statistics and style. New York: American Elsevier.Google Scholar
Dutton, J. & Gleitman, L. (1979). Language development in hearing-impaired children: some effects of maternal speech style. Paper presented at the fourth annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Wait for me, roller skate. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1977). Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Fraser, C. & Roberts, N. (1975). Mothers' speech to children of four different ages. JPsycholingRes 4. 916.Google ScholarPubMed
Gelman, R. & Shatz, M. (1977). Appropriate speech adjustments: the operation of conversational constraints on talk to 2-year-olds. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Glanzer, D. & Dodd, D. (1975). Developmental changes in the language spoken to children. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. Berko (1973). Code-switching in children's language. In Moore, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. Berko (1977). Talking to children: some notes on feedback. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. Berko & Weintraub, S. (1978). Input language and the acquisition of communicative competence. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Children's language. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Herdan, G. (1964). Quantitative linguistics. Washington: Butterworth.Google Scholar
Longhurst, T. & Stepanich, L. (1975). Mothers' speech addressed to one-, two-, and three-year-old normal children. ChStudyJ 5. 311.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Devel. No. 149.Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1976). Motherese: the speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, J., Pisoni, D. & Potts, G. (eds), Cognitive theory, Vol. 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Phillips, J. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. ChDev 44. 182–5.Google Scholar
Sachs, J., Brown, R. & Salerno, R. (1976). Adults' speech to children. In von Raffler-Engel, W. & Lebrun, Y. (eds), Baby talk and infant speech. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Schiff, N. (1979). The effect of deviant maternal input on the development of language during the preschool years. JSHR 22. 581603.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1977). Mothers' speech research: an overview. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Vorster, J. (1975). Mommy linguist: the case for motherese. Lingua 37. 281312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wulbert, M., Inglis, S., Kriegsmann, E. & Mills, B. (1975). Language delay and associated mother–child interaction. DevPsych 11. 6170.Google Scholar