Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T09:35:42.830Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“The spotty cow tickled the pig with a curly tail”: How do sentence position, preferred argument structure, and referential complexity affect children's and adults’ choice of referring expression?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2011

ANNA L. THEAKSTON*
Affiliation:
University of Manchester
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Anna L. Theakston, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PL, UK. E-mail: anna.theakston@manchester.ac.uk

Abstract

In this study, 5-year-olds and adults described scenes that differed according to whether (a) the subject or object of a transitive verb represented an accessible or inaccessible referent, consistent or inconsistent with patterns of preferred argument structure, and (b) a simple noun was sufficient to uniquely identify an inaccessible referent. Results showed that although adults did not differ in their choice of referring expression based on sentence position, 5-year-olds were less likely to provide informative referring expressions for subjects than for objects when the referent was inaccessible. In addition, under complex discourse conditions, although adults used complex noun phrases to identify inaccessible referents, 5-year-olds increased their use of pronominal/null reference for both accessible and inaccessible referents, thus reducing their levels of informativeness. The data suggest that 5-year-olds are still learning to integrate their knowledge of discourse features with preferred argument structure patterns, that this is particularly difficult in complex discourse contexts, and that in these contexts children rely on well-rehearsed patterns of argument realization.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Exemplar learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. Linguistic Review, 23, 275290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child Language, 26, 339356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, S. E. M. (1997). A discourse–pragmatic explanation for the subject–object asymmetry in early null arguments: The principle of informativeness revisited. In Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Shillcock, R. (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ‘97 conference on language acquisition (pp. 1015). Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Allen, S. E. M. (2000). A discourse–pragmatic explanation for argument representation in child Inuktitut. Linguistics, 38, 483521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S. E. M. (2006). Formalism and functionalism working together? Exploring roles for complementary contributions in the domain of child null arguments. In Slabakova, R., Montrul, S., & Prévost, P. (Eds.), Inquiries in linguistic development: In honor of Lydia White (pp. 233255). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S. E. M. (2008). Interacting pragmatic influences on children's argument realization. In Bowerman, M. & Brown, P. (Eds.), Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: Implications for learnability (pp. 191210). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Allen, S. E. M., Skarabela, B., & Hughes, M. (2008). Using corpora to examine discourse effects in syntax. In Behrens, H. (Ed.), Trends in corpus research: Finding structure in data (pp. 99137). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing noun phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ariel, M. (1994). Interpreting anaphoric expressions: A cognitive versus a pragmatic approach. Journal of Linguistics, 30, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M. (2001). Accessibility theory: An overview. In Sanders, T., Schilperoord, J., & Sporen, W. (Eds.), Text representation: Linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects (pp. 2987). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J. (2008). Reference production: Production-internal and addressee-oriented processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 495527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J., & Griffin, Z. (2007). The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 521536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, J. E., Bennetto, L., & Diehl, J. J. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition, 110, 131146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, J. E., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. C. (2007). Children's use of gender and order-of mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 7–8, 140.Google Scholar
Arnold, J. E., Eisenband, J. G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. C. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of the time course for pronoun resolution from eyetracking. Cognition, 76, B13B26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnon, I. (2010). Rethinking child difficulty: The effect of NP type on children's processing of relative clauses in Hebrew. Journal of Child Language, 37, 2757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modelling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahtiyar, S., & Kuntay, A. C. (2009). Integration of communicative partner's visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language, 36, 529555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (1990). Subjectless sentences in child language. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 491504.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Warren, R. K. (1985). Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation. Cognition, 21, 4767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braine, M., & Brooks, P. (1995). Verb argument structure and the problem of avoiding an overgeneral grammar. In Tomasello, M. & Merriman, W. (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children's acquisition of verbs. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brandt, S., Kidd, E., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The discourse bases of relativization: An investigation of young German and English-speaking children's comprehension of relative clauses. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 539570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E. (1995). Centering attention in discourse. Language and Cognitive Processes, 102, 137167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27, 843873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, A. L., Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Factors affecting young children's use of pronouns as referring expressions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 13371349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chafe, W. L. (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Chan, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Children's understanding of the agent–patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German and English. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 267300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S. (1998). Surface cues and the development of new/given interpretation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 553582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Childers, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The role of pronouns in young children's acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology, 37, 730748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clancy, P. (1993). Preferred argument structure in Korean acquisition. In Clark, E. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 307314). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Clancy, P. (2003). The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In Du Bois, J., Kumpf, L., & Ashby, W. (Eds.), Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function (pp. 81108). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabrowska, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2009). Children's acquisition of questions with long distance dependencies. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 571597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). German children's comprehension of word order and case marking in causative sentences. Child Development, 79, 11521167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuBois, J. W. (1987). The discourse basis of ergativity. Language, 63, 805855.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective taking. In Beilin, H. & Pufall, P. (Eds.), Piaget's theory: Prospects and possibilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 2145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17, 99103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L. A. (1991). The metrical basis for children's subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givon, T. (1983). Topic continuity in discourse: An introduction. In Givon, T. (Ed.), Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study (pp. 142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2001). Patient arguments of causative verbs can be omitted: The role of information structure in argument distribution. Language Sciences, 34, 503524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2005). Pragmatics and argument structure. In Horn, L. & Ward, G. (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 427441). London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, P. C., Grosz, B. J., & Gilliom, L. A. (1993). Pronouns, names and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Guerriero, A. M. S., Cooper, A., Oshima-Takane, Y., & Kuriyama, Y. (2006). The development of referential choice in English and Japanese: A discourse–pragmatic perspective. Journal of Child Language, 33, 823857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gundel, J., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69, 274307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children's narratives: A comparison of English, French, German and Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 26, 419452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M., & Allen, S. (2009). Child-directed speech and the development of referential choice in child English. Paper presented at the Biennial Conference of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, CO.Google Scholar
Hurewitz, F., Brown-Schmidt, S., Thorpe, K., Gleitman, L., & Trueswell, J. (2000). One frog, two frog, red frog, blue frog: Factors affecting children's syntactic choices in production and comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 597626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyams, N., & Wexler, K. (1993). On the grammatical basis of null subjects in child language. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 421459.Google Scholar
Ibbotson, P., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). The role of pronoun frames in early comprehension of transitive constructions in English. Language Learning and Development, 7, 2439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, M., & Hickmann, M. (1992). French children's ability to introduce referents in narratives as a function of mutual knowledge. First Language, 12, 7394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E. J., Brandt, S., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Object relatives made easy: A crosslinguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children's processing of relative clauses. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 860897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E. J., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Examining the role of lexical frequency in children's acquisition and processing of sentential complements. Cognitive Development, 21, 93107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., & Kliegl, R. (1984). Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E. V. M., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2006). The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children's use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 403422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents. Child Development, 78, 17441759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narasimhan, B., Budwig, N., & Murty, L. (2005). Argument realization in Hindi child–caregiver discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 461495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orsolini, M., Rossi, F., & Pontecorvo, C. (1996). Re-introduction of referents in Italian children's narratives. Journal of Child Language, 23, 465486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., & Navarro, S. (2003). Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English: What is the role of the input? Journal of Child Language, 30, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechmann, T., & Deutsch, W. (1982). The development of verbal and nonverbal devices for reference. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 330341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prat-Sala, M., & Hahn, U. (2007). Catalan children's sensitivity to the discourse constraints imposed by different kinds of question. Language Learning, 57, 443467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radford, A. (1990). Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax: The nature of early child grammars of English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., & Noble, C. H. (in press). The role of syntactic structure in children's sentence comprehension: Evidence from the dative. Language Learning and Development.Google Scholar
Rozendaal, M., & Baker, A. (2009). The acquisition of reference: Pragmatic aspects and the influence of language input. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 18661879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salomo, D., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children's sensitivity to new and given information when answering predicate-focus questions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 101115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of young children's linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions? Developmental Science, 6, 557567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serratrice, L. (2005). The role of discourse pragmatics in the acquisition of subjects in Italian. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 437462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2008). The role of discourse and perceptual cues in the choice of referential expressions in English preschoolers, school-age children, and adults. Language Learning and Development, 4, 309332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S. (2004). Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax–pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English–Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 183205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B. (2007). Signs of early social cognition in children's syntax: The case of joint attention in argument realization in child Inuktitut. Lingua, 117, 18371857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B., & Allen, S. E. M. (2002). The role of joint attention in argument realization in child Inuktitut. In Skarabela, B., Fish, S. A., & Do, A. H.-J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 620630). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children's interpretation of pronouns. Lingua, 117, 19591987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A., & Rowland, C. (2009). The acquisition of auxiliary syntax: A longitudinal elicitation study. Part 1: Auxiliary BE. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 11491470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2005). The acquisition of auxiliary syntax: BE and HAVE. Cognitive Linguistics, 16, 247277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition, 40, 2181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warden, D. A. (1976). The influence of context on children's use of identifying expressions and references. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 101112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Young children's sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 541558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, A. M. Y., & Johnstone, J. R. (2004). The development of discourse referencing in Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 31, 633660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed