Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T17:26:20.767Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On the adaptive advantage of always being right (even when one is not)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 January 2010

Nathalia L. Gjersoe
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, United Kingdom. N.L.Gjersoe@bristol.ac.ukB.M.Hood@bristol.ac.ukhttp://psychology.psy.bris.ac.uk/people/nathaliagjersoe.htmlhttp://brucemhood.wordpress.com
Bruce M. Hood
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, United Kingdom. N.L.Gjersoe@bristol.ac.ukB.M.Hood@bristol.ac.ukhttp://psychology.psy.bris.ac.uk/people/nathaliagjersoe.htmlhttp://brucemhood.wordpress.com

Abstract

We propose another positive illusion – overconfidence in the generalisability of one's theory – that fits with McKay & Dennett's (M&D's) criteria for adaptive misbeliefs. This illusion is pervasive in adult reasoning but we focus on its prevalence in children's developing theories. It is a strongly held conviction arising from normal functioning of the doxastic system that confers adaptive advantage on the individual.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aronson, E. (1969) A theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 4, ed. Berkowitz, L., pp. 134. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baker, S. T., Murray, K. & Hood, B. M. (2009) Children's expectations about weight and speed of falling objects; the younger the judge the better? Poster presented at the Society for Research into Children's Development meeting, Denver, CO, April 2–4, 2009.Google Scholar
Hood, B. M. (1995) Gravity rules for 2- to 4-year olds? Cognitive Development 10:577–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986) From meta-processes to conscious access: Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition 23:95147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective in cognitive science. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. & Inhelder, B. (1974) If you want to get ahead, get a theory. Cognition 3:195212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klahr, D. & Dunbar, K. (1988) Dual search space during scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science 12:148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Massey, C. & Gelman, R. (1988) Preschoolers' ability to decide whether pictured or unfamiliar objects can move themselves. Developmental Psychology 24:307–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. L. (1981) Constraints on structure: Evidence from American Sign Language and language learning. In: Aspects of development of competence: Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, vol. 14, ed. Collins, W. A., pp. 93258. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rozinblit, L. & Keil, F. (2002) The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science 26(216):521–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar