Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-lvwk9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-18T03:39:32.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The psychology of language: From data to theory. Trevor A. Harley. Taylor & Francis, 1995. Hove: Erlbaum. Pp. 496.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Carla Viana Coscarelli
Affiliation:
Federal University of Minas Gerais

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 11971211.Google Scholar
Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6, 291325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogaboam, T. W., & Perfetti, C. A. (1975). Lexical ambiguity and sentence comprehension: The common sense effect. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 265275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The use of knowledge in discourse processing: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part I. An account of the basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onifer, W., & Swinney, D. A. (1981). Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: Effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias. Memory and Cognition, 9, 225236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, K. E., & Morton, J. (1985). From orthography to phonology: An attempt to an old interpretation. In Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 335359). Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89, 6094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed developmental model of word recognition. Psychological Review, 96, 523568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed