Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T17:32:00.266Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Before Virtue: Biology, Brain, Behavior, and the “Moral Sense”

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 January 2015

Eugene Sadler-Smith*
Affiliation:
University of Surrey
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract:

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Biological, brain, and behavioral sciences offer strong and growing support for the virtue ethics account of moral judgment and ethical behavior in business organizations. The acquisition of moral agency in business involves the recognition, refinement, and habituation through the processes of reflexion and reflection of a moral sense encapsulated in innate modules for compassion, hierarchy, reciprocity, purity, and affiliation adaptive for communal life both in ancestral and modern environments. The genetic and neural bases of morality exist independently of institutional frameworks and social structures. The latter not only shape moral behaviors within circumscribed limits, they also imply a plurality and compartmentalization of roles which may enable or impede the habituation of virtue. Becoming a virtuous agent entails the practical refinement of predispositions in situ as a member of a community of practitioners rather than entailing a normative ethical educational project seeking an intellectual resolution of abstract moral questions.

Type
Special Issue: Reviving Traditions: Virtue and the Common Good in Business and Management
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Business Ethics 2012

References

REFERENCES

Aristotle., 1953/2004. The Nicomachean ethics. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Arnhart, L. 1998. Darwinian natural right: The biological ethics of human nature. Biology and Philosophy, 15: 119–32.Google Scholar
Arnold, D.G., Audi, R., & Zwolinski, M. 2010. Recent work in ethical theory and its implications for business ethics. Business Ethics Quarterly, 20(4): 559–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, W., & Spicer, A. 2007. When does national identity matter? Convergence and divergence in international business ethics. Academy of Management Journal, 50(6): 1462–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. 2002. Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31(2): 101–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54: 462–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beadle, R., & Moore, G. 2006. Maclntyre on virtue and organization. Organization Studies, 27(3): 323–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beugré, C.D. 2009. Exploring the neural bases of fairness: A model of neuro-organizational justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110: 129–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, K.C. 2009. Darwinian conservatism: A disputed question. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Blasi, A. 1990. How should psychologists define morality? Or, The negative side effects of philosophy’s influence on psychology. In Wren, T. (Ed.), The moral domain: Essays on the ongoing discussion between philosophy and the social sciences: 3870. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Blum, L. 1998. Community and virtue. In Crisp, R. (Ed.), How should one live? Essays on the virtues: 231–51.Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolender, J. 2001. A two-tiered cognitive architecture model for moral reasoning. Biology and Philosophy, 16: 339–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casebeer, W.D., & Churchland, P.S. 2003. Neural mechanisms of moral cognition: A multiple aspect approach to moral judgment and decision-making. Biology and Philosophy, 18: 169–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornwell, J. 2010. Maclntyre on money. Prospect, 176 (accessed online 17 January 2011 at http://www.prospectmagazine.co.uk/2010/10/alasdair-MacIntyre-on-money).Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. 2004. Knowing thyself: The evolutionary psychology of moral reasoning and moral sentiments. In Freeman, R.E., & Werhane, P.H. (Eds.), Business, science, and ethics: 93128.Charlottesville, Va.: Philosophy Documentation Center.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.R. 1994. Descartes’ error: emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Dane, E., & Pratt, M.G. 2007. Exploring intuition and its role in Managerial decision making, Academy of Management Review, 32(1): 3354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. 1879/2004. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. 1882. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex(2nd ed.). London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. 1989. The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. 2006. The selfish gene(30th anniversary edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Schrijver, J. 2009. An evolutionary and cognitive neuroscience perspective on moral modularity. In Verplaeste, J., De Schrijver, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (Eds.),The moral brain: Essays on the evolutionary and neuroscientific aspects of morality: 255–69.New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobson, J. 2005. Monkey business: A neo-Darwinist approach to ethics codes. Financial Analysis Journal, 61(3): 5964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. 1992. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22(6): 469–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutton, J.E., Roberts, L.M., & Bednar, J. 2010. Pathways for positive identity construction at work: Four types of positive identity and the building of social resources. Academy of Management Review, 35(2): 265–93.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49: 709–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S. 2008. Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In Plessner, H., Betsch, C., and Betsch, T. (Eds.),Intuition in judgment and decision making: 2337.New York: Taylor&Francis Group, LLC.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. 2008. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59: 1–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flack, J.C. , & de Waal, F.B.M. 2004. Monkey business and business ethics: Evolutionary origins of human morality. In Freeman, R.H. & Werhane, P.H. (Eds.), Business, science, and ethics: 741.Charlottesville, Va.: Philosophy Documentation Center.Google Scholar
Flavell, J.H. 1976. Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In Resnick, L.B. (Ed.), The nature of intelligence: 231–36.Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Frederick, W.C. 1995. Values, nature and culture in the American corporation. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U., & Happe, F. 1994. Autism: Beyond “theory of mind.”. Cognition, 50: 115–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goel, V., & Dolan, R.J. 2003. Reciprocal neural response within lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold reasoning. Neurolmage, 20: 2314–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goenka, S.N. 1993. Moral conduct, concentration, and wisdom. In Bercholz, S. & Köhn, S.C. (Eds.), The Buddha and his teachings: 96121. Boston: Shambala.Google Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B.A. 2009. Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96 (5): 1029–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Rimm-Kaufman, S.E. 2008. Ideology and intuition in moral education. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2 (3): 269–86.Google Scholar
Greene, J.D. 2003. From neural “is” to moral “ought”: What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology?. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 4 (October): 847–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J.D., & Haidt, J. 2002. How (and where) does moral judgment work?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6 (12): 517–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108 (4): 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. 2004. The emotional dog gets mistaken for a possum. Review of General Psychology, 8 (4): 283–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Bjorklund, F. 2007. Social intuitionists answer six questions about morality. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.), Moral psychology, vol. 2: The cognitive science of morality: 181217. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. 2007. When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20: 98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. 2004. Intuitive ethics: how innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Dædalus (Fall): 5566.Google Scholar
Halliday, J., & Johnson, M. 2009. A MacIntyrian perspective on organizational learning. Management Learning, 41 (1): 3751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, E.M. 1996. Organizational ethics and the good life. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, E.M. 2004. De rerum natura. In Freeman, R.E. & Werhane, P.H. (Eds.), Business, science, and ethics: 201–20. Charlottesville, Va.: Philosophy Documentation Center.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. 2006. Moral minds, London: Abacus.Google Scholar
Hodgkinson, G.P., Langan-Fox, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2008. Intuition: A fundamental bridging construct in the behavioural sciences. British Journal of Psychology, 99: 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. 1902/1934/1982. The varieties of religious experience. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Klein, G.A. 2009. Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64 (6): 515–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lapsley, D.K., & Hill, P.L. 2008. On dual-processing and heuristic approaches to moral cognition. Journal of Moral Education, 37 (3): 313–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. 1991. Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, P.R. 2004. The biological bases of morality. In Freeman, R.E. & Werhane, P.H. (Eds.), Business, science, and ethics: 5979. Charlottesville, Va.: Philosophy Documentation Center.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. 2009. What zombies can't do. In Evans, J. St. B. T. & Frankish, K. (Eds.), In two minds: Dual-processes and beyond: 296315. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M.D. 2007. Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58: 259–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, M.D., Jarcho, J.M., & Satpute, A.B. 2004. Evidence-based and intuition-based self-knowledge: An fMRI study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87: 421–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacIntyre, A. 1979. Corporate modernity and moral judgement: Are they mutually exclusive?. In Goodpaster, K.E. & Sayre, K.M. (Eds.), Ethics and problems of the 21st century: 122–33 (Notre Dame, Ind.: University of Notre Dame Press).Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. 1981. After virtue: A study in moral theory. Notre Dame, Ind.: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. 1985. After virtue. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. 1999a. Dependent rational animals. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. 1999b. Social structures and their threats to moral agency, Philosophy, 74: 311–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntyre, A. 2009. God, philosophies, universities: A history of the Catholic philosophical tradition. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Maitland, I. 1997. Virtuous markets. The market as school of the virtues. Business Ethics Quarterly, 7 (1): 1731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messick, D. 2004. Human nature and business ethics. In Freeman, R.E. & Werhane, P.H. (Eds.), Business, science, and ethics: 129–33. Charlottesville, Va.: Philosophy Documentation Center.Google Scholar
Midgley, M. 2010a. The pseudo-Darwinist conspiracy. Royal Society of Arts Journal(Winter): 3639.Google Scholar
Midgley, M. 2010b. The solitary self. London: Acumen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikhail, J. 2007. Universal moral grammar: Theory, evidence and the future. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11 (4): 143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mithen, S. 1996. The prehistory of the mind: The search for the origins of art, religion, and science. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Moore, G. 2002. On the implications of the practice-institution distinction: MacIntyre and the application of modern virtue ethics to business. Business Ethics Quarterly, 12 (1): 1932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, G. 2005. Humanizing business: A modern virtue ethics approach. Business Ethics Quarterly, 15 (2): 237–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, G., & Beadle, R. 2006. In search of organizational virtue in business: Agents, goods, practices, institutions and environments. Organization Studies, 27 (3): 369–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, N. 2000. Managing the human animal. London: Texere.Google Scholar
Nielsen, R.P. 2006. Introduction to the special issue. In search of organizational virtue: Moral agency in organizations. Organization Studies, 27 (3): 317–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, P., & Petrinovich, L. 1998. A preliminary cross-cultural study of moral intuitions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19: 349–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakley, J., & Cocking, D. 2001. Virtue ethics and professional roles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. 2002. The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 2007. The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Regan, M. 2007. Moral intuitions and organizational culture. St. Louis University Law Journal (2006-2007): 941–87.Google Scholar
Reynolds, S.J. 2006. A neuro-cognitive model of the ethical decision making process: Implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91: 737–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roca, E. 2007. Introducing practical wisdom in business schools. Journal of Business Ethics, 82: 607–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooney, D., & McKenna, B. 2008. Wisdom in public administration: Looking for a sociology of wise practice. Public Administration Review (July/August): 709–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadler-Smith, E., & Shefy, E. 2004. The intuitive executive: Understanding and applying “gut feel” in decision-making. Academy of Management Executive, 18 (4): 7691.Google Scholar
Salavdor, R., & Folger, R.G. 2009. Business ethics and the brain. Business Ethics Quarterly, 19: 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, R. 2005. Introduction: Environmental virtue ethics. In Sandler, R. & Cafaro, P. (Eds.), Environmental virtue ethics: 112. Lanham, Md.: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R.M. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84: 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schön, D.A. 1983. The reflective practitioner. London: Temple Smith.Google Scholar
Scott, E.D. 2002. Organizational moral values. Business Ethics Quarterly, 12 (1): 3355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M.E.P. 2002. Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Simon, H.A. 1987. Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion. Academy of Management Executive, 1 (1): 5764.Google Scholar
Snell, R.S. 2001. Moral foundations of the learning organization. Human Relations, 54 (3): 319–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R.C. 2003. Victims of circumstances? A defense of virtue ethics in business. Business Ethics Quarterly, 13 (1): 4362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R.C. 2004a. Aristotle, ethics and business organizations. Organization Studies, 25: 1021–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R.C. 2004b. Sympathy as a “natural” sentiment. In Freeman, R.E. & Werhane, P.H. (Eds.), Business, science, and ethics: 5358. Charlottesville, Va.: Philosophy Documentation Center.Google Scholar
Sonenshein, S. 2007. The role of construction, intuition, and justification in responding to ethical issues at work: The sensemaking-intuition model. Academy of Management Review, 32 (4): 1022–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, K., & Hughes, J. 2000. Is Darwin right?. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7 (7): 6386.Google Scholar
Tappan, M.B. 2004b. The development of justice reasoning during young adulthood: A three dimensional model. In Commons, M.L., Armon, C., Kohlberg, L., Richards, F.A., Grotzer, T.A., & Sinnot, J.D. (Eds.), Adult development: Models and methods in the study of adolescent and adult thought, vol. 2: 235–47. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Trivers, R.L. 1971. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46 (1): 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Wensveen, L. 2005. Cardinal environmental virtues: A neurobiological perspective. In Sandler, R. & Cafaro, P. (Eds.), Environmental virtue ethics: 173–94.Lanham, Md.: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Verplaeste, J., Braeckman, J., & De Schrijver, J. 2009. Introduction. In Verplaeste, J., De Schrijver, J., S. Vanneste, J. & Braeckman, J. (Eds.), The moral brain: Essays on the evolutionary and neuroscientific aspects of morality: 143.New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. 1934/1962. Thought and language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wasieleski, D.M., & Hayibor, S. 2009. Evolutionary psychology and business ethics research. Business Ethics Quarterly, 19 (4): 587616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, G.R. 2006. Virtue in organizations: Moral identity as a foundation for moral agency. Organization Studies, 27 (3): 341–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenger, E. 1998. Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E.O. 1998. Consilience: The unity of knowledge. London: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Wilson, J.Q. 1993. The moral sense. New York: Free Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T.D., Lindsay, S., & Schooler, T.Y. 2000. A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107 (1): 101–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar