Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T06:49:22.766Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Demographic and psychosocial correlates of measurement error and reactivity bias in a 4-d image-based mobile food record among adults with overweight and obesity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2022

Clare Whitton
Affiliation:
Curtin School of Population Health, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia
Janelle D. Healy
Affiliation:
Curtin School of Population Health, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia
Satvinder S. Dhaliwal
Affiliation:
Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia Singapore University of Social Sciences, 463 Clementi Road, 599494, Singapore Institute for Research in Molecular Medicine (INFORMM), Universiti Sains Malaysia, 11800 Minden, Pulau Pinang, Malaysia Duke-NUS Medical School, National University of Singapore, 8 College Rd, Singapore 169857, Singapore
Charlene Shoneye
Affiliation:
Curtin School of Population Health, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia
Amelia J. Harray
Affiliation:
Curtin School of Population Health, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia Telethon Kids Institute, 15 Hospital Ave, Nedlands, WA 6009, Australia
Barbara A. Mullan
Affiliation:
Enable Institute, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia
Joanne A. McVeigh
Affiliation:
Curtin School of Allied Health, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia Movement Physiology Laboratory, University of Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa
Carol J. Boushey
Affiliation:
Epidemiology Program, University of Hawaii Cancer Center, Honolulu, HI, USA
Deborah A. Kerr*
Affiliation:
Curtin School of Population Health, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, Kent Street, GPO Box U1987, Perth 6845, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Deborah Kerr, email d.kerr@curtin.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Improving dietary reporting among people living with obesity is challenging as many factors influence reporting accuracy. Reactive Reporting may occur in response to dietary recording, but little is known about how image-based methods influence this process. Using a 4-d image-based mobile food record (mFRTM), this study aimed to identify demographic and psychosocial correlates of measurement error and reactivity bias, among adults with BMI 25–40 kg/m2. Participants (n 155, aged 18–65 years) completed psychosocial questionnaires and kept a 4-d mFRTM. Energy expenditure (EE) was estimated using ≥ 4 d of hip-worn accelerometer data, and energy intake (EI) was measured using mFRTM. EI:EE ratios were calculated, and participants in the highest tertile were considered to have Plausible Intakes. Negative changes in EI according to regression slopes indicated Reactive Reporting. Mean EI was 72 % (sd = 21) of estimated EE. Among participants with Plausible Intakes, mean EI was 96 % (sd = 13) of estimated EE. Higher BMI (OR 0·81, 95 % CI 0·72, 0·92) and greater need for social approval (OR 0·31, 95 % CI 0·10, 0·96) were associated with lower likelihood of Plausible Intakes. Estimated EI decreased by 3 % per d of recording (interquartile range − 14 %,6 %) among all participants. The EI of Reactive Reporters (n 52) decreased by 17 %/d (interquartile range − 23 %,–13 %). A history of weight loss (> 10 kg) (OR 3·4, 95 % CI 1·5, 7·8) and higher percentage of daily energy from protein (OR 1·1, 95 % CI 1·0, 1·2) were associated with greater odds of Reactive Reporting. Identification of reactivity to measurement, as well as Plausible Intakes, is recommended in community-dwelling studies to highlight and address sources of bias.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Errors in measurement and estimation of usual dietary intakes have been observed in self-reported dietary assessment methods(Reference Subar, Freedman and Tooze1Reference Burrows, Ho and Rollo3). Measurement error in dietary assessment results in inaccurate estimates of food, energy and nutrient intakes, compromising the reliability of dietary surveillance data, diet–health relationships observed in epidemiology and nutrition intervention evaluations. Despite being widely acknowledged, there are gaps in understanding the drivers of measurement error, which has prevented the development of mitigation strategies.

Measurement error, particularly misestimation of energy intake (EI), has been observed in people with obesity, more so than among people with a lower BMI(Reference Lissner, Troiano and Midthune4). In attempts to explain the relationship between BMI and misestimation of EI, many psychological and psychosocial factors have been investigated. For example, associations have been reported between misestimation of EI and high cognitive restraint(Reference Poppitt, Swann and Black5,Reference Asbeck, Mast and Bierwag6) , social desirability(Reference Taren, Tobar and Hill7), fear of negative evaluation(Reference Tooze, Subar and Thompson8), poor body image(Reference Abbot, Thomson and Ranger-Moore9), perceived stress(Reference Karelis, Lavoie and Fontaine10), and depression(Reference Kretsch, Fong and Green11). The reasons for the association between BMI and misestimation of EI remain unclear, and further studies within populations living with overweight and obesity are needed.

Most dietary interventions conducted with community populations use self-report measures such as food records, due to considerations regarding participant burden and cost. However, the recording process is known to result in reactivity bias, a change in behaviour in response to being observed(12), also referred to as the ‘observation effect’. Another component is misreporting. Identifying misreporting of EI in community-dwelling studies, without the use of controlled conditions or biomarkers, has frequently been achieved using estimates of energy expenditure (EE), such as accelerometer data or energy requirement equations based on basal metabolic rate and body mass(Reference Freedson, Melanson and Sirard13,Reference Black14) . These methods have enabled research into the determinants of misreporting, such as personal, demographic, social desirability and psychological characteristics. However, methods to identify reactivity bias in community populations have rarely been developed and undertaken, and there is no such body of research on determinants of reactivity bias.

Recently, reactivity to measurement was described as a neglected source of bias in trials, and it was recommended that risk of reactivity be identified, and followed up with quantitative investigation if necessary(Reference French, Miles and Elbourne15). However, standardised methods to identify reactivity bias in both dietary interventions and large-scale studies of diet are lacking. To date, reactivity bias has been detected and quantified in a few dietary studies, using a range of techniques. In residential feeding studies, EI was 5–6 % lower when participants were overtly observed, compared with when they were covertly observed(Reference Stubbs, O’Reilly and Whybrow16,Reference Hopkins, Michalowska and Whybrow17) , but these studies did not report whether the effect of observation changed over time. In a metabolic study among women, reported EI was 16 % lower than EE, and this was solely attributed to reactivity bias based on changes in body mass and accuracy of water intake reporting(Reference Goris and Westerterp18). This study did not report the effect of study day on misestimation of EI, so it is unclear whether the magnitude increased over time in response to recording. Findings from two US studies in community populations suggested that reactivity bias may increase in magnitude over a period of observation. Rebro et al. (1998) reported that in 4-d non-consecutive food records, US women recorded significantly fewer snacks and food items/ingredients overall on the last day of a dietary record as compared with the first day(Reference Rebro, Patterson and Kristal19). In contrast, Kirkpatrick et al. (2012) observed no reactivity bias in a 4-d food record, but slight declines in the number of food items reported over time in 7-d and 30-d food checklists. However, this study did not consider other possible sources of measurement error(Reference Kirkpatrick, Midthune and Dodd20).

In previous dietary studies investigating reactivity bias, study participants were asked to keep written food records. Digital technologies have enabled participants to capture images of their food and beverage consumption, and such methods are frequently used in dietary studies(Reference Boushey, Spoden and Zhu21). The mobile food record (mFRTM) is an image-based dietary assessment method where participants capture before and after eating images of eating occasions(Reference Boushey, Spoden and Delp22Reference Zhu, Bosch and Woo24). A community-dwelling study designed to test the accuracy of the mFRTM with EE using the doubly labelled water method, demonstrated the mFRTM was comparable to other studies with written dietary records. A unique aspect of the mFRTM method is that the portion size estimation from the images is undertaken either by automated methods or a human-trained analyst, rather than the participant as is the case in the present study(Reference Boushey, Spoden and Delp22). How this image review process may influence energy misestimation in participants living with obesity is unclear. Furthermore, it is unclear whether reactivity bias occurs with the mFRTM, to what extent, and whether it increases with the length of the recording period.

The present study of adults living with overweight and obesity aimed to: (1) identify patterns of dietary intake measurement accuracy in a 4-d image-based mFRTM, including reactivity to measurement; and (2) determine demographic and psychosocial correlates of measurement accuracy.

Methods

Participants and study design

We used baseline data from 160 participants enrolled in the ‘Tailored Diet and Activity study’ (ToDAy), a 1-year diet and physical activity randomised controlled trial in Perth, Western Australia. The protocol is described in detail elsewhere(Reference Halse, Shoneye and Pollard25). Briefly, participants were recruited via social media, letterbox drops and radio interviews. Eligible participants were aged 18–65 years, had a BMI of 25–40 kg/m2 and owned a smartphone with Internet access. Participants with serious illnesses or medical conditions or weight loss > 4 kg in the previous 2 months were ineligible. Participants using appetite suppressants, weight loss or hormone replacement medication were also ineligible. Baseline data were collected before randomisation into the intervention and active control groups. During the first study visit, anthropometric measures were collected, and participants received training on using the mFRTM application and a hip-worn accelerometer. ToDAy was registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12617000554369). All study protocols were approved by the Curtin University Human Research Ethics Committee (approval number HR61/2016).

Demographic and psychosocial measures

Prior to the first study visit, participants were invited to complete online demographic, lifestyle and psychosocial questionnaires. Data were collected on gender identity, age, highest level of educational attainment, ethnicity, socio-economic status, income and smoking status.

Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire

Assessment of eating behaviour (cognitive restraint, hunger and disinhibition) was conducted using the fifty-one-item Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire(Reference Stunkard and Messick26). Cognitive restraint refers to the conscious restriction of dietary intake, hunger refers to the desire to eat and disinhibition refers to a loss of self-control. Good reliability of Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire subscales (Cronbach’s α > 0·7) has been demonstrated in populations with obesity(Reference Bohrer, Forbush and Hunt27,Reference Küçükerdönmez, Akder and Seçkiner28) .

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales (DASS-21)

Participants also completed the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales (DASS-21)(Reference Sinclair, Siefert and Slavin-Mulford29), a twenty-one-item scale used to measure current states of depression, anxiety and stress. Each subscale has high reliability and high convergent validity with other measures of depression and anxiety(Reference Henry and Crawford30).

Weight loss history questionnaire

Participants completed an eight-item weight loss history questionnaire(Reference Myers, McVay and Champagne31) assessing the frequency, nature and short-term success of previous weight loss attempts.

Social Desirability Scale

A short thirteen-item version(Reference Reynolds32) of the Social Desirability Scale(Reference Marlow and Crowne33) was completed, to measure the need for social approval and acceptance. The shortened scale shows internal consistency (Kuder–Richardson Formula 20 coefficient = 0·76) and is highly correlated (r = 0·93, P < 0·001) with the original thirty-three-item scale(Reference Reynolds32). Higher scores indicate a tendency to provide responses in questionnaires or interviews that are socially acceptable rather than objective and that present an individual in a favourable light(Reference Hébert34).

Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale

Participants completed a Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale(Reference Leary35), a twelve-item scale assessing the level of concern a person has about others’ opinions of them. Higher scores indicate a greater level of concern about being negatively evaluated. The Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale has demonstrated a high level of internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0·90) and a test–retest reliability coefficient of 0·75 over a 4-week interval(Reference Leary35).

Dietary assessment

At the first study visit, participants received training and were instructed to record their dietary intake over four consecutive days using the mFRTM, an image-based food record application(Reference Zhu, Bosch and Woo24). Participants were asked to capture ‘before eating’ and ‘after eating’ images of all foods and beverages consumed over four consecutive days, including at least one weekend day. As in other studies(Reference Jahns, Conrad and Johnson36,Reference An37) , Friday was considered a weekend day because Friday alcohol intake resembles weekend alcohol intake(Reference Wymond, Dickinson and Riley38). Participants were instructed that images were to contain a fiducial marker (an object of known shape, size and colour)(Reference Zhu, Bosch and Woo24), to aid in portion size estimation. Approximately 1 week later, during the second study visit, participants returned and a dietitian (CS, DAK and JDH) clarified the contents of the images where they were unclear and probed for any food and beverages not captured or unclear/hidden. A trained analyst dietitian (JDH) selected matching food codes and estimated portion sizes based on the contents of the images and the review with participants. In a validation study of EI from the mFR using doubly labelled water, estimated EI was significantly correlated with total EE (r = 0·58, P < 0·0001)(Reference Boushey, Spoden and Delp22). Nutrition analysis software (FoodWorks 9, Xyris Software) which was linked to the Australian Food Composition Database, AUSNUT 2011–13, was used. Mean daily intakes of energy and nutrients were calculated.

Other measures

During the first study visit, participants were instructed to wear a triaxial accelerometer (GT3X+, Actigraph) on their right hip for seven consecutive days without removal during sleeping. The Actigraph GT3X+ is a reliable, research-grade tool for measuring physical activity in community-dwelling conditions(Reference Aadland and Ylvisåker39). The GT3X+ was programmed to record raw data at a frequency of 30 Hz. Data were later reduced to vertical axis movement counts per 60 s epoch for the current analysis. Participants who provided at least 4 d with at least 10 h of wear time per d were included in the analyses. Cut-off points were used to classify each minute of accelerometer data as sedentary (< 100 counts per minute)(Reference Matthews, Chen and Freedson40), light intensity (100–1951 counts per minute), moderate intensity (1952–5724 counts per minute) or vigorous intensity (> 5724 counts per minute), and total metabolic equivalent of task (MET) minutes were calculated using the Freedson equation(Reference Freedson, Melanson and Sirard13). One MET is equivalent to uptake of approximately 3·5 ml oxygen per kg body weight per minute, with consumption of 1 l of oxygen being equivalent to approximately 5 kcal(Reference Hills, Mokhtar and Byrne41). Thus, each participants’ mean daily MET minutes were multiplied by their body weight (kg) and oxygen uptake (3·5 ml), and divided by 200, to calculate estimated average daily EE in kilocalories. Weight and height were measured according to established protocols(Reference Stewart, Marfell-Jones and Olds42) during the second study visit.

Statistical analysis

For each participant, the ratio of estimated mean daily EI and EE was calculated. Intakes of carbohydrate, protein, total fat, saturated fat and alcohol as a percentage of daily EI were calculated by multiplying grams by 37 kJ for fats, 19 kJ for protein and carbohydrate, and 29 kJ for alcohol, then dividing by total kJ × 100. Tertiles for EI:EE ratio were calculated, and participants in the tertile with the highest EI:EE ratio were considered to have Plausible Intakes. In order to examine reactivity to recording, linear regression models of changes in EI over time were constructed for each participant, regressing EI (kJ) against recording days (1, 2, 3 and 4). Resulting unstandardised β-coefficients indicated the gradient of change in EI per unit time (day) and were used to categorise participants into tertiles. The lowest tertile which contained negative β-coefficients were considered to be Reactive Reporters.

Univariate logistic regression was conducted to assess associations of all demographic and psychosocial characteristics with the odds of having Plausible Intakes and with the odds of being a Reactive Reporter. It was calculated that with a total of 152 participants, there would be at least 80 % power (at a 5 % level of significance) of detecting a correlation of at least 0·25 between continuous variables, and a difference in proportions of at least 25 % for categorical variables. Characteristics with a P-value < 0·25 were considered for inclusion in the multivariate regression model. In the multivariate model, a backward regression procedure was used with a cut-off of 0·1 on the likelihood ratio test. This cut-off was used to prevent the loss of potentially important variables, which may have resulted if a more stringent cut-off was used. Subsequently, variables with the highest P-values in the final step of the backwards model were removed one at a time to establish the impact on other variables. Interactions between variables in the final step were considered, and interaction terms assessed where necessary. All statistical analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics 26 (IBM Corp).

Results

A total of 155 participants provided at least 3 d of dietary data and at least 4 d of valid accelerometer data. Participants were female (68 %), highly educated (60 % held a bachelor’s degree or higher) and had never smoked cigarettes (70 %) (Table 1). The mean daily EI was 7280 kJ, sd 2008 kJ (1740 kcal, sd 480 kcal) and the mean BMI was 31·2 kg/m2 (sd 4·0 kg/m2). Among participants whose food records covered both weekdays and weekend days (n 139), there was no difference between EI on weekend days (7632 kJ (equivalent to 1824 kcal)) as compared with weekdays (7452 kJ (equivalent to 1781 kcal), P = 0·5).

Table 1. Characteristics of ToDAy participants at baseline

(Numbers and percentages, n 155)

Plausible Intakes

The mean EI:EE ratio was 0·72 (sd = 0·21). Participant characteristics across tertiles of EI:EE ratio were generally similar (Table 2), apart from differences in BMI, and percentage energy from protein. In the lowest tertile (EI:EE ratio ≤ 0·64), BMI was at least 2 BMI points higher (P = 0·001) compared with the other tertiles. Percentage energy from protein was approximately 2 percentage points higher in the lowest EI:EE ratio tertile (P = 0·004) as compared with the other tertiles. The total amount of time between non-consecutive record days was slightly higher by approximately half a day in the lowest tertile of EI:EE ratio, as compared with in the other tertiles (P = 0·025).

Table 2. Demographic, lifestyle and psychosocial characteristics of ToDAy participants at baseline, by tertiles of EI:EE ratio

(Numbers and percentages, n 155)

EI, energy intake; EE, energy expenditure.

* P-values are derived from chi-squared tests for categorical variables, from ANOVA for normally distributed continuous variables and from Kruskal–Wallis tests for non-normally distributed continuous variables.

Chi-square test excluded ‘severe’ and ‘extremely severe’ categories due to low numbers.

In multivariate analyses, higher BMI (OR 0·81, 95 % CI 0·72, 0·92), greater social desirability scores (OR 0·31, 95 % CI 0·10, 0·96) and moderate v. low fear of negative evaluation by others (OR 0·17, 95 % CI 0·06, 0·54) were all associated with lower likelihood of having Plausible Intakes (EI:EE ratio tertile ≥ 0·80) (Table 3). No associations were detected between other participant characteristics and the likelihood of having Plausible Intakes.

Table 3. Associations between participant characteristics and Plausible Intakes among ToDAy participants at baseline

(Odd ratio and 95 % confidence intervals, n 155)

Ref, reference category.

* Dependent variable is being in highest tertile (≥ 0·80) of energy intake:energy expenditure ratio, v. being in tertile 1 or 2.

For the following variables P > 0·25 in univariate analyses, so they were not included in multivariable models: energy from total fat, %; energy from carbohydrates, %; energy from alcohol, %; highest level of educational attainment; total annual household income; smoking status; depression score category; disinhibition score category and previous weight loss.

First step of backwards regression model included BMI (continuous), energy from protein (continuous), energy from saturated fat (continuous), social desirability score (categorical), fear of negative evaluation score (categorical), anxiety score (categorical), stress score (categorical), restraint score (categorical) and hunger score (categorical).

§ Both multivariable models were adjusted for the covariates: age; gender; accelerometer wear time, min/d; food record duration, days; weekend days included in food record, days; total time between non-consecutive record days, days; day type of food record day 1 (weekend v. weekday); day type of final day of food record (weekend v. weekday).

|| Final step of backwards regression model included BMI (continuous), social desirability score (categorical) and fear of negative evaluation score (categorical).

'Extremely severe categories for anxiety and stress are not shown because of low numbers.

Reactive Reporting

On average, EI decreased by 3 % per d (interquartile range (IQR) − 14 %, 6 %). Participants in the lowest tertile of EI change (ranging from −3910 to −762 kJ per d) (online Supplementary Fig. 1) were classified as Reactive Reporters, with a median change in EI equivalent to a decrease of 17 % per d (IQR − 23 %, −13 %). Tertile 2 ranged from −721 to 271 kJ per d, and tertile 3 ranged from 272 to 2280 kJ per d. Characteristics of participants according to tertile of change in EI are shown in Table 4. Mean daily EI of Reactive Reporters were significantly lower (mean difference 267 kcal, P = 0·01) than intakes in the opposite tertile. A significantly larger proportion of Reactive Reporters had a history of substantial weight loss (> 10 kg), with more than 25 percentage points difference, compared with the other two tertiles (P = 0·002). There was also a significantly larger proportion of Reactive Reporters (81 %) with low v. high disinhibition scores compared with in the opposite tertile (57 %, P = 0·021).

Table 4. Demographic, lifestyle and psychosocial characteristics of ToDAy study participants at baseline, by tertiles of change in energy intake over the recording period

(Numbers and percentages, n 155)

* P-values are derived from chi-squared tests for categorical variables, from ANOVA for normally distributed continuous variables and from Kruskal–Wallis tests for non-normally distributed continuous variables.

A history of substantial weight loss (> 10 kg) (OR 3·4, 95 % CI 1·5, 7·8), mild v. no depression (OR 4·2, 95 % CI 1·5, 12·1) and moderate v. low fear of negative evaluation by others (OR 3·8, 95 % CI 1·4, 10·4) were associated with increased likelihood of being a Reactive Reporter (Table 5). Participants with a higher percentage of EI from protein were more likely to be Reactive Reporters (OR 1·1, 95 % CI 1·0, 1·2). No other associations were detected between participant characteristics and the likelihood of Reactive Reporting. The interaction between BMI and weight loss history was not associated with Reactive Reporting. Most participants with Plausible Intakes (78 %) were not classified as Reactive Reporters. More than one-quarter (26 %) of participants had Plausible Intakes with no evidence of Reactive Reporting.

Table 5. Associations between participant characteristics and Reactive Reporting among ToDAy study participants at baseline

(Odd ratio and 95 % confidence intervals, n 155)

Ref, reference category.

* Dependent variable is being in first tertile (T1) of change in energy intake over recording period, v. being in tertile 2 or 3.

For the following variables P > 0·25 in univariate analyses, so they were not included in multivariable models: energy from total fat, %; energy from carbohydrates, %; highest level of educational attainment; total annual household income; smoking status; anxiety score category and hunger score category.

First step of backwards regression model included BMI (continuous), energy from protein (continuous), energy from saturated fat (continuous), energy from alcohol (continuous), previous weight loss (categorical), social desirability score (categorical), fear of negative evaluation score (categorical), depression score (categorical), stress score (categorical), restraint score (categorical) and disinhibition score (categorical).

§ Both multivariable models were adjusted for the covariates: age; gender; accelerometer wear time, min/d; dood record duration, days; weekend days included in food record, days; total time between non-consecutive record days, days; day type of food record day 1 (weekend v. weekday); day type of final day of food record (weekend v. weekday).

|| Final step of backwards regression model included energy from protein (continuous), previous weight loss (categorical), fear of negative evaluation score (categorical) and depression score (categorical).

Extremely severe categories for stress are not shown because of low numbers.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to identify patterns of dietary intake measurement error in a community-dwelling setting and identify demographic and psychosocial factors associated with these patterns in adults living with overweight and obesity. Higher BMI and the need for social approval were inversely associated with having Plausible Intakes. We observed variation in the extent of reactivity to recording dietary intake with the mFR and found that a history of losing more than 10 kg body weight was associated with Reactive Reporting. In contrast, BMI was not associated with Reactive Reporting.

The magnitude of the dietary intake measurement error that we observed was comparable to other studies of populations with overweight and obesity. For example, in the current study, EI was 72 % of EE, while in a doubly labelled water study evaluating a technology-based dietary record in Australian women with overweight and obesity, EI was 80 % of expenditure(Reference Hutchesson, Truby and Callister43). This suggests that the use of accelerometer-derived EE was a valid method to evaluate the accuracy of EI from self-reported dietary intake. The average extent of Reactive Reporting in the current study was similar to observations of Kirkpatrick et al. (2012) using a 7-d food checklist, in which the reported frequency of consumption of total food items declined slightly over the recording period (–2 % per d)(Reference Kirkpatrick, Midthune and Dodd20). Unlike that study, Reactive Reporting in the current study was identified within a 4-d study period using the mFR and was shown to increase over the study period.

Higher BMI was associated with a lower likelihood of having Plausible Intakes, a finding observed in many previous studies(Reference Wehling and Lusher44Reference Bachman, Phelan and Wing46). Our findings demonstrate that among people with overweight and obesity, there continues to be an inverse association between BMI and the accurate estimation of dietary intakes. However, our findings also show that among populations with overweight and obesity, there are participants whose estimated dietary intakes are plausible and without reactivity bias (26 % of participants in the present study). Among populations with overweight and obesity, associations between weight status and under-estimation of EI were observed in Australian women aged 70–80 years who kept a 3-d weighed food record(Reference Meng, Kerr and Zhu47) but not among Canadian women aged 46–69 years who kept a 3-d estimated food record(Reference Karelis, Lavoie and Fontaine10). In the Canadian study, fat mass and perceived stress were associated with lower likelihood of accurate estimation of dietary intake. In a US study, women aged 22–42 years with obesity kept a 7-d estimated food record, and results indicated that depression but not BMI predicted misestimation of EI(Reference Kretsch, Fong and Green11). Reasons for these discrepancies in associations between BMI and dietary intake measurement error are unclear but suggest the presence of other underlying factors. For example, it has been suggested that the association between weight status and accurate estimation of dietary intake is underpinned by awareness of the types and amounts of foods consumed(Reference Tooze, Subar and Thompson8).

In the present study, there was no association between BMI and Reactive Reporting. Similarly, other studies have found little evidence of a role of BMI in Reactive Reporting(Reference Rebro, Patterson and Kristal19,Reference Kirkpatrick, Midthune and Dodd20) . However, previous weight loss attempts of more than 10 kg were associated with greater likelihood of Reactive Reporting, compared with having never lost 10 kg. History of weight loss attempts and frequent weight fluctuations have been associated with total error in the measurement of dietary intake(Reference Maurer, Taren and Teixeira45). To our knowledge, the current study is the first to examine and detect an association between weight loss history and reactivity bias. Dietary self-monitoring is known to bring about dietary behaviour change through raising awareness of intake and is a key behaviour change technique in weight management(Reference Burke, Wang and Sevick48,Reference Michie, Richardson and Johnston49) . In a recent systematic review of randomised controlled trials, fostering awareness and attention during eating, known as mindful eating, were associated with weight loss(Reference Fuentes Artiles, Staub and Aldakak50). Thus, it is conceivable that participants with experience of dietary self-monitoring and a history of weight loss, when asked to undertake dietary recording in the current study, reacted by changing their dietary intake.

A higher percentage of energy derived from protein was associated with higher likelihood of Reactive Reporting. It was also associated with lower likelihood of Plausible Intakes in univariate analysis but was not retained in the final multivariable model, possibly due to low power. Previous research has found that protein intake is misestimated to less extent than EI(Reference Mossavar-Rahmani, Tinker and Huang51). As such, higher contributions of protein to total EI among underreporters has been observed, as compared with among participants with plausible intakes(Reference Karelis, Lavoie and Fontaine10,Reference Price, Paul and Cole52Reference Lafay, Basdevant and Charles54) . This may be because high protein foods are often part of a main meal, and using mFR, participants have reported remembering to take images of snacks as being more difficult than meals(Reference Kerr, Dhaliwal and Pollard55). On the other hand, Reactive Reporting may be characterised by reduced overall intake of carbohydrate and fat-rich food choices. A US study found that women recorded significantly fewer snacks on the last day of a dietary record as compared with the first day(Reference Rebro, Patterson and Kristal19). Further investigation is required into the patterns of reporting of foods, meals and snacks over time, to better understand the sources of bias that exist.

Our study used an image-based mFRTM, in which foods and beverages present in each image were assessed for descriptions and portion sizes by a trained analyst. As such, participants had a reduced role in the process of dietary reporting as compared with traditional food record or 24-h recall methods in which all description and quantification of foods/beverages originate from participants. Misestimation errors may have occurred through several sources. Participants may have reacted to the reporting process by either reducing their intake or forgetting to take images of consumed items. In usability research, participants have reported remembering to take images of snacks as being more difficult than meals(Reference Kerr, Dhaliwal and Pollard55). Our finding on protein may indicate that participants continued to take images of main meals but were more likely to omit images of smaller eating occasions, and this requires further investigation. Misestimating EI may have occurred from inaccurate description or quantification of items in images. Previous research has shown foods of amorphous shape and higher density are more difficult to estimate from images(Reference Howes, Boushey and Kerr56). Future improvements in automated methods to estimate EI may improve the accuracy of image-based dietary assessment methods(Reference Fang, Shao and Kerr57).

The Social Desirability Scale assessed the tendency to provide responses in questionnaires or interviews that are socially acceptable rather than objective, and that present an individual in a favourable light(Reference Hébert34). In our study, higher social desirability scores were associated with lower likelihood of having Plausible Intakes, which aligns with numerous other studies on social desirability and reporting accuracy, across dietary assessment methodologies and across population subgroups(Reference Taren, Tobar and Hill7,Reference Tooze, Subar and Thompson8,Reference Mossavar-Rahmani, Tinker and Huang51,Reference Novotny, Rumpler and Riddick58Reference Hébert, Peterson and Hurley60) . In contrast, we found no association between social desirability scores and Reactive Reporting. This indicates that factors underlying Reactive Reporting and having Plausible Intakes may not be synonymous, and that the presence and prediction of Reactive Reporting in dietary data require separate attention in order to improve data quality and reliability.

We found that a moderate score on the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale was associated with higher odds of Reactive Reporting and lower odds of having Plausible Intakes. This finding is difficult to interpret, as we would expect to see a trend including the highest scores. Similarly, in a recent study in a group of weight-stable participants with a range of BMI, psychological factors (personality, social desirability, body image, intelligence quotient and eating behaviour) were weakly and inconsistently associated with measurement error when diet was assessed using a range of methods (weighed food record, 24-h recall, FFQ and diet history)(Reference Hopkins, Michalowska and Whybrow17). The results on the depression score in the current study in relation to Reactive Reporting may also be spurious due to low variation in depression scores and the small number of participants with higher scores. As such, we believe that the results of the current study on depression and fear of negative evaluation score do not warrant interpretation.

This study had several strengths and limitations to be considered when interpreting the findings. We considered total measurement error and reactivity bias in the same sample using the same demographic and psychosocial measurements. This allowed us to demonstrate that there are some distinct characteristics associated with reactivity bias as opposed to total measurement error. The method we used for categorising individuals based on the extent of Reactive Reporting of dietary intakes (online Supplementary Fig. 1) provides a novel contribution to the literature and aligns with recent recommendations on exploring reactions to measurement in trials(Reference French, Miles and Elbourne15). Our study sample of people with overweight and obesity was highly suitable for the assessment of dietary intake measurement error, reactivity bias and psychosocial correlates, given that previous research observed high frequency of measurement error in such populations. Nevertheless, the self-selected sample in our study may not represent the wider population. For example, a greater proportion of our participants were educated to the bachelor’s degree level or higher than in the general population (60 % v. 24 %)(61). Our study was powered to detect a difference in proportions of at least 25 % for categorical variables. As such, we may not have detected smaller effects and recommend that future studies are conducted with larger sample sizes to increase the power. Our study used baseline data from participants enrolled in a weight loss intervention, and although recent weight loss was an exclusion criteria, study enrolment alone may have induced some change in usual behaviour. For example, it is possible that some participants began restricting their EI before commencing with the mFR. However, if the extent of restriction increased in response to keeping the 4-d mFR, then this behaviour is also considered to be reactivity to measurement, the outcome measure we were attempting to capture. Furthermore, increased physical activity levels have been observed in control groups of weight loss interventions(Reference Waters, Reeves and Fjeldsoe62), possibly because study enrolment and/or study measurements provide some motivation to increase activity levels. Some evidence suggests that accelerometer wear causes an increase in activity levels(Reference Albright, Steffen and Wilkens63,Reference Motl, McAuley and Dlugonski64) , and this may have occurred in our study. This could have resulted in misclassification of some participants in the lower two tertiles of EI:EE ratio. However, studies that detected an increase in activity as a response to accelerometer wear reported that moderate to vigorous physical activity was less affected than light activity and sedentary time(Reference Ullrich, Baumann and Voigt65,Reference Baumann, Groß and Voigt66) . This suggests the impact on estimation of average daily EE in our study is likely to be minor.

In conclusion, BMI and the need for social approval were inversely associated with plausible estimates of EI using an image-based mFR. Image-based technology reduces energy misestimation, but challenges in collecting reliable self-reported dietary data remain. A change in behaviour in response to being observed (Reactive Reporting) is a distinct type of dietary intake measurement error. Variation in Reactive Reporting between individuals presents a challenge in addressing this bias at the group level. Our study adds to the literature by demonstrating a practical method, for categorising individuals based on the extent of Reactive Reporting displayed in multiple days of dietary data. This method may be applied in future large-scale dietary studies and surveys, to enable better understanding and management of measurement error.

Supplementary material

For supplementary materials referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114522001532

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all ToDAy participants for contributing their time to this study.

Funding for the ToDAy study was provided by a Healthway Health Promotion Research Grant and the East Metropolitan Health Service. The mFR app used in the ToDAy study was funded by NIH-NCI (1U01CA130784–01) and NIH-NIDDK (1R01-DK073711–01A1, 2R56DK073711–04). The sponsors had no role in the design of the study, collection, analyses, or interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript, and decision to publish the results. The mobile food record is a registered trademark.

The study was conceived by C. W., D. A. K., S. A. D., A. J. H. and C. J. B. Data collection was conducted by J. D. H., C. S. and D. A. K. Dietary analysis was conducted by J. D. H. Data analysis and manuscript drafting were conducted by C. W. The manuscript was edited by C. W., J. D. H., J. A. M., A. H., C. S., B. A. M., D. A. K., S. A. D., A. J. H. and C. J. B. All authors approved the final content of the manuscript.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

Subar, AF, Freedman, LS, Tooze, JA, et al. (2015) Addressing current criticism regarding the value of self-report dietary data. J Nutr 145, 26392645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poslusna, K, Ruprich, J, de Vries, JHM, et al. (2009) Misreporting of energy and micronutrient intake estimated by food records and 24 hour recalls, control and adjustment methods in practice. Br J Nutr 101, S73S85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burrows, TL, Ho, YY, Rollo, ME, et al. (2019) Validity of dietary assessment methods when compared to the method of doubly labeled water: a systematic review in adults. Front Endocrinol 10, 850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lissner, L, Troiano, RP, Midthune, D, et al. (2007) OPEN about obesity: recovery biomarkers, dietary reporting errors and BMI. Int J Obes 31, 956961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poppitt, SD, Swann, D, Black, AE, et al. (1998) Assessment of selective under-reporting of food intake by both obese and non-obese women in a metabolic facility. Int J Obes 22, 303311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asbeck, I, Mast, M, Bierwag, A, et al. (2002) Severe underreporting of energy intake in normal weight subjects: use of an appropriate standard and relation to restrained eating. Public Health Nutr 5, 683690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taren, D, Tobar, M, Hill, A, et al. (1999) The association of energy intake bias with psychological scores of women. Eur J Clin Nutr 53, 570578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooze, JA, Subar, AF, Thompson, FE, et al. (2004) Psychosocial predictors of energy underreporting in a large doubly labeled water study. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 795804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbot, JM, Thomson, CA, Ranger-Moore, J, et al. (2008) Psychosocial and behavioral profile and predictors of self-reported energy underreporting in obese middle-aged women. J Am Diet Assoc 108, 114119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karelis, AD, Lavoie, M-E, Fontaine, J, et al. (2010) Anthropometric, metabolic, dietary and psychosocial profiles of underreporters of energy intake: a doubly labeled water study among overweight/obese postmenopausal women—a Montreal Ottawa New Emerging Team study. Eur J Clin Nutr 64, 6874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kretsch, MJ, Fong, AK & Green, MW (1999) Behavioral and body size correlates of energy intake underreporting by obese and normal-weight women. J Am Diet Assoc 99, 300306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institutes of Health National Cancer Institute (2020) Dietary Assessment Primer, Reactivity. https://dietassessmentprimer.cancer.gov/learn/reactivity.html (accessed May 2020).Google Scholar
Freedson, PS, Melanson, E & Sirard, J (1998) Calibration of the Computer Science and Applications, Inc. accelerometer. Med Sci Sport Exerc 30, 777781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, A (2000) Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake:basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations. Int J Obes 24, 11191130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, DP, Miles, LM, Elbourne, D, et al. (2021) Reducing bias in trials due to reactions to measurement: experts produced recommendations informed by evidence. J Clin Epidemiol 139, 130139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stubbs, RJ, O’Reilly, LM, Whybrow, S, et al. (2014) Measuring the difference between actual and reported food intakes in the context of energy balance under laboratory conditions. Br J Nutr 111, 20322043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, M, Michalowska, J, Whybrow, S, et al. (2021) Identification of psychological correlates of dietary misreporting under laboratory and free-living environments. Br J Nutr 126, 264275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goris, AHC & Westerterp, KR (1999) Underreporting of habitual food intake is explained by undereating in highly motivated lean women. J Nutr 129, 878882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebro, SM, Patterson, RE, Kristal, AR, et al. (1998) The effect of keeping food records on eating patterns. J Am Diet Assoc 98, 11631165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, SI, Midthune, D, Dodd, KW, et al. (2012) Reactivity and its association with body mass index across days on food checklists. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 110118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boushey, C, Spoden, M, Zhu, FM, et al. (2017) New mobile methods for dietary assessment: review of image-assisted and image-based dietary assessment methods. Proc Nutr Soc 76, 283294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boushey, C, Spoden, M, Delp, E, et al. (2017) Reported energy intake accuracy compared to doubly labeled water and usability of the mobile food record among community dwelling adults. Nutrients 9, 312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, F, Bosch, M, Khanna, N, et al. (2015) Multiple hypotheses image segmentation and classification with application to dietary assessment. IEEE J Biomed Heal Informat 19, 377388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, F, Bosch, M, Woo, I, et al. (2010) The use of mobile devices in aiding dietary assessment and evaluation. IEEE J Sel Top Signal Process 4, 756766.Google ScholarPubMed
Halse, RE, Shoneye, CL, Pollard, CM, et al. (2019) Improving nutrition and activity behaviors using digital technology and tailored feedback: protocol for the LiveLighter Tailored Diet and Activity (ToDAy) randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res Protoc 8, e12782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stunkard, AJ & Messick, S (1985) The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 29, 7183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohrer, BK, Forbush, KT & Hunt, TK (2015) Are common measures of dietary restraint and disinhibited eating reliable and valid in obese persons? Appetite 87, 344351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Küçükerdönmez, Ö, Akder, RN, Seçkiner, S, et al. (2021) Turkish version of the ‘Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-51’ for obese individuals: a validity and reliability study. Public Health Nutr 24, 32693275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, SJ, Siefert, CJ, Slavin-Mulford, JM, et al. (2012) Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the depression, anxiety, and stress scales-21 (DASS-21) in a Nonclinical Sample of U.S. Adults. Eval. Heal. Prof 35, 259279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, JD & Crawford, JR (2005) The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol 44, 227239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, VH, McVay, MA, Champagne, CM, et al. (2012) Weight loss history as a predictor of weight loss: results from Phase I of the weight loss maintenance trial. J Behav Med 36, 574582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, WM (1982) Development of reliable and valid short forms. J Clin Psychol 38, 119125.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlow, D & Crowne, DP (1961) Social desirability and response to perceived situational demands. J Consult Psychol 25, 109115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hébert, JR (2016) Social desirability trait: biaser or driver of self-reported dietary intake? J Acad Nutr Diet 116, 18951898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leary, MR (1983) A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 9, 371375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahns, L, Conrad, Z, Johnson, LK, et al. (2017) Diet quality is lower and energy intake is higher on weekends compared with weekdays in midlife women: a 1-year cohort study. J Acad Nutr Diet 117, 10801086.e1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
An, R (2016) Weekend-weekday differences in diet among U.S. adults, 2003–2012. Ann Epidemiol 26, 5765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wymond, BS, Dickinson, KM & Riley, MD (2016) Alcoholic beverage intake throughout the week and contribution to dietary energy intake in Australian adults. Public Health Nutr 19, 25922602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aadland, E & Ylvisåker, E (2015) Reliability of the actigraph GT3X+ accelerometer in adults under free-living conditions. PLOS ONE 10, e0134606.Google ScholarPubMed
Matthews, CE, Chen, KY, Freedson, PS, et al. (2008) Amount of time spent in sedentary behaviors in the United States, 2003–2004. Am J Epidemiol 167, 875881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, AP, Mokhtar, N & Byrne, NM (2014) Assessment of physical activity and energy expenditure: an overview of objective measures. Front Nutr 1, 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, A, Marfell-Jones, M, Olds, T, et al. (2011) International Standards for Anthropometric Assessment. Adelaide: International Society for the Advancement of Kinanthropometry.Google Scholar
Hutchesson, MJ, Truby, H, Callister, R, et al. (2013) Can a web-based food record accurately assess energy intake in overweight and obese women? A pilot study. J Hum Nutr Diet 26, 140144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wehling, H & Lusher, J (2019) People with a body mass index ≥ 30 under-report their dietary intake: a systematic review. J Health Psychol 24, 20422059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurer, J, Taren, DL, Teixeira, PJ, et al. (2006) The psychosocial and behavioral characteristics related to energy misreporting. Nutr Rev 64, 5366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachman, JL, Phelan, S, Wing, RR, et al. (2011) Eating frequency is higher in weight loss maintainers and normal-weight individuals than in overweight individuals. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 17301734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meng, X, Kerr, DA, Zhu, K, et al. (2013) Under-reporting of energy intake in elderly Australian women is associated with a higher body mass index. J Nutr Health Aging 17, 112118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, LE, Wang, J & Sevick, MA (2011) Self-monitoring in weight loss: a systematic review of the literature. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 92102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michie, S, Richardson, M, Johnston, M, et al. (2013) The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann Behav Med 46, 8195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes Artiles, R, Staub, K, Aldakak, L, et al. (2019) Mindful eating and common diet programs lower body weight similarly: systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 20, 16191627.Google ScholarPubMed
Mossavar-Rahmani, Y, Tinker, LF, Huang, Y, et al. (2013) Factors relating to eating style, social desirability, body image and eating meals at home increase the precision of calibration equations correcting self-report measures of diet using recovery biomarkers: findings from the Women’s Health Initiative. Nutr J 12, 63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, GM, Paul, AA, Cole, TJ, et al. (1997) Characteristics of the low-energy reporters in a longitudinal national dietary survey. Br J Nutr 77, 833851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heitmann, BL & Lissner, L (1995) Dietary underreporting by obese individuals – is it specific or non-specific? BMJ 311, 986989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lafay, L, Basdevant, A, Charles, M-A, et al. (1997) Determinants and nature of dietary underreporting in a free-living population: the Fleurbaix Laventie Ville Santé (FLVS) study. Int J Obes 21, 567573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, D, Dhaliwal, S, Pollard, C, et al. (2017) BMI is associated with the willingness to record diet with a mobile food record among adults participating in dietary interventions. Nutrients 9, 244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, E, Boushey, C, Kerr, D, et al. (2017) Image-based dietary assessment ability of dietetics students and interns. Nutrients 9, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fang, S, Shao, Z, Kerr, DA, et al. (2019) An end-to-end image-based automatic food energy estimation technique based on learned energy distribution images: protocol and methodology. Nutrients 11, 877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novotny, JA, Rumpler, WV, Riddick, H, et al. (2003) Personality characteristics as predictors of underreporting of energy intake on 24-hour dietary recall interviews. J Am Diet Assoc 103, 11461151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebert, J, Ebbeling, CB, Matthews, CE, et al. (2002) Systematic errors in middle-aged women’s estimates of energy intake comparing three self-report measures to total energy expenditure from doubly labeled water. Ann Epidemiol 12, 577586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hébert, JR, Peterson, KE, Hurley, TG, et al. (2001) The effect of social desirability trait on self-reported dietary measures among multi-ethnic female health center employees. Ann Epidemiol 11, 417427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2017) Census of Population and Housing: Reflecting Australia – Stories from the Census, 2016. Educational Qualifications in Australia. https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup (accessed May 2021).Google Scholar
Waters, L, Reeves, M, Fjeldsoe, B, et al. (2012) Control group improvements in physical activity intervention trials and possible explanatory factors: a systematic review. J Phys Act Heal 9, 884895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albright, CL, Steffen, AD, Wilkens, LR, et al. (2014) Effectiveness of a 12-month randomized clinical trial to increase physical activity in multiethnic postpartum women: results from Hawaii’s Nā Mikimiki Project. Prev Med 69, 214223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motl, RW, McAuley, E & Dlugonski, D (2012) Reactivity in baseline accelerometer data from a physical activity behavioral intervention. Heal Psychol 31, 172175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullrich, A, Baumann, S, Voigt, L, et al. (2021) Measurement reactivity of accelerometer-based sedentary behavior and physical activity in 2 assessment periods. J Phys Act Heal 18, 185191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, S, Groß, S, Voigt, L, et al. (2018) Pitfalls in accelerometer-based measurement of physical activity: the presence of reactivity in an adult population. Scand J Med Sci Sport 28, 10561063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of ToDAy participants at baseline(Numbers and percentages, n 155)

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographic, lifestyle and psychosocial characteristics of ToDAy participants at baseline, by tertiles of EI:EE ratio(Numbers and percentages, n 155)

Figure 2

Table 3. Associations between participant characteristics and Plausible Intakes among ToDAy participants at baseline(Odd ratio and 95 % confidence intervals, n 155)

Figure 3

Table 4. Demographic, lifestyle and psychosocial characteristics of ToDAy study participants at baseline, by tertiles of change in energy intake over the recording period(Numbers and percentages, n 155)

Figure 4

Table 5. Associations between participant characteristics and Reactive Reporting among ToDAy study participants at baseline(Odd ratio and 95 % confidence intervals, n 155)

Supplementary material: File

Whitton et al. supplementary material

Whitton et al. supplementary material 1

Download Whitton et al. supplementary material(File)
File 353.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Whitton et al. supplementary material

Whitton et al. supplementary material 2

Download Whitton et al. supplementary material(File)
File 353.2 KB