Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T12:36:33.678Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bilingual interactional contexts predict executive functions in older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2022

Hwajin Yang*
Affiliation:
Singapore Management University, Singapore
Germaine Y. Q. Tng
Affiliation:
Singapore Management University, Singapore
Gilaine Rui Ng
Affiliation:
Singapore Management University, Singapore
Wee Qin Ng
Affiliation:
Singapore Management University, Singapore
*
Address for correspondence:Hwajin YangSingapore Management UniversitySchool of Social Sciences90 Stamford RoadSingapore 178903hjyang@smu.edu.sg

Abstract

Drawing on the adaptive control hypothesis, we examined whether older adults’ bilingual interactional contexts of conversational exchanges would predict important indices of executive functions (EF). We assessed participants’ engagement in each bilingual interactional context – single-language, dual-language, and dense code-switching – and their performance on a series of nonverbal EF measures. Sixty-nine healthy older adults (Mage = 70.39 years; ages 60–93) were recruited from local community centers. We found that the dense code-switching context was associated with enhanced overall EF, but not individual facets of EF (inhibitory control, shifting, and updating). These findings held true when we controlled for a host of covariates. Our findings shed light on aging bilinguals’ interactional contexts as crucial bilingual experiences that modulate overall EF. Given that bilingualism is a multidimensional construct, rather than a unidimensional variable, our study underscores the importance of more fine-grained operationalisation of bilingualism when studying its impacts on EF.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Singapore Management University, 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abutalebi, J and Green, DW (2008). Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes 23, 557582. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960801920602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, NE, Epel, ES, Castellazzo, G and Ickovics, JR (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women. Health Psychology 19, 586592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adrover-Roig, D, Sesé, A, Barceló, F and Palmer, A (2012). A latent variable approach to executive control in healthy ageing. Brain and Cognition 78, 284299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, JAE, Chung-Fat-Yim, A, Bellana, B, Luk, G and Bialystok, E (2018). Language and cognitive control networks in bilinguals and monolinguals. Neuropsychologia 117, 352363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.06.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, JAE, Hawrylewicz, K and Grundy, JG (2020). Does bilingualism protect against dementia? A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27, 952965. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01736-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ansaldo, AI, Ghazi-Saidi, L and Adrover-Roig, D (2015). Interference control in elderly bilinguals: Appearances can be misleading. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology 37, 455470. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2014.990359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antón, E, García, YF, Carreiras, M and Duñabeitiai, JA (2016). Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly?. Journal of Memory and Language 90, 147160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.04.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, AL, Navarro-Torres, CA, Dussias, PE, Bajo, MT, Guzzardo Tamargo, RE and Kroll, JF (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46, 10221047. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000770Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143, 233262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E and Craik, FIM (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19, 1923. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409358571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Freedman, M (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45, 459464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM, Klein, R and Viswanathan, M (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging 19, 290303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Luk, G (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34, 859873. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Martin, MM and Viswanathan, M (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism 9, 103119. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069050090010701CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Poarch, G, Luo, L and Craik, FIM (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging 29, 696705. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037254CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvo, N, García, AM, Manoiloff, L and Ibáñez, A (2016). Bilingualism and cognitive reserve: A critical overview and a plea for methodological innovations. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coderre, EL and van Heuven, WJB (2014). The effect of script similarity on executive control in bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 1070. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colomé, À. (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals’ speech production: Language-specific or language-independent? Journal of Memory and Language 45, 721736. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2793CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A (2019). Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences (Basel, Switzerland) 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033Google Scholar
De Bruin, A, Bak, TH and Della Sala, S (2015). Examining the effects of active versus inactive bilingualism on executive control in a carefully matched non-immigrant sample. Journal of Memory and Language 85, 1526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T. & Van Heuven, E. J. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5(3), 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dong, Y and Li, P (2015). The cognitive science of bilingualism. Language and Linguistics Compass 9, 113. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Draheim, C, Tsukahara, JS, Martin, J, Mashburn, C and Engle, RW (2019). A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/q985dGoogle Scholar
Fjell, AM, Sneve, MH, Grydeland, H, Storsve, AB and Walhovd, KB (2017). The disconnected brain and executive function decline in aging. Cerebral Cortex 27, 23032317. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw082Google ScholarPubMed
Friedman, NP and Miyake, A (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 133, 101135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, BT, Kim, C, Johnson, NF, Kryscio, RJ and Smith, CD (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 387396. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 515530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW and Wei, L (2014). A control process model of code-switching. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29, 499511. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.882515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnerud, HL, ten Braak, D, Reikerås, EKL, Donolato, E and Melby-Lervåg, M (2020). Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 146, 10591083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A and Yang, H (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150, 1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A and Yang, H (2019). Does early active bilingualism enhance inhibitory control and monitoring? A propensity-matching analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 45, 360378. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000581Google ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A and Yang, H (2020). The role of bilingual interactional contexts in predicting interindividual variability in executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 149, 609633. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofweber, J, Marinis, T and Treffers-Daller, J (2020). How different code-switching types modulate bilinguals’ executive functions: A dual control mode perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23, 909925. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houtzager, N, Lowie, W, Sprenger, S and De Bot, K (2015). A bilingual advantage in task switching? Age-related differences between German monolinguals and Dutch-Frisian bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 6979. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728915000498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, R, Martin, RC, Beier, ME, Lane, D and Hamilton, AC (2008). Executive function in older adults: a structural equation modeling approach. Neuropsychology 22, 508522. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.508Google ScholarPubMed
Incera, S and McLennan, CT (2018). Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 25, 443463. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2017.1319902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jared, D and Kroll, JF (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words?. Journal of memory and language 44, 231. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kałamała, P, Szewczyk, J, Chuderski, A, Senderecka, M and Wodniecka, Z (2020). Patterns of bilingual language use and response inhibition: A test of the adaptive control hypothesis. Cognition 204, 104373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoo, SS and Yang, H (2020). Social media use improves executive functions in middle-aged and older adults: A structural equation modeling analysis. Computers in Human Behavior 111, 106388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, NW, Fiala, L, Scott-Brown, KC and Kempe, V (2014). No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 26, 640648. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.929580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S and Phillips, NA (2012). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65, 356369. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.604788CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehtonen, M, Soveri, A, Laine, A, Järvenpää, J, De Bruin, A and Antfolk, J (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological bulletin 144, 394425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linck, JA, Hoshino, N and Kroll, JF (2008). Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. The Mental Lexicon 3, 349374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zunini, López, Morrison, RA, Kousaie, C, Taler, S, V. (2019). Task switching and bilingualism in young and older adults: A behavioral and electrophysiological investigation. Neuropsychologia 133, 107186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107186Google Scholar
Luk, G, Bialystok, E, Craik, FI and Grady, CL (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 1680816813. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macnamara, BN and Conway, ARA (2014). Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, 520525. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0524-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V and Spivey, M (2003). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 173193. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000092CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A and Friedman, NP (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21, 814. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH, Howerter, A and Wager, TD (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowszowski, L, Lampit, A, Walton, C and Naismith, S (2016). Strategy-based cognitive training for improving executive functions in older adults: A systematic review. Neuropsychology Review 26, 252270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9329-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, HS and Yang, H (2021). Coping strategies mediate the relation between executive functions and life satisfaction in middle and late adulthood: a structural equational analysis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 120. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2021.1917502Google ScholarPubMed
Oschwald, J, Schättin, A, von Bastian, CC and Souza, AS (2018). Bidialectalism and bilingualism: Exploring the role of language similarity as a link between linguistic ability and executive control. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 1997. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01997CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Darrow, J, Dalibar, C and Johnson, HA (2015). Effects of script similarity on bilingual advantages in executive control are likely to be negligible or null. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 1539. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papageorgiou, A, Bright, P, Periche Tomas, E and Filippi, R (2019). Evidence against a cognitive advantage in the older bilingual population. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72, 13541363. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818796475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A and MacWhinney, B (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 253262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990526CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramos, S, Fernández García, Y, Antón, E, Casaponsa, A and Duñabeitia, JA (2017). Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability? Journal of Neurolinguistics 43, 3948. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhodes, MG and Kelley, CM (2005). Executive processes, memory accuracy, and memory monitoring: An aging and individual difference analysis. Journal of Memory and Language 52, 578594. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.01.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvatierra, JL and Rosselli, M (2010). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism 15, 2637. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910371021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroop, JR (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulpizio, S, Del Maschio, N, Del Mauro, G and Fedeli, D and Abutalebi, J (2020). Bilingualism as a gradient measure modulates functional connectivity of language and control networks. NeuroImage 205, 116306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thierry, G and Wu, YJ (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104, 1253012535. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609927104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toh, WX, Yang, H and Hartanto, A (2019). Executive function and subjective well-being in middle and late adulthood. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 75, e69-e77. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz006Google Scholar
Tun, PA and Lachman, ME (2006). Telephone assessment of cognitive function in adulthood: The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone. Age and Ageing 35, 629632. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afl095CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verreyt, N, Woumans, E, Vandelanotte, D, Szmalec, A and Duyck, W (2016). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19, 181190. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale--Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). APA PsycTests. https://doi.org/10.1037/t15169-000Google Scholar
Xiong, J, Ye, M, Wang, L and Zheng, G (2021). Effects of physical exercise on executive function in cognitively healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Nursing Studies 114, 103810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2020.103810CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H, Hartanto, A and Yang, S (2016a). The importance of bilingual experience in assessing bilingual advantages in executive functions. Cortex 75, 237240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H, Hartanto, A and Yang, S (2016b). The complex nature of bilinguals’ language usage modulates task-switching outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology 7:560. https://doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H, Hartanto, A and Yang, S (2018). Bilingualism confers advantages in task switching: Evidence from the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21, 10911109. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891700044XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, H and Yang, S (2017). Are all interferences bad? Bilingual advantages in working memory are modulated by varying demands for controlled processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 184196. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000632CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, S, Yang, H and Hartanto, A (2019). The effects of script variation, literacy skills, and immersion experience on executive attention: A comparison of matched monoscriptal and biscriptal bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22, 142156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, CM, John, PDS, Menec, V and Tyas, SL (2014). Is bilingualism associated with a lower risk of dementia in community-living older adults? Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders 28, 326332. https://doi.org/10.1097/wad.0000000000000019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed