Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T16:02:58.678Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The heterogeneity of knowledge representation and the elimination of concept

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2010

Edouard Machery
Affiliation:
Department of History and Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh PA 15260. Machery@pitt.eduwww.pitt.edu/~machery/

Abstract

In this response, I begin by defending and clarifying the notion of concept proposed in Doing without Concepts (Machery 2009) against the alternatives proposed by several commentators. I then discuss whether psychologists and philosophers who theorize about concepts are talking about distinct phenomena or about different aspects of the same phenomenon, as argued in some commentaries. Next, I criticize the idea that the cognitive-scientific findings about induction, categorization, concept combination, and so on, could be explained by positing a single kind of concept, and I insist that many categories (substances, types of events, etc.) are represented by distinct coreferential concepts that belong to very different kinds of concept. This is followed by an assessment of the hybrid theories of concepts offered by commentators, according to which categories, substances, and types of events are represented by hybrid concepts made of several parts. Finally, I defend the proposal that it may be useful to eliminate concept from the theoretical vocabulary of psychology.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, S. W. & Brooks, L. R. (1991) Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120:319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. Memory and Cognition 10:8293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1993) Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In: Theories of memory, ed. Collins, A. C., Gathercole, S. E. & Conway, M. A., pp. 29101. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Burge, T. (1979) Individualism and the mental. Midwest Studies in Philosophy 4:73121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collini, E., Wong, C. Y., Wilk, K. E., Curmi, P. M. G., Brumer, P. & Scholes, G. D. (2010) Coherently wired light-harvesting in photosynthetic marine algae at ambient temperature. Nature 463:644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danks, D. (2007) Theory unification and graphical models in human categorization. In: Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation, ed. Gopnik, A. & Schulz, L., pp. 173–89. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dove, G. (2009) Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition 110:412–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. (1982) The varieties of reference. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1990) A theory of content and other essays. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forster, M. & Sober, E. (1994) How to tell when simpler, more unified, or less ad hoc theories will provide more accurate predictions. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45:136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V. & Lakoff, G. (2005) The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology 21:455–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, J. A. (2001) The role of similarity in natural categorization. In: Similarity and categorization, ed. Hahn, U. & Ramscar, M., pp. 1328. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, C. (2010) I love Machery's book, but love concepts more. Philosophical Studies 149:411–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoenig, K., Sim, E.-J., Bochev, V., Herrnberger, B. & Kiefer, M. (2008) Conceptual flexibility in the human brain: Dynamic recruitment of semantic maps from visual, motor, and motion-related areas. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(10):1799–814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, T. W. & Gauthier, I. (2003) Auditory and action semantic features activate sensory-specific perceptual brain regions. Current Biology 13:1792–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kulatanga-Moruzi, C., Brooks, L. R. & Norman, G. R. (2001) Coordination of analytical and similarity based processing strategies and expertise in dermatological diagnosis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30:563–72.Google Scholar
Luhmann, C. C., Ahn, W. & Palmeri, T. (2006) Theory-based categorization under speeded conditions. Memory and Cognition 34:1102–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machery, E. (2005) Concepts are not a natural kind. Philosophy of Science 72:444–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. (2006b) Review of A. Zilhao, ed.: Evolution, rationality, and cognition: A cognitive science for the twenty-first century. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews. Retrieved from http://ndpr.nd.edu/review.cfm?id=6342.Google Scholar
Machery, E. (2006c) Two dogmas of neo-empiricism. Philosophy Compass 1:398412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. (2007) Concept empiricism: A methodological critique. Cognition 104:1946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machery, E. (2009) Doing without concepts. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. (forthcoming) Reply to Barbara Malt and Jesse Prinz. Mind and Language.Google Scholar
Machery, E. (2010) Reply to my critics. Philosophical Studies 149:429–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. & Seppälä, S. (forthcoming) Against hybrid theories of concepts. Anthropology & Philosophy.Google Scholar
Malt, B. C. (forthcoming) Why we should do without concepts. Mind and Language.Google Scholar
Martin, A. (2007) The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review of Psychology 58:2545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercier, H. (2010) How to cut a concept? Review of Doing without concepts by Edouard Machery. Biology and Philosophy 25:269–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, G., Eva, K., Brooks, L. & Hamstra, S. (2006) Expertise in medicine and surgery. In: The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance, ed. Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J. & Hoffman, R. R., pp. 339–53. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osherson, D. N. & Smith, E. E. (1981) On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts. Cognition 9:3558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paivio, A. (1991) Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology 45:255–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (2002) Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (forthcoming) Can concept empiricism forestall eliminativism? A commentary on Machery. Mind & Language.Google Scholar
Regehr, G., Cline, J., Norman, G. R. & Brooks, L. R. (1994) Effect of processing on diagnostic skill in dermatology. Academic Medicine I:S34S36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., Patalano, A. L. & Jonides, J. (1998) Alternative strategies of categorization. Cognition 65:167–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D. & Minda, J. P. (1998) Prototypes in the mist: The early epochs of category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24:1411–36.Google Scholar
Weisberg, J., van Turrennout, M., & Martin, A. (2007) A neural system for learning about object function. Cerebral Cortex 17:513–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, P., McKay, T., Kellas, G. & Emerson, W. A. Jr. (1985) Semantic activation of noun concepts in context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 20:804–23.Google Scholar