Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T02:43:20.212Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Seasonality and symptoms of depression: A systematic review of the literature

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 April 2019

Simon Øverland*
Affiliation:
Division of Mental and Physical Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Bergen, Norway Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
Wojtek Woicik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Royal Infirmary of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
Lindsey Sikora
Affiliation:
Health Sciences Library, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
Kristoffer Whittaker
Affiliation:
The Research Institute, Modum Bad Psychiatric Center, Vikersund, Norway
Hans Heli
Affiliation:
Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Fritjof Stein Skjelkvåle
Affiliation:
Innlandet hospital trust, Norway
Børge Sivertsen
Affiliation:
Division of Mental and Physical Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Bergen, Norway Department of Mental Health, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway Department of Research and Innovation, Helse Fonna HF, Haugesund, Norway
Ian Colman
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology & Public Health, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
*
Author for correspondence: Simon Øverland, E-mail: simon.overland@fhi.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

Lay opinions and published papers alike suggest mood varies with the seasons, commonly framed as higher rates of depression mood in winter. Memory and confirmation bias may have influenced previous studies. We therefore systematically searched for and reviewed studies on the topic, but excluded study designs where explicit referrals to seasonality were included in questions, interviews or data collection.

Methods

Systematic literature search in Cochrane database, DARE, Medline, Embase, PsychINFO and CINAHL, reporting according to the PRISMA framework, and study quality assessment using the Newcastle-Ottawa scale. Two authors independently assessed each study for inclusion and quality assessment. Due to large heterogeneity, we used a descriptive review of the studies.

Results

Among the 41 included studies, there was great heterogeneity in regards to included symptoms and disorder definitions, operationalisation and measurement. We also observed important heterogeneity in how definitions of ‘seasons’ as well as study design, reporting and quality. This heterogeneity precluded meta-analysis and publication bias analysis. Thirteen of the studies suggested more depression in winter. The remaining studies suggested no seasonal pattern, seasonality outside winter, or inconclusive results.

Conclusions

The results of this review suggest that the research field of seasonal variations in mood disorders is fragmented, and important questions remain unanswered. There is some support for seasonal variation in clinical depression, but our results contest a general population shift towards lower mood and more sub-threshold symptoms at regular intervals throughout the year. We suggest future research on this issue should be aware of potential bias by design and take into account other biological and behavioural seasonal changes that may nullify or exacerbate any impact on mood.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Introduction

Depression is common (Waraich et al., Reference Waraich, Goldner, Somers and Hsu2004) with reported 1-year prevalence estimates ranging around 6.6% in the USA (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Koretz, Merikangas, Rush, Walters and Wang2003), 5.5% in Canada (Patten et al., Reference Patten, Williams, Lavorato, Fiest, Bulloch and Wang2015), 7.4% in Finland (Markkula et al., Reference Markkula, Suvisaari, Saarni, Pirkola, Peña, Saarni, Ahola, Mattila, Viertiö and Strehle2015) and is associated with significant disease burden worldwide (Whiteford et al., Reference Whiteford, Ferrari, Degenhardt, Feigin and Vos2015). The causes and mechanisms behind depression are not fully understood but is commonly framed as a complex outcome of genetic, cognitive, behavioural and environmental risk factors operating in concert.

One of the environmental factors that continuously attracts attention from researchers and the public is how seasonal changes affects mood and depressive symptoms. Seasonal variations impact the prevalence and expression of certain diseases, with influenza serving as one example (Weinberger et al., Reference Weinberger, Krause, Molbak, Cliff, Briem, Viboud and Gottfredsson2012). A host of single studies suggest potential risk factors for depression may vary with seasons (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Sack, Gillin, Lewy, Goodwin, Davenport, Mueller, Newsome and Wehr1984; Roecklein and Rohan, Reference Roecklein and Rohan2005). For example, sleep patterns (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Sack, Gillin, Lewy, Goodwin, Davenport, Mueller, Newsome and Wehr1984; Lewy et al., Reference Lewy, Sack, Singer and White1987), levels of physical activity (Shephard and Aoyagi, Reference Shephard and Aoyagi2009), reproductive behaviours (Roenneberg and Aschoff, Reference Roenneberg and Aschoff1990; Bronson, Reference Bronson1995), a host of neurobiological factors (Carlsson et al., Reference Carlsson, Svennerholm and Winblad1980; Kivela et al., Reference Kivela, Kauppila, Ylostalo, Vakkuri and Leppaluoto1988; Avery et al., Reference Avery, Dahl, Savage, Brengelmann, Larsen, Kenny, Eder, Vitiello and Prinz1997; Neumeister et al., Reference Neumeister, Pirker, Willeit, Praschak-Rieder, Asenbaum, Brücke and Kasper2000; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Reid, Kaye, Jennings and Esler2002; Morera and Abreu, Reference Morera and Abreu2006; Kalbitzer et al., Reference Kalbitzer, Erritzoe, Holst, Nielsen, Marner, Lehel, Arentzen, Jernigan and Knudsen2010; Abell et al., Reference Abell, Stalder, Ferrie, Shipley, Kirschbaum, Kivimäki and Kumari2016) are reported to co-vary with seasonal variation and might impact on mood. However, the extent of this impact, and whether or not it translates to functional and clinical significance, remains controversial.

At the individual clinical level, some individuals report seasonal changes in mood that surpass thresholds of clinical significance (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Sack, Gillin, Lewy, Goodwin, Davenport, Mueller, Newsome and Wehr1984; Roecklein and Rohan, Reference Roecklein and Rohan2005). The label ‘seasonal affective disorder’ (SAD) emerged in the early 1980s to capture this phenomenon. Still, neither the ICD nor the DSM diagnostic system includes SAD as a distinct diagnosis. The DSM, since DSM-III-R, has included the possibility to specify if major depression or bipolar disorders occur in a seasonal pattern (Roecklein and Rohan, Reference Roecklein and Rohan2005). In ICD-11, seasonal pattern is now a specifier under mood disorders. The scientific controversy around the concept of SAD remains (Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Skre and Lund2008; Traffanstedt et al., Reference Traffanstedt, Mehta and LoBello2016; Young, Reference Young2017).

Mood is influenced by perceptions and psychosocial factors (Crum and Phillips, Reference Crum and Phillips2015). One study found that more people searched for depression-related terms on Internet-based search engines in winter (Ayers et al., Reference Ayers, Althouse, Allem, Rosenquist and Ford2013). This could be due to more people suffering from depression in winter, but possibly also a stronger focus on depression in media and peers during this time of year. Those processes may also reinforce each other, and an increased societal and media focus could make people attribute ambiguous symptoms to the season and depression during winter. Attribution sets are also likely to influence research on subjects' experience of seasonality and has relevance for the most commonly used measurement of seasonality, the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire (SPAQ). The items in that questionnaire make the intent of measure seasonal variations in mood and behaviour explicit for the respondents. It, therefore, invites a mix of seasonal variation but also reports that reflect subjects' attributions of their symptoms. The questionnaire is criticised for this feature as it might invite memory and confirmation bias (Nayyar and Cochrane, Reference Nayyar and Cochrane1996), and potentially lead to overestimation of seasonal effects. Furthermore, the reliability and validity of the SPAQ have been criticised (Mersch et al., Reference Mersch, Vastenburg, Meesters, Bouhuys, Beersma, van den Hoofdakker and den Boer2004), and it is not considered a valid measurement of depression (Traffanstedt et al., Reference Traffanstedt, Mehta and LoBello2016).

Knowing if, or how, depressive symptoms and mood fluctuate across seasons would contribute to an improved understanding of risk factors, mechanisms and epidemiology of depression. We therefore systematically reviewed the literature to examine if existing evidence supports the assumption of seasonal variation in the prevalence and symptoms of depression. Informed by the potential confirmation bias by self-report, we restricted our search to designs that circumvent this problem and asked, interviewed or collected data from participants without any explicit referral to seasonality as a topic of interest.

Methods

Literature search

We used a broad search strategy and selected the subset of papers on depression and depressive symptoms during the full-text paper review. The following databases were accessed as part of our search strategy: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (via OVID), DARE (Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects via OVID), Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL via OVID), Medline and Medline in Process (via OVID), Embase (via OVID), PsycINFO (via OVID) and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL via EBSCOHost). A search strategy was developed in consultation with a health sciences librarian (author LS) to identify keywords and Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) in Medline, which were then adapted for all other databases (see the Appendix). The search was conducted from the inception of each database to April 2015, with an updated search July 2017. There were no language exclusion criteria and no publication restrictions. All references were entered into Endnote for processing (n = 4393). After removal of duplicates, 2121 papers remained.

Inclusion and exclusion

Papers were included based on the following:

Type of study. General population studies, registrybased studies, experimental studies and self-report studies published in peer-reviewed journals were considered for inclusion. We did not restrict papers on language or date of publication.

Participants. Youth and adults in the general population (i.e. animal studies and studies with children were excluded).

Exposure. Participants or the sample must have been exposed to more than one season individually or as a group.

Comparison. Repeated measurements over a year or enough measurements per month or per season to provide meaningful comparisons. Time-points had to be defined and presented in the paper. In studies where each participant was measured only once, other design features must have been in place to reasonably assume unbiased selection of time of measurement between subjects.

Outcomes. For the broader search, outcomes were defined as depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, symptoms of mental illness, depression, anxiety, mental illness, insomnia, sleep problems, sleep duration and -length, difficulties initiating sleep, suicidal thoughts, suicidal acts, self-harm, suicide, psychiatric hospital admissions. For the purpose of this paper, we focused on depression and depressive symptoms, and hospital admissions and prescriptions related to depression. Most studies on depression prevalence used a screening tool with case identification by the cut-off score. We accepted the authors' approach in these cases and labelled this ‘depression’ despite not having used a diagnostic interview schedule.

Exclusion criteria

We excluded studies where the research hypothesis was available to the participants, or if the research hypothesis or variable measurement overtly related to seasonal variation. Due to these criteria, studies using the SPAQ or similar instruments eliciting the subjective experience of seasonality (Young et al., Reference Young, Hutman, Enggasser and Meesters2015) were excluded.

Procedure

Title and abstract (if available) from the search was listed. The selection procedure (Fig. 1) from the initial papers were done in two rounds. First, two independent evaluators went through the list and excluded papers based on title and abstract, according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Disagreement in this phase led to the paper being included in the next round for full-text evaluation. In the next phase, the remaining papers were collected in full text and split into three separate lists. Two persons appraised each of the papers on the list against inclusion criteria. In case of disagreement, the third of this team of three was consulted to reach consensus. The reasons for disagreement were recorded. From the final set of papers, we selected those that had data on seasonal variation in depression. In July 2017, we updated the search following the same process as outlined for the main search and identified additional studies from other sources (ancestry approach).

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the literature search and study exclusion process.

Study quality

Individual study quality and risk of bias were examined through the use of an adapted version of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Shea, O'connell, Peterson, Welch, Losos and Tugwell2000). NOS is a tool to evaluate non-randomised studies. In its original form, it includes eight items across three dimensions: selection, comparability, and outcomes. Study quality is semi-quantified, with a maximum score of nine ‘stars’. The independent variable of interest in this study (seasons) leaves everyone exposed. There are therefore no non-exposed control groups in the studies. For this reason, we disregarded the second item of the scale (selection of the non-exposed cohort). Furthermore, as we were interested in the variability of depression over time (seasons), we excluded the fourth item of the scale (demonstration that outcome of interest was not present at the start of study) and were left with seven stars as the maximum.

Summary measures

We expected and observed large degrees of heterogeneity in definitions, method of assessment, and summary measures amongst the included studies. Consequently, a meta-analysis of studies was not possible and a descriptive review follows.

Results

Of the initial 2108 papers, 378 remained after title and abstract screening and were examined in full text. For the purpose of this review, a total of 32 papers were first included after exclusion by topic and study design (Fig. 1), one was discarded upon further examination of the full text. Another four papers were added after an updated literature search, and a total of six studies were identified through other papers and included. The final list comprised 41 papers (Table 1). Six and 18 studies got a high-quality rating with full score or only point deducted, respectively, using the adapted Newcastle-Ottawa rating scale (Table 2).

Table 1. Description and main findings of included studies

Table 2. Study quality assessment through an adapted version of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS)

The studies were sorted in five categories defined by study content (Table 2): The first comprised ten studies on prevalence of depression. Six of these were cross-sectional studies with data collections that spanned across seasons, four were cohort studies of which one used a repeated measurement design. Five of the studies (Murase et al., Reference Murase, Murase, Kitabatake, Yamauchi and Mathe1995; Stordal et al., Reference Stordal, Morken, Mykletun, Neckelmann and Dahl2008; Kristjansdottir et al., Reference Kristjansdottir, Olsson, Sundelin and Naessen2013; Cobb et al., Reference Cobb, Coryell, Cavanaugh, Keller, Solomon, Endicott, Potash and Fiedorowicz2014; Patten et al., Reference Patten, Williams, Lavorato, Bulloch, Fiest, Wang and Sajobi2017) observed indications of seasonality with higher prevalence in winter compared to summer. Notably, Patten et al. (Reference Patten, Williams, Lavorato, Bulloch, Fiest, Wang and Sajobi2017) pooled data from ten surveys in Canada where depression was measured through standardised clinical interviews and found higher prevalence rates in the winter months. In Cobb et al. (Reference Cobb, Coryell, Cavanaugh, Keller, Solomon, Endicott, Potash and Fiedorowicz2014), indications of seasonality was found in a post hoc test where winter was defined as lasting from December through April. Huibers et al. (Reference Huibers, de Graaf, Peeters and Arntz2010) found indications of seasonality in depression, but with the highest prevalence in summer and autumn compared to spring. The study by Doganer et al. (Reference Doganer, Angstman, Kaufman and Rohrer2015) primarily focused on 6-month remission rates, but in their clinical sample, a higher rate were diagnosed in spring (26.9%) v. winter (21.5%). Three of the studies (Michalak et al., Reference Michalak, Murray, Wilkinson, Dowrick, Lasa, Lehtinen, Dalgard, Ayuso-Mateos, Vazquez-Barquero, Casey and Group2004; De Graaf et al., Reference de Graaf, van Dorsselaer, ten Have, Schoemaker and Vollebergh2005; Traffanstedt et al., Reference Traffanstedt, Mehta and LoBello2016) found no indications of seasonality.

Nine studies were sorted under depressive symptoms, all based on self-reported symptom levels through the use of questionnaires. Six of the studies used repeated measurement designs while three studies were single cross-sectional surveys spanning a year. In four of them, no indications of seasonality were found (Albin, Reference Albin1982; Magnusson et al., Reference Magnusson, Axelsson, Karlsson and Oskarsson2000; De Craen et al., Reference de Craen, Gussekloo, van der Mast, le Cessie, Lemkes and Westendorp2005; Winthorst et al., Reference Winthorst, Post, Meesters, Penninx and Nolen2011). Park et al. (Reference Park, Kripke and Cole2007) found higher mean scores on CES-D during winter in a subsample, while Harris and Dawson-Hughes (Reference Harris and Dawson-Hughes1993) found higher levels of depressive symptoms in October and November compared to August and September. Schlager et al. (Reference Schlager, Schwartz and Bromet1993) found seasonal variation among women with a variety of symptoms elevated in winter, but no similar variation in men. O'hare et al. (Reference O'Hare, O'Sullivan, Flood and Kenny2016) reported a cohort study in Ireland in which on a single cross-sectional measure, depression scores in autumn and spring only were lower than winter (summer scores were not significantly different). Kerr et al. (Reference Kerr, Shaman, Washburn, Vuchinich, Neppl, Capaldi and Conger2013) followed two independent cohorts from school age into adulthood with 10–19 measurements (8316 person observations). In both samples, they observed a modest increase in depressive symptoms in winter, but no effect on caseness.

Seven studies covered postpartum depression, thus consisting of populations that recently have given birth. The most common design in this group were studies with repeated cross-sectional measurements, and most common symptoms were assessed with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987). In four of these studies, the prevalence of depressive symptoms was higher among mothers who gave birth in winter/autumn (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, Mohan and Davis1993; Sit et al., Reference Sit, Seltman and Wisner2011; Sylven et al., Reference Sylven, Papadopoulos, Olovsson, Ekselius, Poromaa and Skalkidou2011; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Shen, Ping, Wang and Chien2011). Henriksson et al. (Reference Henriksson, Sylven, Kallak, Papadopoulos and Skalkidou2017) reported no overall association in Swedish mothers at one hospital. Jewell et al. (Reference Jewell, Dunn, Bondy and Leiferman2010) used a large sample from the US PRAMS dataset and found no indications of seasonal variation in postpartum depression. In the final study in this group, Weobong et al. (Reference Weobong, Ten Asbroek, Soremekun, Danso, Owusu-Agyei, Prince and Kirkwood2015) found a higher prevalence of depressive symptoms in the drought season compared to the rainy season of Ghana (near the equator).

Two sets of studies focussed on health care use. Three studies used registry data on antidepressant prescriptions. All these three observed seasonal patterns; Balestrieri et al. (Reference Balestrieri, Bragagnoli and Bellantuono1991) found more prescriptions in autumn and spring. Skegg et al. (Reference Skegg, Skegg and McDonald1986) found a higher rate in December and June in men but not women, while the last study by Gardarsdottir et al. (Reference Gardarsdottir, Egberts, van Dijk and Heerdink2010) found more prescriptions in winter. Twelve studies addressed aspects of admissions and care based on individual contact with health services. With the exception of Belleville et al. (Reference Belleville, Foldes-Busque, Dixon, Marquis-Pelletier, Barbeau, Poitras, Chauny, Diodati, Fleet and Marchand2013), all were registry studies. Six of them (Cerbus and Dallara, Reference Cerbus and Dallara1975; Christensen and Dowrick, Reference Christensen and Dowrick1983; Posternak and Zimmerman, Reference Posternak and Zimmerman2002; Belleville et al., Reference Belleville, Foldes-Busque, Dixon, Marquis-Pelletier, Barbeau, Poitras, Chauny, Diodati, Fleet and Marchand2013; Holloway and Evans, Reference Holloway and Evans2014) found no indications of seasonality, including Sato et al. (Reference Sato, Bottlender, Sievers and Moller2006) that found no overall association, but higher rates of prescriptions for major depressive episode in spring among individuals with unipolar depression without depressed mixed states, and in autumn for bipolar and unipolar individuals with depressed mixed states. Szabo and Blanche (Reference Szabo and Blanche1995) found more admissions for mood disorder in winter. The remaining five studies in this group found indications of seasonality, but not in winter (Eastwood and Stiasny, Reference Eastwood and Stiasny1978; Harris, Reference Harris1984; Rollnik et al., Reference Rollnik, Dimsdale and Ng2000; Anastasi et al., Reference Anastasi, Eusebi and Quartesan2014; Dominiak et al., Reference Dominiak, Swiecicki and Rybakowski2015).

Discussion

Main finding

The main purpose of this study was to review the question of seasonality of depression excluding studies with high risk of bias through subjective reporting. Of 41 studies, 13 had a main conclusion that suggested more depression in winter (Table 3). The remaining studies either suggested no seasonal pattern, indications of seasonality but outside winter, or ambiguous results in terms of seasonality. The total evidence across the studies was highly equivocal with great heterogeneity in both research questions addressed, study design, definition of seasons, data collection, and statistical analysis. The results were not uniform across the studies, and it is not clear which months are implicated and how to define the season with increased risk. Half of the included studies on depression prevalence found results in line with seasonality in clinical depression. Beyond a possible impact of seasonality on clinical depression, we did not find convincing evidence for seasonality effect in depressive symptoms at the population level.

Table 3. Crude classification of number of papers with main result suggesting no seasonality, winter seasonality, other seasonality or ambiguous results in each of the study categories

Strengths and limitations

The main strength of this study was the systematic approach to search and appraise the literature with design constraints to minimise risk of bias. The broad search strategy could be both a strength and a limitation, but the opportunity to review adjacent aspects of depression together could be of value given the scattered literature on this topic.

We did not register a protocol for this review in advance, which is a limitation. The large heterogeneity of studies, data, and designs restricted us from conducting meta-analyses. It also precluded any approximation of the impact of publication bias, which typically results in non-conservative results (i.e. studies that support the associations of interest are more likely to appear in the published literature) (Dwan et al., Reference Dwan, Gamble, Williamson, Kirkham and Reporting Bias2013). Any bias that increases the likelihood of studies with no difference across seasons to remain unpublished would weaken the empirical support for seasonality of depression. Due to heterogeneity between study designs and reporting it was a challenge to find a standard tool to assess study quality. We ended up with adapting an existing framework (NOS), but assessment and analysis of study quality remained difficult due to the range of approaches used in this literature. Finally, study search and selection was challenging due to study heterogeneity and the broad scope we set up for this search. Some of the included studies were found through in additional searches and reference lists and additional relevant data and studies not identified by us may exist. Our scope for this review did not include careful differentiation between depression subtypes such as unipolar or bipolar depression.

Interpretation

There was a notable lack of consistency of effect in several studies observing seasonal effects. Skegg et al. (Reference Skegg, Skegg and McDonald1986) found a difference for males only and only after adjusting for a declining time-trend in antidepressant use. Schlager et al. (Reference Schlager, Schwartz and Bromet1993) found differences for women but no difference for men. Cobb et al. (Reference Cobb, Coryell, Cavanaugh, Keller, Solomon, Endicott, Potash and Fiedorowicz2014) found the difference in a post hoc test after the definition of winter was extended to include April, and Huibers et al. (Reference Huibers, de Graaf, Peeters and Arntz2010) found increased rates in summer and autumn. Park et al. (Reference Park, Kripke and Cole2007) found a trend in only one of two samples. The large study from Patten et al. (Reference Patten, Williams, Lavorato, Bulloch, Fiest, Wang and Sajobi2017) used a diagnostic interview to identify depression but still relied on subjective recall of onset, with some inherent risk of memory bias. Kerr et al. (Reference Kerr, Shaman, Washburn, Vuchinich, Neppl, Capaldi and Conger2013) used within-subjects repeated measurements. Although they found indications of more depressive symptoms in winter, effect sizes were minute. Many of the studies reported prevalence rates by month, rather than incidence rates that arguably are better suited to inform causal hypotheses on season and illness onset.

Four of seven studies on post-natal depression presented seasonal differences with higher prevalence among mothers who gave birth in autumn/winter compared to spring and summer. Biological causal models, often based on daylight deprivation, are frequently proposed. Social factors might also be of relevance and can coincide and/or reinforce with biological factors. For example, lack of social support is an acknowledged risk factor for postpartum depression (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Connolly and Tamim2014) and availability of social support could vary with seasons due to fewer outdoor activities or seasonal work patterns.

The studies on antidepressant prescriptions all observed seasonal variation, and two of them found the highest prescription rates in winter. These studies have high internal validity in that they present objective data with accurate dates, but they also reflect a response to illness rather than incidence of depression itself. Increased prescription rates can be a result of more severe episodes of clinical depression during the winter which increases both help-seeking and treatment response during those periods. It is also possible that some GPs more readily attribute symptom presentations to depression during certain seasons, which could also contribute to increased prescription.

The literature on seasonality of depressive illness have frequently cited access to daylight as a plausible mechanism, based on the phase shift hypothesis (Lewy et al., Reference Lewy, Sack, Singer and White1987) and the latitude hypothesis (Potkin et al., Reference Potkin, Zetin, Stamenkovic, Kripke and Bunney1986). Melatonin levels correlate negatively with light stimulus and promotes drowsiness (Srinivasan et al., Reference Srinivasan, Smits, Spence, Lowe, Kayumov, Pandi-Perumal, Parry and Cardinali2006). It is suggested that light deprivation brings on seasonal phase shifts in hormone levels, with Melatonin particularly implicated, which in turn may increase the risk of depression. Our results do not provide any clear support to this hypothesis as no clear population level trend was found and reiterates results of previous reviews of this question (Mersch et al., Reference Mersch, Middendorp, Bouhuys, Beersma and van den Hoofdakker1999). The latitude hypothesis and reduced daylight access have also given rise to light-therapy as intervention, but the evidence for its efficacy in preventing depression remains limited (Nussbaumer et al., Reference Nussbaumer, Kaminski-Hartenthaler, Forneris, Morgan, Sonis, Gaynes, Greenblatt, Wipplinger, Lux, Winkler, Van Noord, Hofmann and Gartlehner2015).

This systematic review did not point towards a clear and unified pattern on seasonal variation in depression and depressive symptoms. This does not exclude that seasonal variation influences individuals. Neither does it exclude that for some, such variation may shift individuals to clinically relevant states. It is possible that environmental seasonal change to some extent affects everyone, but that we cope and adapt in ways embedded in culture, behavioural patterns, technology, and societal structures. As exemplified by Kerr et al. (Reference Kerr, Shaman, Washburn, Vuchinich, Neppl, Capaldi and Conger2013), other risk factors for depression seem more salient.

Our results are relevant for the longstanding discussion around seasonal affective disorder. Some of the studies included here did point to a change in the prevalence of depression with seasons. However, we do not see the results from the studies included in this review to be in support of any strong general and public health relevant effect of seasons on mood.

Suggestions for future research in this field

The identified studies used highly heterogeneous study designs and the fragmented results suggest a potential for methodological improvements in this research. The many ways to measure and operationalise depression was also reflected here in terms of scales used, cut-offs and case definitions. Regarding measurement density, some studies had two measurements over the course of a year, while others had monthly registrations. There was also little consensus as to how seasons or winter was defined across studies. Some examined specific months while others used broader categories such as spring and autumn. For example, Cobb et al. (Reference Cobb, Coryell, Cavanaugh, Keller, Solomon, Endicott, Potash and Fiedorowicz2014) included April in winter, while Michalak et al. (Reference Michalak, Murray, Wilkinson, Dowrick, Lasa, Lehtinen, Dalgard, Ayuso-Mateos, Vazquez-Barquero, Casey and Group2004) defined April as part of spring. Yet others defined seasons in relation to winter and summer solstice and in many studies definitions of seasons remained unclear.

Many of the studies included in this review used cross-sectional data collections that ran over time and covered the seasons of interest, but that was set up for other purposes than to study seasonality. This design ensures that participants were indeed blind to the research hypothesis. A disadvantage is that design features, such as choice of measurement, timing and frequency seemed less than optimal for many of the papers. For many of the cross-sectional data collections, it was unknown when cases had their onset. As such, cases identified at a given time point may both reflect increased incidence at that time, but also reduced remission rates. This challenges interpretations.

Our results suggest there is a need for more high quality, unbiased studies on seasonal variation in depression. Nominal exposure categories such as ‘winter’ is a crude term to describe exposures, and future studies should accurately state the time-period definitions coupled with informative data on the assumed underlying mechanism. Where possible, analyses should include geographical data and other contexts that could relate to observations such as climate and weather. There may also be important confounders to consider, such as physical activity, sleep and food intake that could both be confounders but also potential mechanisms between season and mental health. Clinical registry data could provide an excellent data source by providing incidence rates per time. Repeated surveys with screening tools will most often reflect prevalence, which could both be a derivative of seasonal variation in remission rates as well as seasonal onset. Precision around these features of studies is important for interpretation and allow for meta-analysis in future reviews in this area.

Conclusion

We conclude that there is some support for seasonal variation in clinical depression, but that this is not likely due to a broad and general mechanism where entire populations are shifted towards lower mood and more sub-threshold symptoms at regular intervals throughout the year. This could be an important nuance for the public, particularly those exposed to major shifts in daylight that frequently get information that suggest winter and less daylight will bring down your mood. Further development in this field will require higher study quality and more unbiased population-based studies on the potential relationship between seasonal changes and depression.

Availability of data and materials

All data for this review are available in the included papers. Details on quality assessment of single studies is available upon request to the corresponding author.

Author ORCIDs

Simon Øverland, 0000-0001-6967-9355; Ian Colman, 0000-0001-5924-0277.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the contributions from Anja Steinsland Ariansen and Alexanda Loro who assisted in the initial screening for relevant papers.

Financial support

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have any competing interests to report.

Appendix

Search terms used for: seasonality and symptoms of depression: a systematic review of the literature

Approach

The following databases were accessed during the electronic component of the systematic review: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, DARE (Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects), Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), Medline and Medline in Process (via OVID), Embase (via OVID), PsycINFO (via OVID) and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL). A search strategy was developed to identify keywords and Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) in Medline, which were then adapted for all other databases.

=2121

Medline in Process and OVID Medline

  1. 1. Seasons/

  2. 2. (season* adj3 varia*).tw.

  3. 3. seasonalit*.tw.

  4. 4. (season* adj2 pattern*).tw.

  5. 5. (periodic* adj3 varia*).tw.

  6. 6. (periodic* adj3 fluctuation*).tw.

  7. 7. (season* adj2 adjust*).tw.

  8. 8. (season* adj2 change*).tw.

  9. 9. (season* adj2 rhythm*).tw.

  10. 10. (season* adj2 inciden*).tw.

  11. 11. or/1-10

  12. 12. Depression/

  13. 13. exp Self-Injurious Behavior/

  14. 14. exp Anxiety/

  15. 15. exp Anxiety Disorders/

  16. 16. exp Mood Disorders/

  17. 17. exp Sleep Disorders/

  18. 18. depress*.tw.sea

  19. 19. parasuicide*.tw.

  20. 20. automutilation.tw.

  21. 21. (self adj1 harm*).tw.

  22. 22. (self adj1 destruct*).tw.

  23. 23. (self adj1 injur*).tw.

  24. 24. (self adj1 mutilat*).tw.

  25. 25. suicid*.tw.

  26. 26. anxiet*.tw.

  27. 27. nervousness.tw.

  28. 28. hypervigilance*.tw.

  29. 29. (anxi* adj3 (dis* or syndrome* or neuros#s)).tw.

  30. 30. (personalit* adj2 anankastic*).tw.

  31. 31. agoraphob*.tw.

  32. 32. (panic adj2 (disorder* or attack*)).tw.

  33. 33. hoard*.tw.

  34. 34. (phobia adj2 disorder*).tw.

  35. 35. (stress adj3 disorder*).tw.

  36. 36. (hyperkinetic adj2 heart adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  37. 37. (neuros#s adj2 cardiac*).tw.

  38. 38. (effort* adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  39. 39. (neurocirculator* adj2 asthenia*).tw.

  40. 40. (affective adj2 (disorder* or psychos#s)).tw.

  41. 41. (mood adj1 disorder*).tw.

  42. 42. (bipolar adj2 disorder*).tw.

  43. 43. manic-depressi*.tw.

  44. 44. melancholi*.tw.

  45. 45. (cyclothymic* adj2 disorder*).tw.

  46. 46. insomn*.tw.

  47. 47. (sleep adj2 (dis* or dysfunction* or syndrome* or deprivation or paroxysm* or myoclonu* or hyponea* or apnea*)).tw.

  48. 48. dyssomnia*.tw.

  49. 49. hypersomnia*.tw.

  50. 50. (excessiv* adj2 somnolence*).tw.

  51. 51. hypersomnolence*.tw.

  52. 52. narcoleps*.tw.

  53. 53. (gelineau adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  54. 54. catalep*.tw.

  55. 55. (kleine-levin adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  56. 56. (toneless* adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  57. 57. (nocturnal adj2 myoclon*).tw.

  58. 58. (henneberg adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  59. 59. (restless adj3 syndrome*).tw.

  60. 60. (periodic adj3 movement adj1 disorder*).tw.

  61. 61. (willis-ekbom adj1 (dis* or syndrome*)).tw.

  62. 62. (wittmaack-ekbom adj1 (dis* or syndrome*)).tw.

  63. 63. (apnea adj2 central*).tw.

  64. 64. (central adj3 hypoventilation*).tw.

  65. 65. (ondine adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  66. 66. (pickwickian adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  67. 67. or/12-66

  68. 68. exp Adult/

  69. 69. Adolescent/

  70. 70. adult*.tw.

  71. 71. adolescent*.tw.

  72. 72. teen*.tw.

  73. 73. or/68-72

  74. 74. 11 and 67 and 73

Embase

  1. 1. exp season/

  2. 2. (season* adj3 varia*).tw.

  3. 3. (season* adj2 pattern*).tw.

  4. 4. (periodic* adj3 varia*).tw.

  5. 5. (periodic* adj3 fluctuation*).tw.

  6. 6. (season* adj2 adjust*).tw.

  7. 7. (season* adj2 change*).tw.

  8. 8. (season* adj2 rhythm*).tw.

  9. 9. (season* adj2 inciden*).tw.

  10. 10. seasonalit*.tw.

  11. 11. or/1-10

  12. 12. exp depression/

  13. 13. automutilation/

  14. 14. anxiety/

  15. 15. exp anxiety disorder/

  16. 16. exp mood disorder/

  17. 17. sleep disorder/

  18. 18. depression.tw.

  19. 19. parasuicide*.tw.

  20. 20. (self adj1 harm*).tw.

  21. 21. (self adj1 destruct*).tw.

  22. 22. (self adj1 injur*).tw.

  23. 23. (self adj1 mutilat*).tw.

  24. 24. suicid*.tw.

  25. 25. anxiet*.tw.

  26. 26. nervousness.tw.

  27. 27. hypervigilance*.tw.

  28. 28. (anxi* adj3 (dis* or syndrome* or neuros#s)).tw.

  29. 29. (personalit* adj2 anankastic*).tw.

  30. 30. (panic adj2 (disorder* or attack*)).tw.

  31. 31. agoraphob*.tw.

  32. 32. hoarder*.tw.

  33. 33. (phobia adj2 disorder*).tw.

  34. 34. (stress adj3 disorder*).tw.

  35. 35. (hyperkinetic adj2 heart adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  36. 36. (neuros#s adj2 cardiac*).tw.

  37. 37. (effort* adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  38. 38. (neurocirculator* adj2 asthenia*).tw.

  39. 39. (affective adj2 (disorder* or psychos#s)).tw.

  40. 40. (mood adj1 disorder*).tw.

  41. 41. (bipolar adj2 disorder*).tw.

  42. 42. manic-depressi*.tw.

  43. 43. melancholi*.tw.

  44. 44. (cyclothymic* adj2 disorder*).tw.

  45. 45. insomn*.tw.

  46. 46. (sleep adj2 (dis* or dysfunction* or syndrome* or deprivation or paroxysm* or myoclonu* or hyponea* or apnea*)).tw.

  47. 47. dyssomnia*.tw.

  48. 48. hypersomnia*.tw.

  49. 49. (excessiv* adj2 somnolence*).tw.

  50. 50. hypersomnolence*.tw.

  51. 51. narcoleps*.tw.

  52. 52. (gelineau adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  53. 53. catalep*.tw.

  54. 54. (kleine-levin adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  55. 55. (toneless* adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  56. 56. (nocturnal adj2 myoclon*).tw.

  57. 57. (henneberg adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  58. 58. (restless adj3 syndrome*).tw.

  59. 59. (periodic adj3 movement adj1 disorder*).tw.

  60. 60. (willis-ekbom adj1 (dis* or syndrome*)).tw.

  61. 61. (wittmaack-ekbom adj1 (dis* or syndrome*)).tw.

  62. 62. (apnea adj2 central*).tw.

  63. 63. (central adj3 hypoventilation*).tw.

  64. 64. (ondine adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  65. 65. (pickwickian adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  66. 66. or/12-65

  67. 67. adult/

  68. 68. adolescent/

  69. 69. adult*.tw.

  70. 70. adolescent*.tw.

  71. 71. teen*.tw.

  72. 72. or/67-71

  73. 73. 11 and 66 and 72

PsycINFO

  1. 1. seasonal variations/

  2. 2. seasonalit*.tw.

  3. 3. (season* adj2 adjust*).tw.

  4. 4. (season* adj2 change*).tw.

  5. 5. (season* adj2 inciden*).tw.

  6. 6. (season* adj2 pattern*).tw.

  7. 7. (season* adj2 rhythm*).tw.

  8. 8. (season* adj3 varia*).tw.

  9. 9. (periodic* adj3 fluctuation*).tw.

  10. 10. (periodic* adj3 varia*).tw.

  11. 11. or/1-10

  12. 12. exp affective disorders/

  13. 13. exp self destructive behavior/

  14. 14. exp anxiety/

  15. 15. exp anxiety disorders/

  16. 16. exp sleep disorders/

  17. 17. depress*.tw.

  18. 18. parasuicide*.tw.

  19. 19. automutilation.tw.

  20. 20. (self adj1 harm*).tw.

  21. 21. (self adj1 destruct*).tw.

  22. 22. (self adj1 injur*).tw.

  23. 23. (self adj1 mutilat*).tw.

  24. 24. suicid*.tw.

  25. 25. anxiet*.tw.

  26. 26. nervousness.tw.

  27. 27. hypervigilance*.tw.

  28. 28. (anxi* adj3 (dis* or syndrome* or neuros#s)).tw.

  29. 29. (personalit* adj2 anankastic*).tw.

  30. 30. agoraphob*.tw.

  31. 31. (panic adj2 (disorder* or attack*)).tw.

  32. 32. hoard*.tw.

  33. 33. (phobia adj2 disorder*).tw.

  34. 34. (stress adj3 disorder*).tw.

  35. 35. (hyperkinetic adj2 heart adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  36. 36. (neuros#s adj2 cardiac*).tw.

  37. 37. (effort* adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  38. 38. (neurocirculator* adj2 asthenia*).tw.

  39. 39. (affective adj2 (disorder* or psychos#s)).tw.

  40. 40. (mood adj1 disorder*).tw.

  41. 41. (bipolar adj2 disorder*).tw.

  42. 42. manic-depressi*.tw.

  43. 43. melancholi*.tw.

  44. 44. (cyclothymic* adj2 disorder*).tw.

  45. 45. insomn*.tw.

  46. 46. (sleep adj2 (dis* or dysfunction* or syndrome* or deprivation or paroxysm* or myoclonu* or hyponea* or apnea*)).tw.

  47. 47. dyssomnia*.tw.

  48. 48. hypersomnia*.tw.

  49. 49. (excessiv* adj2 somnolence*).tw.

  50. 50. hypersomnolence*.tw.

  51. 51. narcoleps*.tw.

  52. 52. (gelineau adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  53. 53. catalep*.tw.

  54. 54. (kleine-levin adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  55. 55. (toneless* adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  56. 56. (nocturnal adj2 myoclon*).tw.

  57. 57. (henneberg adj1 syndrome*).tw.

  58. 58. (restless adj3 syndrome*).tw.

  59. 59. (periodic adj3 movement adj1 disorder*).tw.

  60. 60. (willis-ekbom adj1 (dis* or syndrome*)).tw.

  61. 61. (wittmaack-ekbom adj1 (dis* or syndrome*)).tw.

  62. 62. (apnea adj2 central*).tw.

  63. 63. (central adj3 hypoventilation*).tw.

  64. 64. (ondine adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  65. 65. (pickwickian adj2 syndrome*).tw.

  66. 66. or/12-65

  67. 67. 11 and 66

  68. 68. limit 67 to (200 adolescence or ‘300 adulthood’)

References

Abell, JG, Stalder, T, Ferrie, JE, Shipley, MJ, Kirschbaum, C, Kivimäki, M and Kumari, M (2016) Assessing cortisol from hair samples in a large observational cohort: the Whitehall II study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 73, 148156.Google Scholar
Albin, R (1982) Christmas blues: reality or myth? In The Graduate School of Arts and Sciences. The George Washington University.Google Scholar
Anastasi, S, Eusebi, P and Quartesan, RJPD (2014) Psychiatry in the emergency room: one year period of clinical experience. Psychiatria Danubina 26, 5665.Google Scholar
Avery, DH, Dahl, K, Savage, MV, Brengelmann, GL, Larsen, LH, Kenny, MA, Eder, DN, Vitiello, MV and Prinz, PN (1997) Circadian temperature and cortisol rhythms during a constant routine are phase-delayed in hypersomnic winter depression. Biological Psychiatry 41, 11091123.Google Scholar
Ayers, JW, Althouse, BM, Allem, JP, Rosenquist, JN and Ford, DE (2013) Seasonality in seeking mental health information on Google. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 44, 520525.Google Scholar
Balestrieri, M, Bragagnoli, N and Bellantuono, C (1991) Antidepressant drug prescribing in general practice: a 6-year study. Journal of Affective Disorders 21, 4555.Google Scholar
Ballard, CG, Mohan, M and Davis, A (1993) Seasonal variation in the prevalence of postnatal depression. European Journal of Psychiatry 7, 7376.Google Scholar
Belleville, G, Foldes-Busque, G, Dixon, M, Marquis-Pelletier, E, Barbeau, S, Poitras, J, Chauny, JM, Diodati, JG, Fleet, R and Marchand, A (2013) Impact of seasonal and lunar cycles on psychological symptoms in the ED: an empirical investigation of widely spread beliefs. General Hospital Psychiatry 35, 192194.Google Scholar
Bronson, FH (1995) Seasonal variation in human reproduction: environmental factors. Quarterly Review of Biology 70, 141164.Google Scholar
Carlsson, A, Svennerholm, L and Winblad, B (1980) Seasonal and circadian monoamine variations in human brains examined post mortem. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. Supplementum 280, 75.Google Scholar
Cerbus, G and Dallara, RF Jr (1975) Seasonal differences of depression in mental hospital admissions as measured by the MMPI. Psychological Reports 36, 737738.Google Scholar
Christensen, R and Dowrick, PW (1983) Myths of mid-winter depression. Community Mental Health Journal 19, 177186.Google Scholar
Cobb, BS, Coryell, WH, Cavanaugh, J, Keller, M, Solomon, DA, Endicott, J, Potash, JB and Fiedorowicz, JG (2014) Seasonal variation of depressive symptoms in unipolar major depressive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry 55, 18911899.Google Scholar
Cox, JL, Holden, JM and Sagovsky, R (1987) Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 150, 782786.Google Scholar
Crum, A and Phillips, DJ (2015) Self-fulfilling prophesies, placebo effects, and the social–psychological creation of reality. In Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
de Craen, AJ, Gussekloo, J, van der Mast, RC, le Cessie, S, Lemkes, JW and Westendorp, RG (2005) Seasonal mood variation in the elderly: the Leiden 85-plus study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20, 269273.Google Scholar
de Graaf, R, van Dorsselaer, S, ten Have, M, Schoemaker, C and Vollebergh, WA (2005) Seasonal variations in mental disorders in the general population of a country with a maritime climate: findings from the Netherlands mental health survey and incidence study. American Journal of Epidemiology 162, 654661.Google Scholar
Doganer, YC, Angstman, KB, Kaufman, TK and Rohrer, JE (2015) Seasonal variation in clinical remission of primary care patients with depression: impact of gender. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice 21, 160165.Google Scholar
Dominiak, M, Swiecicki, L and Rybakowski, J (2015) Psychiatric hospitalizations for affective disorders in Warsaw, Poland: effect of season and intensity of sunlight. Psychiatry Research 229, 287294.Google Scholar
Dwan, K, Gamble, C, Williamson, PR, Kirkham, JJ and Reporting Bias, G (2013) Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias – an updated review. PloS One 8, e66844.Google Scholar
Eastwood, MR and Stiasny, S (1978) Psychiatric disorder, hospital admission, and season. Archives of General Psychiatry 35, 769771.Google Scholar
Gardarsdottir, H, Egberts, TC, van Dijk, L and Heerdink, ER (2010) Seasonal patterns of initiating antidepressant therapy in general practice in the Netherlands during 2002–2007. Journal of Affective Disorders 122, 208212.Google Scholar
Hansen, V, Skre, I and Lund, E (2008) What is this thing called ‘SAD’? A critique of the concept of Seasonal Affective Disorder. Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale 17, 120127.Google Scholar
Harris, C (1984) Seasonal variations in depression and osteoarthritis. Journal of Royal College of General Practitioners 34, 436439.Google Scholar
Harris, S and Dawson-Hughes, B (1993) Seasonal mood changes in 250 normal women. Psychiatry Research 49, 7787.Google Scholar
Henriksson, HE, Sylven, SM, Kallak, TK, Papadopoulos, FC and Skalkidou, A (2017) Seasonal patterns in self-reported peripartum depressive symptoms. European Psychiatry 43, 99108.Google Scholar
Holloway, LE and Evans, S (2014) Seasonality of depression referrals in older people. Community Mental Health Journal 50, 336338.Google Scholar
Huibers, MJ, de Graaf, LE, Peeters, FP and Arntz, A (2010) Does the weather make us sad? Meteorological determinants of mood and depression in the general population. Psychiatry Research 180, 143146.Google Scholar
Jewell, JS, Dunn, AL, Bondy, J and Leiferman, J (2010) Prevalence of self-reported postpartum depression specific to season and latitude of birth: evaluating the PRAMS data. Maternal and Child Health Journal 14, 261267.Google Scholar
Kalbitzer, J, Erritzoe, D, Holst, KK, Nielsen, , Marner, L, Lehel, S, Arentzen, T, Jernigan, TL and Knudsen, GM (2010) Seasonal changes in brain serotonin transporter binding in short serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region-allele carriers but not in long-allele homozygotes. Biological Psychiatry 67, 10331039.Google Scholar
Kerr, D, Shaman, J, Washburn, I, Vuchinich, S, Neppl, T, Capaldi, D and Conger, R (2013) Two longterm studies of seasonal variation in depressive symptoms among community participants. Journal of Affective Disorders 151, 837842.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Demler, O, Jin, R, Koretz, D, Merikangas, KR, Rush, AJ, Walters, EE and Wang, PS (2003) The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). JAMA 289, 30953105.Google Scholar
Kim, TH, Connolly, JA and Tamim, H (2014) The effect of social support around pregnancy on postpartum depression among Canadian teen mothers and adult mothers in the maternity experiences survey. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth 14, 162.Google Scholar
Kivela, A, Kauppila, A, Ylostalo, P, Vakkuri, O and Leppaluoto, J (1988) Seasonal, menstrual and circadian secretions of melatonin, gonadotropins and prolactin in women. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 132, 321327.Google Scholar
Kristjansdottir, J, Olsson, GI, Sundelin, C and Naessen, T (2013) Self-reported health in adolescent girls varies according to the season and its relation to medication and hormonal contraception–a descriptive study. European Journal of Contraception and Reproductive Health Care 18, 343354.Google Scholar
Lambert, GW, Reid, C, Kaye, DM, Jennings, GL and Esler, MD (2002) Effect of sunlight and season on serotonin turnover in the brain. Lancet 360, 18401842.Google Scholar
Lewy, AJ, Sack, RL, Singer, CM and White, DM (1987) The phase shift hypothesis for bright light's therapeutic mechanism of action: theoretical considerations and experimental evidence. Psychopharmacology Bulletin 23, 349353.Google Scholar
Magnusson, A, Axelsson, J, Karlsson, MM and Oskarsson, H (2000) Lack of seasonal mood change in the Icelandic population: results of a cross-sectional study. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 234238.Google Scholar
Markkula, N, Suvisaari, J, Saarni, SI, Pirkola, S, Peña, S, Saarni, S, Ahola, K, Mattila, AK, Viertiö, S and Strehle, J (2015) Prevalence and correlates of major depressive disorder and dysthymia in an eleven-year follow-up–results from the Finnish Health 2011 Survey. Journal of Affective Disorders 173, 7380.Google Scholar
Mersch, PP, Middendorp, HM, Bouhuys, AL, Beersma, DG and van den Hoofdakker, RH (1999) Seasonal affective disorder and latitude: a review of the literature. Journal of Affective Disorders 53, 3548.Google Scholar
Mersch, PP, Vastenburg, NC, Meesters, Y, Bouhuys, AL, Beersma, DG, van den Hoofdakker, RH and den Boer, JA (2004) The reliability and validity of the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire: a comparison between patient groups. Journal of Affective Disorders 80, 209219.Google Scholar
Michalak, EE, Murray, G, Wilkinson, C, Dowrick, C, Lasa, L, Lehtinen, V, Dalgard, OS, Ayuso-Mateos, JL, Vazquez-Barquero, JL, Casey, P and Group, O (2004) Estimating depression prevalence from the Beck Depression Inventory: is season of administration a moderator? Psychiatry Research 129, 99106.Google Scholar
Morera, AL and Abreu, P (2006) Seasonality of psychopathology and circannual melatonin rhythm. Journal of Pineal Research 41, 279283.Google Scholar
Murase, S, Murase, S, Kitabatake, M, Yamauchi, T and Mathe, AA (1995) Seasonal mood variation among Japanese residents of Stockholm. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 92, 5155.Google Scholar
Nayyar, K and Cochrane, R (1996) Seasonal changes in affective state measured prospectively and retrospectively. British Journal of Psychiatry 168, 627632.Google Scholar
Neumeister, A, Pirker, W, Willeit, M, Praschak-Rieder, N, Asenbaum, S, Brücke, T and Kasper, S (2000) Seasonal variation of availability of serotonin transporter binding sites in healthy female subjects as measured by [123 I]-2β-carbomethoxy-3β-(4-iodophenyl) tropane and single photon emission computed tomography. Biological Psychiatry 47, 158160.Google Scholar
Nussbaumer, B, Kaminski-Hartenthaler, A, Forneris, CA, Morgan, LC, Sonis, JH, Gaynes, BN, Greenblatt, A, Wipplinger, J, Lux, LJ, Winkler, D, Van Noord, MG, Hofmann, J and Gartlehner, G (2015) Light therapy for preventing seasonal affective disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, CD011269.Google Scholar
O'Hare, C, O'Sullivan, V, Flood, S and Kenny, RA (2016) Seasonal and meteorological associations with depressive symptoms in older adults: a geo-epidemiological study. Journal of Affective Disorders 191, 172179.Google Scholar
Park, DH, Kripke, DF and Cole, RJ (2007) More prominent reactivity in mood than activity and sleep induced by differential light exposure due to seasonal and local differences. Chronobiology International 24, 905920.Google Scholar
Patten, SB, Williams, JV, Lavorato, DH, Fiest, KM, Bulloch, AG and Wang, J (2015) The prevalence of major depression is not changing. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 60, 3134.Google Scholar
Patten, SB, Williams, JV, Lavorato, DH, Bulloch, AG, Fiest, KM, Wang, JL and Sajobi, TT (2017) Seasonal variation in major depressive episode prevalence in Canada. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 26, 169176.Google Scholar
Posternak, MA and Zimmerman, M (2002) Lack of association between seasonality and psychopathology in psychiatric outpatients. Psychiatry Research 112, 187194.Google Scholar
Potkin, SG, Zetin, M, Stamenkovic, V, Kripke, D and Bunney, WE Jr (1986) Seasonal affective disorder: prevalence varies with latitude and climate. Clinical Neuropharmacology 9, 181183.Google Scholar
Roecklein, KA and Rohan, KJ (2005) Seasonal affective disorder: an overview and update. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 2, 2026.Google Scholar
Roenneberg, T and Aschoff, J (1990) Annual rhythm of human reproduction: II. Environmental correlations. Journal of Biological Rhythms 5, 217239.Google Scholar
Rollnik, JD, Dimsdale, JE and Ng, B (2000) Variation of psychiatric emergencies across seasons in San Diego county. Depression and Anxiety 11, 4849.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, NE, Sack, DA, Gillin, JC, Lewy, AJ, Goodwin, FK, Davenport, Y, Mueller, PS, Newsome, DA and Wehr, TA (1984) Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry 41, 7280.Google Scholar
Sato, T, Bottlender, R, Sievers, M and Moller, HJ (2006) Distinct seasonality of depressive episodes differentiates unipolar depressive patients with and without depressive mixed states. Journal of Affective Disorders 90, 15.Google Scholar
Schlager, D, Schwartz, JE and Bromet, EJ (1993) Seasonal variations of current symptoms in a healthy population. British Journal of Psychiatry 163, 322326.Google Scholar
Shephard, RJ and Aoyagi, Y (2009) Seasonal variations in physical activity and implications for human health. European Journal of Applied Physiology 107, 251271.Google Scholar
Sit, D, Seltman, H and Wisner, KL (2011) Seasonal effects on depression risk and suicidal symptoms in postpartum women. Depression and Anxiety 28, 400405.Google Scholar
Skegg, K, Skegg, DC and McDonald, BW (1986) Is there seasonal variation in the prescribing of antidepressants in the community? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 40, 285288.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, V, Smits, M, Spence, W, Lowe, AD, Kayumov, L, Pandi-Perumal, SR, Parry, B and Cardinali, DP (2006) Melatonin in mood disorders. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 7, 138151.Google Scholar
Stordal, E, Morken, G, Mykletun, A, Neckelmann, D and Dahl, AA (2008) Monthly variation in prevalence rates of comorbid depression and anxiety in the general population at 63–65 degrees North: the HUNT study. Journal of Affective Disorders 106, 273278.Google Scholar
Sylven, SM, Papadopoulos, FC, Olovsson, M, Ekselius, L, Poromaa, IS and Skalkidou, A (2011) Seasonality patterns in postpartum depression. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 204, 413 e1–6.Google Scholar
Szabo, C and Blanche, M (1995) Seasonal variation in mood disorder presentation: further evidence of this phenomenon in a South African sample. Journal of Affective Disorders 33, 209214.Google Scholar
Traffanstedt, MK, Mehta, S and LoBello, SG (2016) Major depression with seasonal variation. Clinical Psychological Science 4, 825834.Google Scholar
Waraich, P, Goldner, EM, Somers, JM and Hsu, L (2004) Prevalence and incidence studies of mood disorders: a systematic review of the literature. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie 49, 124138.Google Scholar
Weinberger, DM, Krause, TG, Molbak, K, Cliff, A, Briem, H, Viboud, C and Gottfredsson, M (2012) Influenza epidemics in Iceland over 9 decades: changes in timing and synchrony with the United States and Europe. American Journal of Epidemiology 176, 649655.Google Scholar
Wells, G, Shea, B, O'connell, D, Peterson, J, Welch, V, Losos, M and Tugwell, P (2000) The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses.Google Scholar
Weobong, B, Ten Asbroek, AH, Soremekun, S, Danso, S, Owusu-Agyei, S, Prince, M and Kirkwood, BR (2015) Determinants of postnatal depression in rural Ghana: findings from the Don population based cohort study. Depression and Anxiety 32, 108119.Google Scholar
Whiteford, HA, Ferrari, AJ, Degenhardt, L, Feigin, V and Vos, T (2015) The global burden of mental, neurological and substance use disorders: an analysis from the global burden of disease study 2010. PloS One 10, e0116820.Google Scholar
Winthorst, W, Post, W, Meesters, Y, Penninx, B and Nolen, W (2011) Seasonality in depressive and anxiety symptoms among primary care patients and in patients with depressive and anxiety disorders; results from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety. BMC Psychiatry 11, 118.Google Scholar
Yang, SN, Shen, LJ, Ping, T, Wang, YC and Chien, CW (2011) The delivery mode and seasonal variation are associated with the development of postpartum depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 132, 158164.Google Scholar
Young, MA (2017) Does seasonal affective disorder exist? A commentary on Traffanstedt, Mehta, and LoBello (2016). Clinical Psychological Science 5, 750754.Google Scholar
Young, MA, Hutman, P, Enggasser, JL and Meesters, Y (2015) Assessing usual seasonal depression symptoms: the seasonality assessment form. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 37, 112121.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the literature search and study exclusion process.

Figure 1

Table 1. Description and main findings of included studies

Figure 2

Table 2. Study quality assessment through an adapted version of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS)

Figure 3

Table 3. Crude classification of number of papers with main result suggesting no seasonality, winter seasonality, other seasonality or ambiguous results in each of the study categories