Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T18:38:07.685Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spontaneous Blink Rates in Children During Different Types of Eye Movements

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2014

Michael S. Salman*
Affiliation:
Children's Hospital University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada
Liumei Liu
Affiliation:
University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada
*
AE308, Children's Hospital, 820 Sherbrook Street, Winnipeg, Manitoba, R3A 1R9, Canada. Email: msalman@hsc.mb.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract:

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Introduction:

The spontaneous blink rate (SBR) is variable in humans. It increases rapidly during childhood before reaching a plateau in adulthood at 10-20 blinks/minute. Our aim was to compare the SBR during different visual tasks in children.

Methods:

Thirty-nine healthy participants (mean age(SD):13.6(3.5)years, M=21), made horizontal and vertical visually-guided saccades, tracked a horizontal and vertical target, performed horizontal and vertical active sinusoidal head rotations in light and darkness while looking straight ahead (VOR task) or fixated straight ahead and in four directions of gaze. The eyes of each participant were videotaped and reviewed twice to determine the SBR. Parametric and non-parametric tests were used to analyze the data.

Results:

The median SBR during fixation straight ahead was seven blinks/minute, which was similar to the SBR during horizontal saccades and horizontal smooth pursuit tasks. The median SBR during vertical smooth pursuit and vertical saccades were significantly lower than during fixation (p≤0.042). The median SBR during the VOR task in light and horizontal VOR in darkness were significantly higher than during fixation (p=0.019-0.024).

Conclusions:

The median SBR during visual fixation was lower than that reported previously in 5-14 years-old children during rest or 11-20 years-old subjects during quiet conversation. The median SBR was even lower during vertical smooth pursuit and saccades tasks. This may be due to differences in concentration required for visual fixation in general and vertical visual tasks more specifically. The higher SBR during the VOR may be due to drying of the eyes during head shaking.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Canadian Journal of Neurological 2013

References

1.Karson, CN.Blinking. Bull Soc Belge Ophtalmol. 1989;237:443–57.Google ScholarPubMed
2.Bacher, LF, Smotherman, WP.Spontaneous eye blinking in human infants: a review. Dev Psychobiol. 2004;44(2):95102.Google Scholar
3.Cruz, AA, Garcia, DM, Pinto, CT, Cechetti, SP.Spontaneous eyeblink activity. Ocul Surf. 2011;9(1):2941.Google Scholar
4.Bentivoglio, AR, Bressman, SB, Cassetta, E, Carretta, D, Tonali, P, Albanese, A.Analysis of blink rate patterns in normal subjects. Mov Disord. 1997;12(6):102834.Google Scholar
5.Cardona, G, García, C, Serés, C, Vilaseca, M, Gispets, J.Blink rate, blink amplitude, and tear film integrity during dynamic visual display terminal tasks. Curr Eye Res. 2011;36(3):1907.Google Scholar
6.Abdulmunem, M, Briggs, ST.Spontaneous blink rate of a normal population sample. ICLC 1999;26:2932.Google Scholar
7.Zametkin, AJ, Stevens, JR, Pittman, R.Ontogeny of spontaneous blinking and of habituation of the blink reflex. Ann Neurol. 1979;5(5):4537.Google Scholar
8.Lavezzo, MM, Schellini, SA, Padovani, CR, Hirai, FE.Eye blink in newborn and preschool-age children. Acta Ophthalmol. 2008;86(3):2758.Google Scholar
9.Salman, MS, Sharpe, JA, Eizenman, M, et al.Saccades in children. Vision Res. 2006;46(8-9):14329.Google Scholar
10.Salman, MS, Sharpe, JA, Lillakas, L, Dennis, M, Steinbach, MJ.Smooth pursuit eye movements in children. Exp Brain Res. 2006;169(1):139–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Salman, MS, Lillakas, L, Dennis, M, Steinbach, MJ, Sharpe, JA.The vestibulo-ocular reflex during active head motion in children and adolescents. Childs Nerv Syst. 2007;23:126974.Google Scholar
12.Salman, MS, Sharpe, JA, Lillakas, L, Steinbach, MJ.Square wave jerks in children and adolescents. Pediatr Neurol. 2008;38:16–9.Google Scholar
13.Bacher, LF, Allen, KJ.Sensitivity of the rate of spontaneous eye blinking to type of stimuli in young infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51(2):186–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Himebaugh, NL, Begley, CG, Bradley, A, Wilkinson, JA.Blinking and tear break-up during four visual tasks. Optom Vis Sci. 2009;86(2):e10614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Evinger, C, Manning, KA, Pellegrini, JJ, Basso, MA, Powers, AS, Sibony, PA.Not looking while leaping: the linkage of blinking and saccadic gaze shifts. Exp Brain Res. 1994;100(2):337–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Fogarty, C, Stern, JA.Eye movements and blinks: their relationship to higher cognitive processes. Int J Psychophysiol. 1989;8(1):3542.Google Scholar
17.Fukuda, K, Stern, JA, Brown, TB, Russo, MB.Cognition, blinks, eye-movements, and pupillary movements during performance of a running memory task. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76(7Suppl):C7585.Google ScholarPubMed
18.Black, RA, Halmagyi, GM, Curthoys, IS, Thurtell, MJ, Brizuela, AE.Unilateral vestibular deafferentation produces no long-term effects on human active eye-head coordination. Exp Brain Res. 1998;122(3):3626.Google Scholar