Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T07:21:03.041Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Twin talk: manifestations of twin status in the speech of toddlers*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Patricia M. Malmstrom
Affiliation:
Twin Services Inc
Marilyn N. Silva
Affiliation:
California State University, Hayward

Abstract

With the expectation that an examination of language between toddler twins in their home environment would reveal characteristics unique to the subjects' twin status, 31 hours of crib-talk between identical twin girls and diary records of their speech were examined. It was found that the subjects developed conventional syntax and vocabulary, but adapted them in ways which appropriately expressed their twin status. Three salient examplesinclude their use of a double name for themselves as a team, their use of singular verbs in reference to themselves together, and their use of the singular pronoun me in pronominal reference to themselves as a team.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Arnold, M. R. & Landau, B. S. (1980). The language development of twins. Paper presented at AERA, Boston, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lahey, M., Hood, L., Lifter, K. & Fiess, K. (1980). Complex sentences: acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode. JChLang 7. 235–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Bruggemann, C. E. (1970). Twins: early grammatical development. FolPhoniat 22. 197215.Google ScholarPubMed
Chubrich, R. E. (1971). Selected phonological, morphological, and syntactic measures for identical and non-identical same-sex twins. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo.Google Scholar
Clancy, P., Jacobsen, T. & Silva, M. (1976). The acquisition of conjunction: a cross-linguistic study. PRCLD 12. 7180.Google Scholar
Davis, E. A. (1937). The development of linguistic skill in twins, singletons, and siblings, and only children from five to ten. University of Minnesota Institute of Child Welfare Monograph 14.Google Scholar
Day, E. J. (1932). The development of language in twins: a comparison of twins and single children. ChDev 3. 179–99.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1975). Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. JChLang 2. 2140.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (1980). A syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic analysis of conjunction. PRCLD 19. 70–8.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. M. (1973). A comparison of the similarity in language skills of identical and fraternal twin pairs. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Guardo, C. & Bohan, J. (1971). Development of a sense of self-identity in children. ChDev 42. 1909–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. In Lenneberg, E. H. & Lenneberg, E.(eds), Foundations of language development: a multidisciplinary approach, Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1974). Conversational competence in children. JChLang 1. 163–83.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1975). Evolving discourse – the next step. PRCLD 10. 80–8.Google Scholar
Koch, H. L. (1966). Twins and twin relations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. & Yudovich, F. Ia. (1959). Speech and the development of mental processes in the child. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Lytton, H., Conway, D. & Sauvé, R. (1977). The impact of twinship on parent–child interaction. JPersSocPsych 35. 97107.Google Scholar
Malmstrom, P. M. (1978). Twin talk: an examination of language of identical twins for information about their self-concepts. Unpublished master's thesis, California State University, Sonoma.Google Scholar
Matheny, A. P. & Bruggemann, C. E. (1973). Children's speech: hereditary components and sex differences. FolPhoniat 25. 442–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Mather, P. L. (1979). Factors affecting heritability estimates of preschool-aged twins' language performance skills. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.Google Scholar
Mittler, P. (1971). The study of twins. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Mittler, P. (1976). Language development in young twins: biological, genetic and social aspects. ActaGenetMedGemellol (Roma) 25. 359–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Munsinger, H. & Douglass, A. (1976). The syntactic abilities of identical twins, fraternal twins, and their siblings. ChDev 47. 4050.Google Scholar
Myrianthopoulos, N., Nichols, P. L. & Broman, S. H. (1976). Intellectual development of twins – comparison with singletons. ActaGenetMedGemellol (Roma) 25. 376–80.Google ScholarPubMed
Savić, S. (1979). Mother–child verbal interaction: the functioning of completions in the twin situation. JChLang 6. 153–8.Google Scholar
Savić, S. (1980). How twins learn to talk. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1971). Psycholinguistics. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
Terry, G. C. (1975). The separation–individuation process in same-sex twins: a review of the literature. MaternChNursJ 4. 121–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Waterman, P. & Shatz, M. (1982). The acquisition of personal pronouns and proper names by an identical twin pair. JSHR 25. 149–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S. (1974). Twins: mental development in the preschool years. DevPsych 10. 580–8.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. S. (1975). Twins: patterns of cognitive development as measured on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. DevPsych 11. 126–34.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. S. (1977). Twins and siblings: concordance for school-age mental development. ChDev 48. 211–16.Google Scholar