Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:01:50.262Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Acoustic cues to coda stop voicing contrasts in Australian English-speaking children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2021

Julien MILLASSEAU*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, Australia 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, North Ryde, NSW2109Australia
Ivan YUEN
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, Australia 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, North Ryde, NSW2109Australia
Laurence BRUGGEMAN
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, Australia 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, North Ryde, NSW2109Australia The MARCS Institute & ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Western Sydney University, Australia
Katherine DEMUTH
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, Australia 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, North Ryde, NSW2109Australia
*
*Address for correspondence: Julien Millasseau, Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, North Ryde, NSW2109Australia E-mail julien.millasseau@mq.edu.au

Abstract

While voicing contrasts in word-onset position are acquired relatively early, much less is known about how and when they are acquired in word-coda position, where accurate production of these contrasts is also critical for distinguishing words (e.g., dog vs. dock). This study examined how the acoustic cues to coda voicing contrasts are realized in the speech of 4-year-old Australian English-speaking children. The results showed that children used similar acoustic cues to those of adults, including longer vowel duration and more frequent voice bar for voiced stops, and longer closure and burst durations for voiceless stops along with more frequent irregular pitch periods. This suggests that 4-year-olds have acquired productive use of the acoustic cues to coda voicing contrasts, though implementations are not yet fully adult-like. The findings have implications for understanding the development of phonological contrasts in populations for whom these may be challenging, such as children with hearing loss.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barton, D., & Macken, M. A. (1980). An instrumental analysis of the voicing contrast in word-initial stops in the speech of four-year-old English-speaking children. Language and Speech, 23(2), 159169. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098002300203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baudonck, N., Lierde, K. V., D'haeseleer, E., & Dhooge, I. (2011). A comparison of the perceptual evaluation of speech production between bilaterally implanted children, unilaterally implanted children, children using hearing aids, and normal-hearing children. International Journal of Audiology, 50(12), 912919. https://doi.org/10.3109/14992027.2011.605803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2019). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (6.1.08) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org/Google Scholar
Chodroff, E., & Wilson, C. (2017). Structure in talker-specific phonetic realization: Covariation of stop consonant VOT in American English. Journal of Phonetics, 61, 3047. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, J., Kim, H., Choi, H., & Hasegawa-Johnson, M. (2007). Prosodic effects on acoustic cues to stop voicing and place of articulation: Evidence from Radio News speech. Journal of Phonetics, 35(2), 180209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2006.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, F., & Palethorpe, S. (2007). Australian English. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 37(3), 341350. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025100307003192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, F., and Palethorpe, S. (2011). Timing differences in the VC rhyme of standard Australian English and Lebanese Australian English, in Lee, W. S. & Zee, E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the ICPhS XVIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, pp. 528–531, Hong Kong.Google Scholar
de Jong, K. (2004). Stress, lexical focus, and segmental focus in English: Patterns of variation in vowel duration. Journal of Phonetics, 32(4), 493516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2004.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K., Culbertson, J., & Alter, J. (2006). Word-minimality, epenthesis and coda licensing in the early acquisition of English. Language and Speech, 49(2), 137173. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309060490020201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Docherty, G. J., & Foulkes, P. (1999). Derby and Newcastle: Instrumental phonetics and variationist studies. In Docherty, G. J. & Foulkes, P. (Eds.), Urban voices: Accent studies in the British Isles (pp. 4771). Arnold.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2), 121138. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, K., Weismer, G., Hodge, M., Dinnsen, D. A., & Elbert, M. (1990). Statistical analysis of word-initial /k/ and /t/ produced by normal and phonologically disordered children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 4(4), 327340. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699209008985495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C. A. (1992). Vowel duration and closure duration in voiced and unvoiced stops: There are no contrast effects here. Journal of Phonetics, 20(1), 143165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30244-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, J. R., Moore, C. A., Higashikawa, M., & Steeve, R. W. (2000). The physiologic development of speech motor control: Lip and jaw coordination. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(1), 239255. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4301.239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. R., Moore, C. A., & Reilly, K. J. (2002). The sequential development of jaw and lip control for speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(1), 6679. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/005)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imbrie, A. (2005). Acoustical study of the development of stop consonants in children [Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology]. https://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/33072Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kewley-Port, D., & Preston, M. S. (1974). Early apical stop production: A voice onset time analysis. Journal of Phonetics, 2(3), 195210. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31270-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, C., & Demuth, K. (2006). Accounting for variability in 2-year-olds’ production of coda consonants. Language Learning and Development, 2(2), 97118. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15473341lld0202_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ko, E.-S. (2007). “Acquisition of vowel duration in children speaking American English,” in Proceedings of Interspeech 2007, pp. 1881–1884, Antwerp, Belgium.Google Scholar
Koenig, L. L. (2001). Distributional characteristics of VOT in children's voiceless aspirated stops and interpretation of developmental trends. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(5), 10581068. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/084)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kowal, S., O'Connell, D. C., & Sabin, E. J. (1975). Development of temporal patterning and vocal hesitations in spontaneous narratives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4(3), 195207. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01066926CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, F., Edwards, J., & Beckman, M. E. (2009). Contrast and covert contrast: The phonetic development of voiceless sibilant fricatives in English and Japanese toddlers. Journal of Phonetics, 37(1), 111124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisker, L. (1957). Closure duration and the intervocalic voiced-voiceless distinction in English. Language, 33(1), 42. https://doi.org/10.2307/410949CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowenstein, J. H., & Nittrouer, S. (2008). Patterns of acquisition of native voice onset time in English-learning children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(2), 11801191. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2945118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1985). Contextual effects on vowel duration, closure duration, and the consonant/vowel ratio in speech production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 78(6), 19491957. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.392651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, M. A., & Barton, D. (1980). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7(1), 4174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900007029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markides, A. (1970). The speech of deaf and partially-hearing children with special reference to factors affecting intelligibility. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 5(2), 126140. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682827009011511CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, K., Demuth, K., & Ching, T. (2012). Acoustic analysis of the speech of an Australian English-speaking child with hearing aids. In Cox, F., Demuth, K., Lin, S., Miles, K., Yuen, I., Palethorpe, S., & Shaw, J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th Australasian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology. (pp. 97100). Australian Speech Science and Technology Association.Google Scholar
Munson, B., Edwards, J., & Beckman, M. E. (2005). Phonological knowledge in typical and atypical speech–sound development. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(3), 190206. https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-200507000-00003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nip, I. S. B., & Green, J. R. (2013). Increases in cognitive and linguistic processing primarily account for increases in speaking rate with age. Child Development, 84(4), 13241337. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S. (1993). The emergence of mature gestural patterns Is not uniform: Evidence from an acoustic study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(5), 959972. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.959CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Neely, S. T. (1996). How children learn to organize their speech gestures: Further evidence from fricative-vowel syllables. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(2), 379389. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3902.379CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penney, J., Cox, F., Miles, K., & Palethorpe, S. (2018). Glottalisation as a cue to coda consonant voicing in Australian English. Journal of Phonetics, 66, 161184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raphael, L. J. (1972). Preceding vowel duration as a cue to the perception of the voicing characteristic of word-final consonants in American English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 51(4B), 12961303. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1912974CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Redi, L., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2001). Variation in the realization of glottalization in normal speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 29(4), 407429. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2001.0145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scobbie, J. M. (2006). Flexibility in the face of incompatible English VOT systems. In Goldstein, L., Whalen, D. H. and Best, C. T. (Eds.), Laboratory Phonology Vol. 1–8, pp. 367392. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Scobbie, J. M., Gibbon, F., Hardcastle, W., & Fletcher, P. (2000). Covert contrast as a stage in the acquisition of phonetics and phonology. In Pierrehumbert, J. B. & Broe, M. (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology V: Acquisition and the Lexicon (pp. 194207). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Demuth, K., Hanson, H., & Stevens, K. (2011). Acoustic cues to stop-coda voicing contrasts in the speech of American English 2–3-year-olds. In Clements Nick, G. & Ridouane, R. (Eds.), Where do phonological features come from? Cognitive, physical and developmental bases of distinctive speech categories (pp. 327341). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. L. (1979). A phonetic analysis of consonantal devoicing in children's speech. Journal of Child Language, 6(1), 1928. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900007595CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Song, J. Y., & Demuth, K. (2008). Compensatory vowel lengthening for omitted coda consonants: A phonetic investigation of children's early representations of prosodic words. Language and Speech, 51(4), 385402. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830908099071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, J. Y., Demuth, K., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2012). The development of acoustic cues to coda contrasts in young children learning American English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131(4), 30363050. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3687467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C., & Buder, E. (1999). Vowel length, postvocalic voicing and VOT in the speech of two-year olds. In Elenius, K. & Branderud, P. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 13th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (Vol. 3, pp. 2485–2488).Google Scholar
Stuart-Smith, J., Sonderegger, M., Rathcke, T., & Macdonald, R. (2015). The private life of stops: VOT in a real-time corpus of spontaneous Glaswegian. Laboratory Phonology, 6(3–4). https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2015-0015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theodore, R. M., Demuth, K., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2012). Segmental and positional effects on children's coda production: Comparing evidence from perceptual judgments and acoustic analysis. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(9), 755773. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2012.700680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 11761190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weismer, G., Dinnsen, D., & Elbert, M. (1981). A study of the voicing distinction associated with omitted, word-final stops. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46(3), 320328. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4603.320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu Rattanasone, N., & Demuth, K. (2014). The acquisition of coda consonants by Mandarin early child L2 learners of English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 646659. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, V. Y., De Nil, L. F., & Pang, E. W. (2015). Effects of age, sex and syllable number on voice onset time: Evidence from children's voiceless aspirated stops. Language and Speech, 58(2), 152167. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830914522994CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zlatin, M. A., & Koenigsknecht, R. A. (1976). Development of the voicing contrast: A comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19(1), 93111. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1901.93CrossRefGoogle Scholar