Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-684899dbb8-vtfg7 Total loading time: 0.238 Render date: 2022-05-26T17:11:59.194Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "useRatesEcommerce": false, "useNewApi": true }

Using nonsense words to investigate vowel merger1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 June 2013

JENNIFER HAY
Affiliation:
University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealandjen.hay@canterbury.ac.nz
KATIE DRAGER
Affiliation:
University of Hawai'i at Mānoa, Department of Linguistics, 561 Moore Hall, 1890 East-West Road, Honolulu, Hawai'i 96822kdrager@hawaii.edu
BRYNMOR THOMAS
Affiliation:
United Arab Emirates University, PO Box 17172, Al Ain, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emiratesb.thomas@uaeu.ac.ae

Abstract

In previous work, we have found that New Zealand listeners who produce merged tokens of near and square can accurately distinguish between the vowels in perception even though they report that they are guessing. The ability to distinguish the vowels is affected by a variety of factors for these listeners, including the likelihood that the speaker and experimenter maintain the distinction (Hay et al. 2006b; Hay et al. 2010). In this article, we report on experiments that examine the production and perception of real and nonsense words in the context of two mergers: the Ellen/Allan merger in New Zealand English and the lot/thought merger found in American English. The results demonstrate that speakers’ degree of merger depends at least partially on whether the word is a real or nonsense word. Additionally, the results indicate that a token's real word status affects the merger differently in production and perception. We argue that these results provide evidence in favour of a hybrid model of speech production and perception, one with both abstract phoneme-level representations and acoustically detailed episodic representations.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

This work was supported by a Rutherford Discovery Fellowship to the first author. We are grateful to our referees for their detailed feedback, to Rebecca Clifford and Liam Walsh for their help with analysis, and to the New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour for their financial support.

References

Aylett, M. & Turk, A.. 2004. The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence and duration in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech 47 (1), 3156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, Guy, Wikle, Tom, Tillery, Jan & Sand, Lori. 1993. Some patterns of linguistic diffusion. Language Variation and Change 5, 359–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, Rachel E. & Bradlow, Ann. 2009. Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody. Language and Speech 52 (4), 391413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, Laurie. 1986. Notes on New Zealand English phonetics and phonology. English Worldwide 7, 225–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigham, Douglas S. 2010. Correlation of the low-back vowel merger and trap-retraction. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 15 (2), 2131.Google Scholar
Boberg, Charles. 2000. Geolinguistic diffusion and the US–Canada border. Language Variation and Change 12, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleveland, William S. 1981. LOWESS: A program for smoothing scatterplots by robust locally weighted regression. The American Statistician 35 (1), 54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, Cynthia G., Pisoni, David B. & de Jong, Kenneth. 2005. Acoustic characteristics of the vowel systems of sex regional varieties of American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 118 (3), 1661–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drager, Katie K. 2010. Sensitivity to grammatical and sociophonetic variability in perception. Laboratory Phonology 1 (1), 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drager, Katie K. 2011a. Sociophonetic variation and the lemma. Journal of Phonetics 39 (4), 694707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drager, Katie K. 2011b. Speaker age and vowel perception. Language and Speech 54 (1), 99121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drager, Katie K., Clifford, Rebecca & Hay, Jennifer. 2011. The production and perception of a low back vowel merger. Paper presented at New Ways of Analyzing Variation 40. Georgetown, October 2011.Google Scholar
Elley, W. B. & Irving, J. C.. 1985. The Elley-Irving socio-economic index: 1981 census revision. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies 20, 115–28.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerard, Khattab, Ghada & Malcah, Yaeger-Dror. 2010. Sound judgments: Perception of indexical features in children's speech. In Preston, Dennis R. & Niedzielski, Nancy (eds.), A reader in sociophonetics. New York: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Gahl, S. 2009. ‘Time’ and ‘thyme’ are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in a corpus of spontaneous speech. Language 84 (3), 474–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, Stephen D. 1996. Words and voices: Episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22 (5), 1166–83.Google ScholarPubMed
Gordon, Elizabeth & Maclagan, Margaret A.. 2001. ‘Capturing a sound change’: A real time study over 15 years of the near/square diphthong merger in New Zealand English. Australian Journal of Linguistics 21 (2), 215–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, Matthew J. 2006. Tracking the low back merger in Missouri. In Edward Murray, Thomas & Simon, Beth Lee (eds.), Language variation and change in the American Midland: A new look at ‘Heartland’ English. Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 5768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer & Bresnan, Joan. 2006. Spoken syntax: The phonetics of giving a hand in New Zealand English. The Linguistic Review 23, 321–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Drager, Katie & Warren, Paul. 2009. Careful who you talk to: An effect of experimenter identity on the production of the near/square merger in New Zealand English. Australian Journal of Linguistics 29 (2), 269–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Drager, Katie & Warren, Paul. 2010. Short-term exposure to one dialect affects processing of another. Language and Speech 53 (4), 447–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Nolan, Aaron & Drager, Katie. 2006a. From fush to feesh: Exemplar priming in speech perception. The Linguistic Review 23, 351–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Warren, Paul & Drager, Katie. 2006b. Factors influencing speech perception in the context of a merger-in-progress. Journal of Phonetics 34 (4), 458–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hintzman, Douglas L. 1986. ‘Schema abstraction’ in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review 93 (4), 411–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, Janet & Bell, Allan. 1992. On shear markets and sharing sheep: The merger of ear and air diphthongs in New Zealand English. Language Variation and Change 4, 251–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irons, Terry Lynn. 2007. On the status of the low back vowels in Kentucky English: More evidence of merger. Language Variation and Change 19, 137–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Keith. 1997. Speech perception without speaker normalization: An exemplar model. In Johnson, K. & Mullennix, J. W. (eds.), Talker variability in speech processing, 145–65. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Keith. 2006. Resonance in an exemplar-based lexicon: The emergence of social identity and phonology. Journal of Phonetics 34 (4), 485–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel, Bell, Alan & Girand, Cynthia. 2002. The role of the lemma in form variation. In Gussenhoven, Carlos & Warner, Natasha (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VII, 134. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 2001. Principles of linguistic change, vol. 2: Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 2010. Principles of linguistic change, vol. 3: Cognitive and cultural factors. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William, Ash, Sharon & Boberg, Charles. 2006. The atlas of North American English: Phonetics, phonology and sound change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William, Karan, Mark & Miller, Corey. 1991. Near mergers and the suspension of phonemic contrast. Language Variation and Change 3, 3374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maclagan, Margaret & Gordon, Elizabeth. 1996. Out of the air and into the ear: Another view of the New Zealand diphthong merger. Language Variation and Change 8, 125–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majors, Tivoli. 2007. Low back vowel merger in Missouri speech: Acoustic description and explanation. American Speech 80 (2), 165–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, Kuniko. 2011. Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. Journal of Phonetics 38: 132–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. 1986. Attention, similarity, and identification-categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology 115, 3957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierrehumbert, Janet. 2001. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Bybee, J. & Hopper, P. J. (eds.), Frequency effects and emergent grammar, 137–58. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2006. The next toolkit. Journal of Phonetics 34 (4), 516–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strand, Elizabeth A. & Johnson, Keith. 1996. Gradient and visual speaker normalization in the perception of fricatives. In Gibbon, Dafydd (ed.), Natural language processing and speech technology, 1426. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Thomas, Brynmor. 2004. In support of an exemplar-based approach to speech perception and production: A case study on the merging of pre-lateral dress and trap in New Zealand English. MA thesis, University of Canterbury.Google Scholar
Thomas, Brynmor & Hay, Jennifer. 2005. A pleasant malady: The Ellen/Allan merger in New Zealand English. Te Reo 48, 6993.Google Scholar
Ullman, T. Michael, Estabrooke, Ivy V., Steinhauer, Karsten, Brovetto, Claudia, Pancheva, Roumyana, Ozawa, Kaori, Mordecai, Kristen & Maki, Pauline. 2002. Sex differences in the neurocognition of language. Brain and Language 83, 141–3.Google Scholar
Wells, John C. 1982. Accents of English, 3 volumes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13
Cited by

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Using nonsense words to investigate vowel merger1
Available formats
×

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Using nonsense words to investigate vowel merger1
Available formats
×

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Using nonsense words to investigate vowel merger1
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *