Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T01:00:09.466Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bilingual deaf readers’ use of semantic and syntactic cues in the processing of English relative clauses*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2016

PILAR PIÑAR*
Affiliation:
Department of World Languages and Cultures, Gallaudet University
MATTHEW T. CARLSON
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish, Italian, and Portuguese, Penn State University
JILL P. MORFORD
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of New Mexico
PAOLA E. DUSSIAS
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish, Italian, and Portuguese, Penn State University
*
Address for correspondence: Pilar Piñar, Department of World Languages and Cultures, Gallaudet University, 800 Florida Avenue NE, Washington DC, 20002pilar.pinar@gallaudet.edu

Abstract

Eye fixation measures were used to examine English relative clause processing by adult ASL–English bilingual deaf readers. Participants processed subject relative clauses faster than object relative clauses, but expected animacy cues eliminated processing difficulty in object relative clauses. This brings into question previous claims that deaf readers’ sentence processing strategies are qualitatively different from those of hearing English native speakers. Measures of English comprehension predicted reading speed, but not differences in syntactic processing. However, a trend for ASL self-ratings to predict the ability to handle syntactic complexity approached significance. Results suggest a need to explore how objective ASL proficiency measures might provide insights into deaf readers’ ability to exploit syntactic cues in English.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported in part by the National Science Foundation Science of Learning Center Program, under cooperative agreement numbers SBE-0541953 and SBE-1041725. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. We thank the NSF Science of Learning Center on Visual Language and Visual Learning at Gallaudet University for providing access to the eye-tracking equipment. Partial support was also provided by a Priority Research Grant from the Gallaudet Research Institute at Gallaudet University to Pilar Piñar, and by NSF Grant OISE-0968369 and NIH Grant 5R21HD071758 to Paola E. Dussias.

References

Allen, T. (1986). Patterns of academic achievement among hearing impaired students: 1974 and 1973. In Schildroth, A. N. and Karchmer, M. A. (Eds.), Deaf children in America (pp. 161206). San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Altmann, G., & Steedman, M. (1988). Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition, 30, 191238.Google Scholar
Anible, B., Twitchell, P., Waters, G. S., Dussias, P. E., Piñar, P., & Morford, J. P. (2015). Sensitivity to verb bias in American Sign Language–English bilinguals. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, doi:10.1093/deafed/env007. Published on-line by Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67 (1), 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalists Approaches to Grammar. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. (eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 173218). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bélanger, N. N., & Rayner, K. (2013). Frequency and predictability effects in eye fixations for skilled and less skilled readers. Visual Cognition, 21, 477497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bélanger, N. N., Slattery, T. J., Mayberry, R. I., & Rayner, K. (2012). Skilled deaf readers have an enhanced perceptual span in reading. Psychological science, 23, 816823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreault, P., & Mayberry, R. I. (2006). Grammatical processing in American Sign Language: Age of first-language acquisition effects in relation to syntactic structure. Language and cognitive processes, 21 (5), 608635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, C., & Mayberry, R. I. (2008). ASL syntactic and narrative comprehension in skilled and less skilled adults readers: Bilingual-bimodal evidence for the linguistic basis of reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 537549.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulter, L., & Goodluck, H. (2015). The processing of simple structures and temporarily ambiguous syntax by deaf readers. The Volta Review, 115, 6796.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1978). Bilingualism and the development of metalinguistic awareness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 9, 131149.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power, and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dalby, K., & Letourneau, C. (1991). Survey of communication history of deaf adults. In Biennial Meeting of the Association of Canadian Educators of the Hearing Impaired, Calgary, AB.Google Scholar
De Villiers, P., & Pomerantz, S. (1992). Hearing impaired students learning new words from written context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 409–31.Google Scholar
Dominguez, A. B., & Alegria, J. (2010). Reading mechanism in orally educated deaf adults. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 15, 136–48.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. (2003). Syntactic ambiguity resolution in L2 learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 529–57.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E., & Piñar, P. (2010). Effects of reading span and plausibility in the reanalysis of wh-gaps by Chinese-English second language speakers. Second Language Research, 26, 443472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish-English L2 speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 101–16.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Clifton, C. Jr (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of memory and language, 25, 348368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. (2002). An on-line look at sentence processing in the second language. In Heredia, R. & Altarriba, J. (eds.), Bilingual sentence processing (pp. 217236). New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C., & Pynte, J. (1997). Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 119148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallaudet Research Institute. (2005). Regional and national summary report of data from the 2003-2004 annual survey of deaf and hard of hearing children and youth. Washington, DC: GRI.Google Scholar
Garnsey, S. M., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Chapman, R. M. (1989). Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 5160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. (1987). The resolution of conflicts among competing systems: A bidirectional perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 329350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gennari, S. P., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Linking production and comprehension processes: The case of relative clauses. Cognition, 111, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 176.Google Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 14111423.Google Scholar
Gormley, K. A., & Franzen, A. M. (1978). Why can't the deaf read? Comments on asking the wrong question. American Annals of the Deaf, 123, 542–7.Google Scholar
Grodner, D., & Gibson, E. (2005). Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentential complexity. Cognitive Science, 29, 261290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodner, D., Watson, D., & Gibson, E. (2000). Locality effects on sentence processing. Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference.Google Scholar
Harrington, M. (1987). Processing transfer: Language-specific processing strategies as a source of interlanguage variation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 351377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., Knoors, H., Ormel, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2008). The relationship between the reading and signing skills of deaf children in bilingual education programs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, 518530.Google Scholar
Hoffmeister, R. J. (2000). A piece of the puzzle: ASL and reading comprehension in deaf children. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P. & Mayberry, R. I. (eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 221–59). Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
Holt, J. A. (1994). Classroom attributes and achievement test scores for deaf and hard of hearing students. American Annals of the Deaf, 139, 430–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopp, H. (2006). Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing. Second Language Research, 22, 369397.Google Scholar
Hoshino, N., Dussias, P. E., & Kroll, J. K. (2010). Processing subject-verb agreement in a second language depends on proficiency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 8798.Google Scholar
Kampfe, C. M., & Turecheck, A. G. (1987). Reading achievement of prelingually deaf students and its relationship to parental method of communication: A review of the literature. American Annals of the Deaf, 132, 1115.Google Scholar
Karchmer, M. A., & Mitchell, R. E. (2003). Demographic and achievement characteristics of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. In Marschark, M. & Spencer, P. E. (eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 2137). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, L. P. (2003). The Importance of Processing Automaticity and Temporary Storage Capacity to the Differences in Comprehension Between Skilled and Less Skilled College-Age Deaf Readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 230249.Google Scholar
Kilborn, K. (1992). On-line integration of grammatical information in a second language. Advances in psychology, 83, 337350.Google Scholar
King, J. W., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic parsing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580602.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K. J., Hernandez, A. E., & Bates, E. (1998). Bilingual performance on the Boston Naming Test: Preliminary norms in Spanish and English. Brain and Language, 65, 422440.Google Scholar
Kubuş, O., Villwock, A., Morford, J. P., & Rathmann, C. (2014). Word recognition in deaf readers: Cross-language activation of German Sign Language and German. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36 (04), 831854.Google Scholar
Kuntze, M. (2004). Literacy acquisition and deaf children: A study of the interaction between ASL and written English. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). UMI.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 3554.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 5698.Google Scholar
Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 5068.Google Scholar
Markwardt, F. C. (1989). Peabody individual achievement test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. (1989). Deaf children's reading comprehension in relation to sign language structure and input. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. (1993). First-language acquisition after childhood differs from second-language acquisition: The case of American Sign Language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1258–70.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. (2007). When timing is everything: Age of first-language acquisition effects on second-language learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 537–49.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., & Eichen, E. (1991). The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 486512.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., & Lock, E. (2003). Age constraints on first versus second language acquisition: Evidence for linguistic plasticity and epigenesist. Brain and Language, 87, 369383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381401.Google Scholar
McQuarrie, L., & Parrila, R. (2009). Phonological representations in deaf children: rethinking the ‘‘functional equivalence’’ hypothesis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14, 137–54.Google Scholar
Miller, P. F. (2000). Syntactic and semantic processing in Hebrew readers with prelingual deafness. American Annals of the Deaf, 145, 436–48.Google Scholar
Miller, P. F. (2005). Reading comprehension and its relation to the quality of functional hearing: evidence from readers with different functional hearing abilities. American Annals of the Deaf, 150, 305–22.Google Scholar
Morford, J. P., Wilkinson, E., Villwock, A., Piñar, P., & Kroll, J. F. (2011). When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition, 118, 286–92.Google Scholar
Ormel, E., Hermans, D., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Cross-language effects in visual word recognition: The case of bilingual deaf children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 288303.Google Scholar
Padden, C., & Ramsey, C. (2000). American Sign Language and reading ability in deaf children. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P. & Mayberry, R. I. (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 165–89). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Papadopoulou, D., & Clahsen, H. (2003). Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 501528.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL http://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Roland, D., Dick, F., & Elman, J. L. (2007). Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 348379.Google Scholar
Staub, A. (2010). Eye movements and processing difficulty in object relative clauses. Cognition, 116, 7186.Google Scholar
Stowe, L. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Carlson, G. N. (1991). Filling gaps on-line: Use of lexical and semantic information in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 34, 319340.Google Scholar
Strong, M., & Prinz, P. (2000). Is American Sign Language skill related to English literacy? In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P. & Mayberry, R. I. (eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 165–89). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Supalla, S. J., Wix, T. R., & McKee, C. (2001). Print as a primary source of English for deaf learners. In Nicol, J. & Langendoen, T. (eds.), One mind, two languages: Studies in bilingual language processing (pp. 177190). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Boland, J., Garnsey, S. M., & Carlson, G. N. (1989). Lexical structure in parsing long-distance dependencies. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 3750.Google Scholar
Traxler, C. B. (2000). The Stanford Achievement Test, 9th Edition: national norming and performance standards for deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 337–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traxler, M. J., Corina, D. P., Morford, J. P., Hafer, S., & Hoversten, L. J. (2014). Deaf readers’ response to syntactic complexity: Evidence from self-paced reading. Memory & Cognition, 42, 97111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traxler, M. J., Long, D. L., Johns, C. L., Tooley, K. M., Zirnstein, M., & Jonathan, E. (2012). Individual differences in eye-movements during reading: Working memory and speed-of-processing effects. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5, 116.Google Scholar
Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object-relative clauses: Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 6990.Google Scholar
Traxler, M. J., & Pickering, M. J. (1996). Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 454475.Google Scholar
Traxler, M. J., Williams, R. S., Blozis, S. A., & Morris, R. K. (2005). Working Memory, Animacy, and Verb Class in the Processing of Relative Clauses. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 204224.Google Scholar
Twitchell, P., Morford, J. P., & Hauser, P. C. (2015). Effects of SES on literacy development of deaf signing bilinguals. American Annals of the Deaf, 159, 433446.Google Scholar
Wauters, L. N., van Bon, W. H., & Tellings, A. E. (2006). Reading comprehension of Dutch deaf children. Reading and Writing, 19 (1), 4976.Google Scholar
Weckerly, J., & Kutas, M. (1999). An electrophysiological analysis of animacy effects in the processing of object-relative sentences. Psychophysiology, 36, 559570.Google Scholar
Wilbur, R. (1997). A prosodic/pragmatic explanation for word order variation in ASL with typological implications. In Lee, K., Sweetser, E. & Verspoor, M. (eds.), Lexical and syntactic constructions and the construction of meaning (pp. 89104). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Williams, J. N., Möbius, P., & Kim, C. (2001). Native and non-native processing of English wh-questions: Parsing strategies and plausibility constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 509540.Google Scholar