Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T02:00:43.668Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Basic level categorization in seven- and nine-month-old infants*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Kenneth Roberts
Affiliation:
Kent State University
Frances Degen Horowitz
Affiliation:
University of Kansas

Abstract

The current reliance on linguistic data for inferences about the concept–word relationship has led to serious questions being raised regarding the existence and facilitative role of prelinguistic categories in early word learning. In three experiments a multiple habituation paradigm was used to examine the ability of 7- and 9-month-old prelinguistic infants to form a natural, basic-level object category. In Experiment 1, 9-month-old infants appeared to form a category of bird, but only when habituated to prototypical exemplars as opposed to poor exemplars. Discriminability between prototypical habituation exemplars and test stimuli was demonstrated in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, 7-month-olds did not form a category of bird when habituated to prototypes. These findings constitute independent evidence for the existence of a linguistically relevant nonlinguistic category prior to the onset of word comprehension. Moreover, formation of prelinguistic categories appears to be facilitated by exposure to prototypes. Such goodness-of-exemplar effects suggest a structural similarity between infants' prelinguistic categories and early semantic categories. Finally, nonlinguistic categorization of natural basic-level objects, as represented in line drawings, appears to emerge between 0;7 and 0;9.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. JChLang 6. 183200.Google ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. (1975). Qualities of color vision in infancy. JExpChPsych 19. 401–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. (1976). Infants are trichromats. JExpChPsych 21. 425–45.Google ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. (1978). Chromatic vision in infancy. In Reese, H. W. & Lipsitt, L. P. (eds), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 21. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M., Kessen, W. & Weiskopf, S. (1976). Color vision and hue categorization in young human infants. JExpPsych: (HumPerceptPerfor) 2. 115–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowerman, M. (1976). Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In Morehead, D. & Morehead, A.. (eds), Normal and deficient child language. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1978). Semantic and syntactic development: a review of what, when and how in language acquisition. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. (ed.), Bases of language intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1980). The structure and origin of semantic categories in the language learning child. In Foster, M. L. & Brandes, S.. (eds), Symbol as sense. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1981). Beyond communicative adequacy: from piecemeal knowledge to an integrated system in the child's acquisition of language. Papers and Reports in Child Language Development 20. 124.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1958). How shall a thing be called? PsychRev 65. 1421.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1976). Reference: in memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg. Cognition 4. 125–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J., Goodnow, J. & Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruning, J. L. & Kintz, B. L. (1977). Computational handbook of statistics, second edition. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Caron, R. F., Caron, A. J. & Meyers, R. S. (1982). Abstraction of invariant facial expressions in infancy. ChDev 53. 1008–15.Google ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E., (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1974). Some aspects of the conceptual basis for first language acquisition. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Language perspectives: acquisition, retardation, and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1975). Knowledge, context and strategy in the acquisition of word meaning. In Dato, D.. (ed.), Proceedings of the 26th annual Georgetown University Roundtable: developmental psycholinguistics: theory and applications. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1983). Meaning and concepts. In Mussen, P.. (ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 3. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cohen, L & Strauss, M. (1979). Concept acquisition in the human infant. ChDev 50. 419–24.Google ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, R. (1978). Language development as related to stage 6 object permanence development. JChLang 5. 173–89.Google Scholar
Dayton, C. M. (1970). The design of educational experiments. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). An ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. & Kerr, J. L. (1978). Infants' perception of semantically defined action role changes in filmed events. MPQ 24. 5362.Google Scholar
Horowitz, F., Paden, L., Bhana, K. & Self, P. (1972). An infant control procedure for studying infant visual fixations. DevPsych 7. 90.Google Scholar
Hupp, S. & Mervis, C. (1982). Development of generalized concepts by severely handicapped children. ChDev 53. 760–7.Google Scholar
Husaim, J. S. & Cohen, L. (1981). Infant learning of ill-defined categories MPQ 27. 443–56.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. & Pani, J. (1980). Acquisition of basic object categories. CogPsych 12. 496522.Google ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. & Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. AnnRevPsych 32. 89115.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. MonogSocResChDev 38. No. 149.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word and sentence: interrelations in acquisition and development. PsychRev 81. 267–85.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1977). The conceptual basis for naming. In Macnamara, J.. (ed.), Language learning and thought. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1980). Overextension in early language development. JChLang 7. 321–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. (1980). Cognition to language: categories, word meanings, and training. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, K. & Schaefer, R. (1984). Cognitive abilities and infant language intervention. In Ruder, K. & Smith, M. D. (eds), Developmental language intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representation of semantic categories. JExpPsych (Gen) 104. 192233.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B.. (eds), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ross, G. (1980). Categorization in 1- to 2-year olds. DevPsych 16. 391–6.Google Scholar
Ruff, H. (1978). Infant recognition of the invariant form of objects. ChDev 49. 293306.Google ScholarPubMed
Ruff, H. (1980). The development of perception and recognition of objects. ChDev 51. 981–92.Google ScholarPubMed
Schlesinger, I. M. (1974). Relational concepts underlying language. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Language perspectives: acquisition, retardation, and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1977). The role of cognitive development and linguistic input in language acquisition. JChLang 4. 153–69.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1982). Steps to language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sherman, T. (1981). Categorization skills in infants. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Boston,Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioural sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies in child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smolak, L. (1982). Cognitive precursors of receptive vs. expressive language. JChLang 9. 1322.Google ScholarPubMed
Snyder, L., Bates, E. & Bretherton, I. (1981). Content and context in early lexical development. JChLang 8. 565–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Strauss, M. (1979). Abstraction of prototypical information by adults and 10-month-olds. JExpPsych: HumLearnMem 5. 618–32.Google Scholar
Strauss, M. (1981). Memory for prototypical information. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Boston,Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Thomas, D., Campos, J., Shucard, D., Ramsay, D. & Shucard, J. (1981). Semantic comprehension in infancy: A signal detection analysis. ChDev 52. 798803.Google ScholarPubMed
Uyeda, K. & Mandler, G. (1980). Prototypicality norms for 28 semantic categories. Behaviour Research Methods and Instrumentation 12. 587–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Younger, B. (1984). Category segregation by 10-month-old infants. Paper presented at the international Conference on Infant Studies,New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Younger, B. & Cohen, L. B. (1983). Infant perception of correlations among attributes. ChDev 54. 858–67.Google ScholarPubMed