Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:23:16.264Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion understanding in postinstitutionalized Eastern European children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2004

ALISON B. WISMER FRIES
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin at Madison
SETH D. POLLAK
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin at Madison

Abstract

To examine the effects of early emotional neglect on children's affective development, we assessed children who had experienced institutionalized care prior to adoption into family environments. One task required children to identify photographs of facial expressions of emotion. A second task required children to match facial expressions to an emotional situation. Internationally adopted, postinstitutionalized children had difficulty identifying facial expressions of emotion. In addition, postinstitutionalized children had significant difficulty matching appropriate facial expressions to happy, sad, and fearful scenarios. However, postinstitutionalized children performed as well as comparison children when asked to identify and match angry facial expressions. These results are discussed in terms of the importance of emotional input early in life on later developmental organization.The authors thank Paul Ekman and Linda Camras for making their facial expression stimuli available to us. Dirk Wilker, Ziggy Bialzik, and Craig Rypstat provided invaluable assistance with computer programming and the computerized implementation of the tasks reported in this article. We also appreciate the assistance of Gabrielle Sowle, Marna Brown, Justin Martin, Susan Perlman, Kristi Johnson, Anne Kolan, Erin Eatough, and Sarah Pluck in the collection of these data. Linda Camras provided very helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. These experiments would not be possible without the participation of many children and their families, for whose collaboration we are extremely appreciative. A preliminary version of these data were presented at the 2003 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health to Seth Pollak (MH 68858 and MH 61285). Alison Wismer Fries was also supported by a National Institutes of Health Training Program in Emotion Research (MH 18931).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ames, E. (1990). Spitz revisited: A trip to Romanian “orphanages.” Newsletter of the Canadian Psychological Association: Developmental Psychology Section 9, 811.Google Scholar
Ames, E. (1997). The development of romanian orphanage children adopted to Canada (Final report to the National Welfare Grants Program: Human Resources Development Canada). Burnaby, British Columbia: Simon Fraser University.
Camras, L. A., & Allison, K. (1985). Children's understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 9, 8494.Google Scholar
Camras, L. A., Grow, J. G., & Ribordy, S. C. (1983). Recognition of emotional expression by abused children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 12, 325328.Google Scholar
Camras, L. A., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Sachs, V., Spaccarelli, S., & Stefani, R. (1990). Maternal facial behavior and the recognition and production of emotional expressions by maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Developmental Psychology 26, 304312.Google Scholar
Cassidy, J., Parke, R. D., Butkovsky, L., & Braungart, J. M. (1992). Family-peer connections: The roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children's understanding of emotions. Child Development 63, 603618.Google Scholar
Castle, J., Groothues, C., Bredenkamp, D., Beckett, C., O'Connor, T., Rutter, M., & ERA Study Team. (1999). Effects of qualities of early institutional care on cognitive attainment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 69, 424437.Google Scholar
Chisholm, K. (1998). A three year follow up of attachment and indiscriminate friendliness in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Child Development 69, 10921106.Google Scholar
Chisholm, K., Carter, M. C., Ames, E. W., & Morison, S. J. (1995). Attachment security and indiscriminately friendly behavior in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 283294.Google Scholar
Chugani, H. T., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Juhasz, C., Nagy, F., & Chugani, D. C. (2001). Local brain functional activity following early deprivation: A study of post-institutionalized Romanian orphans. Neuroimage 14, 12901301.Google Scholar
Dashiell, J. F. (1927). A new method of measuring reactions to facial expressions of emotion. Psychological Bulletin 24, 174175.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (1994). Asymmetric brain function, affective style, and pschopathology: The role of early experience and plasticity. Development and Psychopathology 6, 741758.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development 57, 194201.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., & Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers' understanding of emotions. Child Study Journal 20, 171192.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development 61, 11451152.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers' emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology 30, 928936.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist's Press.
Fisher, L., Ames, E. W., Chisholm, K., & Savoie, L. (1997). Problems reported by parents of Romanian orphans adopted to British Columbia. International Journal of Behavioral Development 20, 6782.Google Scholar
Garner, P. W., Carlson Jones, D., & Miner, J. L. (1994). Social competence among low-income preschoolers: Emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Development 65, 622637.Google Scholar
Groze, V., & Ileana, D. (1996). A follow-up study of adopted children from Romania. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal 13, 541565.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R. (2000). Early adversity and the development of stress reactivity and regulation. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), The effects of adversity on neurobehavioral development: Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 31, pp. 163200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gunnar, M. R. (2001). Effects of early deprivation: Findings from orphanage-reared infants and children. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 617629). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K., & Suomi, S. J. (1971). From thought to therapy: Lessons from a primate laboratory. American Scientist 59, 538549.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., Johnson, C. N., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., & Cooke. (1989). Young children's theory of mind and emotion. Cognition and Emotion 3, 379400.Google Scholar
Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989a). IQ and behavioural adjustment of ex-institutional adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 30, 5375.Google Scholar
Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989b). Social and family relationships of ex-institutionalized adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 30, 7797.Google Scholar
Human Rights Watch. (1998). Abandoned to the state: Cruelty and neglect in Russian orphanages. New York: Human Rights Watch.
Immigration and Naturalization Service (INS). (2002). Immigrant orphans adopted by U.S. citizens by sex, age, and region and country of birth. Available on-line at www.ins.gov/graphics/aboutins/statistics/97imtbls.htm.
Izard, C. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Applenton–Century–Crofts.
Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science 12, 1823.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. E. (2000a). Long-term medical issues in international adoptees. Pediatric Annals 29, 234241.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. E. (2000b). Medical and developmental sequelae of early childhood institutionalization in Eastern European adoptees. In C. Nelson (Ed.), The effects of early adversity on neurobehavioral development. Minnesota symposia on child psychology (vol. 31, pp. 113162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Johnson, D. E., Miller, L. C., Iverson, S., Thomas, W., Franchino, B., Dole, K., Kiernan, M. T., Georgieff, M. K., & Hostetter, M. K. (1992). The health of children adopted from Romania. Journal of the American Medical Association 24, 34463451.Google Scholar
Levine, L. (1995). Young children's understanding of the causes of anger and sadness. Child Development 66, 697709.Google Scholar
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Day, J. C., Francis, D. D., & Meaney, M. J. (2000). Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis, and cognitive development in rats. Nature Neuroscience 3, 799806.Google Scholar
Miller, L. C. (2000). Initial assessment of growth, development, and the effects of institutionalization in internationally adopted children. Pediatric Annals 29, 224233.Google Scholar
Miller, R. E., Caul, W. F., & Mirsky, I. A. (1967). Communication of affect between feral and socially isolated monkeys. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 7, 231239.Google Scholar
O'Connor, T. G., Bredenkamp, D., Rutter, M., & ERA Study Team. (1999). Attachment disturbances and disorders in children exposed to early severe deprivation. Infant Mental Health Journal 20, 1029.Google Scholar
O'Connor, T. G., Rutter, M. R., Beckett, C., Keaveney, L., Kreppner, J. M., & ERA Study Team (2000). The effects of global severe privation on cognitive competence: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Child Development 71, 376390.Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology 36, 679688.Google Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Kalish, C. W., & Perlman, S. B. (2004). The role of experience in children's understanding of the antecedents of emotion. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Pollak, S. D., Klorman, R., Thatcher, J. E., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). P3b reflects maltreated children's reactions to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology 38, 267274.Google Scholar
Ribordy, S. C., Camras, L. A., Stefani, R., & Spaccarelli, S. (1988). Vignettes for emotion recognition research and affective therapy with children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 17, 322325.Google Scholar
Rogosch, F. A., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1995). The role of child maltreatment in early deviations in cognitive and affective processing abilities and later peer relationship problems. Development and Psychopathology 7, 591609.Google Scholar
Rovee–Collier, C. K., & Capatides, J. B. (1979). Positive behavioral contrast in 3-month-old infants on multiple conjugate reinforcement schedules. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32, 1527.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1981). Maternal deprivation reassessed. New York: Penguin Books.
Rutter, M., Andersen–Wood, L., Becket, C., Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., Kreppner, J., Keaveney, L., Lord, C., O'Connor, T. G., & ERA Study Team. (1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following severe early global privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40, 537549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., & ERA Study Team. (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 39, 465476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, M. M., Ladd, C. O., & Plotsky, P. M. (2001). Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology 13, 419450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, D., Izard, C., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotional knowledge in economically disadvantaged children: Self-regulatory antecedents and relations to social difficulties and withdrawal. Development and Psychopathology 13, 5367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S. J., Ginsberg, S. D., Hof, P. R., Foote, S. L., Young, W. G., & Draemer, G. W. (1993). Effects of social deprivation in prepubescent rhesus monkeys: Immunohistochemical analysis of the neurofilament protein triplet in the hippocampal formation. Brain Research 619, 299305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloutsky, V. M. (1997). Institutional care and developmental outcomes of 6- and 7-year old children: A contextualist perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development 20, 131151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Southam–Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation and understanding: Implications for child psychopathology and therapy. Clinical Psychology Review 22, 189222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tizard, B., & Hodges, J. (1978). The effect of early institutional rearing on the development of 8-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 19, 99118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
US State Department. (1999). Available on-line at www.travel.state.gov/orphan_numbers.html.
Verhulst, F. C., Althaus, M., & Versluis den Bieman, H. J. (1990). Problem behavior in international adoptees: II. Age at placement. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 29, 104111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhulst, F. C., Althaus, M., & Versluis den Bieman, H. J. (1992). Damaging backgrounds: Later adjustment of international adoptees. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31, 518524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeanah, C. H. (2000). Disturbances of attachment in young children adopted from institutions. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 21, 230236.Google Scholar