Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-cf9d5c678-cnwzk Total loading time: 0.251 Render date: 2021-08-01T09:13:19.591Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "metricsAbstractViews": false, "figures": true, "newCiteModal": false, "newCitedByModal": true, "newEcommerce": true, "newUsageEvents": true }

The stability of the feedback negativity and its relationship with depression during childhood and adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2015

Jennifer N. Bress
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University
Alexandria Meyer
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University
Greg Hajcak Proudfit
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University
Corresponding

Abstract

Feedback negativity (FN) is an event-related potential elicited by monetary reward and loss; it is thought to relate to reward-related neural activity and has been linked to depression in children and adults. In the current study, we examined the stability of FN, and its relationship with depression in adolescents, over 2 years in 45 8- to 13-year-old children. From Time 1 to Time 2, FN in response to monetary loss and in response to monetary gain showed moderate to strong reliability (rs = .64 and .67, respectively); these relationships remained significant even when accounting for related variables. FN also demonstrated high within-session reliability. Moreover, the relationship between a blunted FN and greater depression observed at Time 1 was reproduced at Time 2, and the magnitude of FN at Time 1 predicted depressive symptomatology at Time 2. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that FN and its relationship with depression remain consistent over the course of development, and that FN may prospectively predict later depressive symptomatology. The current results suggest that FN may be suitable as a biomarker of depressive symptoms during adolescence.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.Google ScholarPubMed
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Worthman, C. M. (1998). Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychological Medicine, 28, 5161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, T. E., & Holroyd, C. B. (2011). Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex in reward and conflict processing as revealed by the feedback error-related negativity and n200. Biological Psychology, 87, 2534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaver, J. D., Lawrence, A. D., Van Ditzhuijzen, J., Davis, M. H., Woods, A., & Calder, A. J. (2006). Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 51605166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, M. P., Nitsch, A. M., Miltner, W. H., & Straube, T. (2014). A single-trial estimation of the feedback-related negativity and its relation to BOLD responses in a time-estimation task. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 30053012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernat, E. M., Nelson, L. D., Steele, V. R., Gehring, W. J., & Patrick, C. J. (2011). Externalizing psychopathology and gain–loss feedback in a simulated gambling task: Dissociable components of brain response revealed by time-frequency analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 352364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bress, J. N., Foti, D., Kotov, R., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts depression in adolescent girls. Psychophysiology, 50, 7481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bress, J. N., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Self-report and behavioral measures of reward sensitivity predict the feedback negativity. Psychophysiology. Advance online publication.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bress, J. N., Meyer, A., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Differentiating anxiety and depression in children and adolescents: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. Advance online publication.Google ScholarPubMed
Bress, J. N., Smith, E., Foti, D., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Neural response to reward and depressive symptoms in late childhood to early adolescence. Biological Psychology, 89, 156162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, J. M., Foti, D., Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Harmon-Jones, E., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Ventral striatal and medial prefrontal bold activation is correlated with reward-related electrocortical activity: A combined ERP and fMRI study. NeuroImage, 57, 16081616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiou, J., & Spreng, R. A. (1996). The reliability of difference scores: A re-examination. Journal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior, 9, 158167.Google Scholar
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Kasen, S., Velez, C. N., Hartmark, C., & Johnson, J. (1993). An epidemiological study of disorders in late childhood and adolescence: I. Age- and gender-specific prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 851867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowley, M. J., Wu, J., Hommer, R. E., South, M., Molfese, P. J., Fearon, R. M. P., et al. (2013). A developmental study of the feedback-related negativity from 10–17 years: Age and sex effects for reward versus non-reward. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38, 595612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyranowski, J. M., Frank, E., Young, E., & Shear, M. K. (2000). Adolescent onset of the gender difference in lifetime rates of major depression: A theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, J. P., & Hajcak, G. (2007). Error-related negativities elirefd by monetary loss and cues that predict loss. NeuroReport, 18, 18751878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J. R. (1994). Regression-analysis as an alternative to difference scores. Journal of Management, 20, 683689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foti, D., Carlson, J. M., Sauder, C. L., & Proudfit, G. H. (2014). Reward dysfunction in major depression: Multimodal neuroimaging evidence for refining the melancholic phenotype. NeuroImage, 101, 5058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D., & Hajcak, G. (2009). Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 81, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D., & Hajcak, G. (2010). State sadness reduces neural sensitivity to nonrewards versus rewards. NeuroReport, 21, 143147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D., Weinberg, A., Dien, J., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Event-related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: Temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 22072216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295, 22792282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, C. R., Klein, D. N., Lissek, S., Britton, J. C., Pine, D. S., & Hajcak, G. (2012). The development of fear learning and generalization in 8- to 13-year-olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 54, 675684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalographical and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, T., Dresler, T., Ehlis, A. C., Plichta, M. M., Heinzel, S., & Polak, T. (2009). Neural response to reward anticipation is modulated by gray's impulsivity. NeuroImage, 46, 11481153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammerer, D., Li, S. C., Muller, V., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Life span differences in electrophysiological correlates of monitoring gains and losses during probabilistic reinforcement learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 579592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammerer, D., Li, S. C., Volkle, M., Muller, V., & Lindenberger, U. (2013). A lifespan comparison of the reliability, test–retest stability, and signal-to-noise ratio of event-related potentials assessed during performance monitoring. Psychophysiology, 50, 111123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychology Review, 109, 679709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., & Quinn, K. (2010). Research domain criteria (rdoc): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, K., Feng, X., Hipwell, A., & Klostermann, S. (2009). Depression begets depression: Comparing the predictive utility of depression and anxiety symptoms to later depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 11671175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., Shankman, S. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (2009). Subthreshold depressive disorder in adolescents: Predictors of escalation to full-syndrome depressive disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 703710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E., & Hommer, D. (2000). FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage, 12, 2027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, M. (1992). Manual for the Children's Depression Inventory. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Kovacs, M., & MHS Staff. (2003). Children's Depression Inventory technical manual update. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Kujawa, A., Proudfit, G. H., & Klein, D. N. (2014). Neural reactivity to rewards and losses in offspring of mothers and fathers with histories of depressive and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 287297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, W. H., Wang, L. Z., Shang, H. R., Shen, Y., Li, Z., Cheung, E. F., et al. (2014). The influence of anhedonia on feedback negativity in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychologia, 53, 213220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J., Mathalon, D. H., O'Donnell, B. F., Hamalainen, M. S., Spencer, K. M., & Javitt, D. C. (2011). A roadmap for the development and validation of event-related potential biomarkers in schizophrenia research. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 2834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukie, C. N., Montazer-Hojat, S., & Holroyd, C. B. (2014). Developmental changes in the reward positivity: An electrophysiological trajectory of reward processing. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 191199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., Bress, J. N., & Proudfit, G. H. (2014). Psychometric properties of the error-related negativity in children and adolescents. Psychophysiology. Advance online publication.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., Weinberg, A., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). The development of the error-related negativity (ERN) and its relationship with anxiety: Evidence from 8- to 13 year-olds. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 152161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nestler, E. J., & Carlezon, W. A. (2006). The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 11511159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neylan, T. C., Jasiukaitis, P. A., Lenoci, M., Scott, J. C., Metzler, T. J., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Temporal instability of auditory and visual event-related potentials in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 216225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Doherty, J. P., Deichmann, R., Critchley, H. D., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron, 33, 815826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature, 442, 10421045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews, 18, 247291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, H. D., Shelton, R. C., & Duman, R. S. (2011). Functional biomarkers of depression: Diagnosis, treatment, and pathophysiology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 23752394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segalowitz, S. J., & Barnes, K. L. (1993). The reliability of ERP components in the auditory oddball paradigm. Psychophysiology, 30, 451459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L., Murphy, T. I., Homan, D., Chantziantoniou, D. K., & Khan, S. (2010). Retest reliability of medial frontal negativities during performance monitoring. Psychophysiology, 47, 260270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinha, R., Bernardy, N., & Parsons, O. A. (1992). Long-term test–retest reliability of event-related potentials in normals and alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 32, 9921003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sitarenios, G., & Kovacs, M. (1999). Use of the Children's Depression Inventory. In Maruish, M. E. (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment (2nd ed., pp. 267298). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 5559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect-theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walhovd, K. B., & Fjell, A. M. (2002). One-year test–retest reliability of auditory ERPs in young and old adults. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 46, 2940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Longer term test–retest reliability of error-related brain activity. Psychophysiology, 48, 14201425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Venables, N. C., Proudfit, G. H., & Patrick, C. J. (2014). Heritability of the neural response to emotional pictures: Evidence from ERPs in an adult twin sample. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Wierzbicki, M. (1987). A parent form of the Children's Depression Inventory: Reliability and validity in nonclinical populations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 390397.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46
Cited by

Send article to Kindle

To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

The stability of the feedback negativity and its relationship with depression during childhood and adolescence
Available formats
×

Send article to Dropbox

To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

The stability of the feedback negativity and its relationship with depression during childhood and adolescence
Available formats
×

Send article to Google Drive

To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

The stability of the feedback negativity and its relationship with depression during childhood and adolescence
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *