Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T14:34:27.419Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Close social relationships and loneliness: the role of subjective age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2019

Naama Spitzer*
Affiliation:
Department of Gerontology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Dikla Segel-Karpas
Affiliation:
Department of Gerontology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Yuval Palgi
Affiliation:
Department of Gerontology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Naama Spitzer, Department of Gerontology, University of Haifa, Mt. Carmel, Haifa, Israel. E-mail: Naama_sp@hotmail.com.

Abstract

Loneliness is considered a major issue, often negatively influencing the quality of life of individuals of all ages, and of older adults, in particular. The aims of this study are: (1) to assess the association between close social relationships and loneliness; and (2) to examine the moderating role of subjective age in this association. Married or cohabiting community-dwelling Israelis in the second half of life (N = 360) were interviewed and reported on their close social relationships, their level of loneliness, and their subjective age. The number of close social relationships was found to have a negative relationship with loneliness. Moreover, subjective age was found to moderate the relationship between close social relationships and loneliness, such that the association was weaker for those with older subjective age. Those with older subjective age are often not able to benefit from close social relationships to alleviate loneliness as much as their younger-subjective-age counterparts. Efforts to address older adults’ loneliness should consider focusing on older adults’ perceptions of aging.

Type
Brief Report
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bergman, Y.S. and Segel-Karpas, D. (2018). Future time perspective, loneliness, and depressive symptoms among middle-aged adults: a mediation model. Journal of Affective Disorders, 241, 173175. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, L.L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 331338. doi: 0882-7974/92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen-Mansfield, J., Hazan, H., Lerman, Y. and Shalom, V. (2016). Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(4), 557576. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong Gierveld, J., van Tilburg, T. and Dykstra, P. (2018). Loneliness and social isolation: new ways of theorizing and conducting research. In Vangelisti, A.L. and Perlman, D. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships (pp. 391404), 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/9781316417867.031.Google Scholar
Dykstra, P.A. (2009). Older adult loneliness: myths and realities. European Journal of Ageing, 6(2), 91100. doi: 10.1007/s10433-009-0110-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A.F. and Rockwood, N.J. (2017). Regression-based statistical mediation and moderation analysis in clinical research: observations, recommendations, and implementation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 98, 3957. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.11.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T.B., Baker, M., Harris, T., and Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, M.E., Waite, L.J., Hawkley, L.C., and Cacioppo, J.T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26, 655672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastenbaum, R., Derbin, V., Sabatini, P. and Artt, S. (1972). “The ages of me”: toward personal and interpersonal definitions of functional aging. Aging and Human Development, 3(2), 197211. doi: 10.2190/TUJR-WTXK-866Q-8QU7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotter-Grühn, D., Kornadt, A.E. and Stephan, Y. (2016). Looking beyond chronological age: current knowledge and future directions in the study of subjective age. Gerontology, 62(1), 8693. doi: 10.1159/000438671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masi, C.M., Chen, H.Y., Hawkley, L.C. and Cacioppo, J.T. (2011). A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(3), 219266. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, B.W. (2019). The effects of loneliness and social isolation on cognitive functioning in older adults: a need for nuanced assessments. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(4), 447449. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218001849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuster, T.L., Kessler, R.C. and Aseltine, R. H. (1990). Supportive interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(3), 423438. doi: 10.1007/BF00938116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segel-Karpas, D., Palgi, Y. and Shrira, A. (2017). The reciprocal relationship between depression and physical morbidity: the role of subjective age. Health Psychology, 36(9), 848851. doi: 10.1037/hea0000542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed